Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 11 de 11
Filtrar
1.
Proc Biol Sci ; 290(1993): 20222189, 2023 02 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36787798

RESUMEN

Humans stand out for their capacity to flexibly cooperate, possibly because they understand their partners' role. Researchers have explored if such understanding is unique to humans by assessing whether non-human species wait to manipulate a cooperative apparatus until a delayed partner arrives. If animals do wait, then it is assumed that they recognize the need for a partner. However, success in these tasks may be the result of social facilitation, while failure may be due to poor inhibitory control. Moreover, this approach does not test if animals take their partners' actions into account. Here we trained dogs to press a button simultaneously with their human partner. Afterwards, we tested them in several conditions to disentangle which elements of their partner's behaviour they take into account. Dogs waited to press the button until the delayed partner arrived, the button was available to the partner and the partner acted (pressed the button). We found no relationship between inhibitory control and success. We conclude that dogs are not merely reacting to the presence of their human partners, but are also taking their actions into account when coordinating with them.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Cooperativa , Conducta Social , Humanos , Perros , Animales , Conducta Animal
2.
Am J Primatol ; 83(10): e23321, 2021 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34435690

RESUMEN

Unequal outcomes disrupt cooperation in some situations, but this has not been tested in the context of coordination in economic games. To explore this, we tested brown capuchins (Sapajus [Cebus] apella) on a manual version of the Stag Hunt (or Assurance) Game, in which individuals sequentially chose between two options, Stag or Hare, and were rewarded according to their choices and that of their partner. Typically, coordination on Stag results in an equal highest payout, whereas coordinating on Hare results in a guaranteed equal but lower payoff and uncoordinated play results in the lowest payoff when playing Stag. We varied this structure such that one capuchin received double the rewards for the coordinated Stag outcome; thus, it was still both animals' best option, but no longer equally rewarding. Despite the inequality, capuchins coordinated on Stag in 78% of trials, and neither payoff structure nor their partner's choice impacted their decision. Additionally, there was no relationship between self-scratching, a measure of stress in capuchins, and choices. After completing the study, we discovered our reward, cheerios, was sufficiently valuable that in another study, capuchins never refused it, so post hoc we repeated the study using a lower value reward, banana flavored pellets. Capuchins completed only 26% of the pellet trials (compared to 98% with cheerios), constraining our ability to interpret the results, but nonetheless the monkeys showed a decrease in preference for Stag, particularly when they received fewer rewards for the coordinated Stag outcome. These results reinforce capuchins' ability to find coordinated outcomes in the Stag Hunt game, but more work is needed to determine whether the monkeys did not mind the inequality or were unwilling to sacrifice a highly preferred food to rectify it. In either case, researchers should carefully consider the impact of their chosen rewards on subjects' choices.


Asunto(s)
Sapajus apella , Sapajus , Animales , Conducta Animal , Cebus , Recompensa
3.
Biomacromolecules ; 19(4): 1294-1304, 2018 04 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29537830

RESUMEN

In this work, 6-phosphogluconic trisodium salt (6-PG-Na+) is introduced as a new aqueous and nontoxic cross-linking agent to obtain ionic hydrogels. Here, it is shown the formation of hydrogels based on chitosan cross-linked with 6-PG-Na+. This formulation is obtained by ionic interaction of cationic groups of polymer with anionic groups of the cross-linker. These hydrogels are nontoxic, do not cause dermal irritation, are easy to extend, and have an adequate adhesion force to be applied as polymeric film over the skin. This formulation exhibits a first order release kinetic and can be applied as drug vehicle for topical administration or as wound dressing for wound healing. The primary goal of this communication is to report the identification and utility of 6-phosphogluconic trisodium salt (6-PG-Na+) as a nontoxic cross-linker applicable for cationic polymers.


Asunto(s)
Quitosano/química , Sistemas de Liberación de Medicamentos , Hidrogel de Polietilenoglicol-Dimetacrilato/química , Cicatrización de Heridas/efectos de los fármacos , Administración Tópica , Quitosano/administración & dosificación , Reactivos de Enlaces Cruzados/química , Gluconatos/administración & dosificación , Gluconatos/química , Humanos , Hidrogel de Polietilenoglicol-Dimetacrilato/administración & dosificación , Cinética , Polímeros/química , Sodio , Técnicas de Cierre de Heridas
4.
iScience ; 27(1): 108698, 2024 Jan 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38205239

RESUMEN

Studies on coordination often present animals with the choice of either cooperating or remaining inactive; however, in nature, animals may also choose to act alone. This can be modeled with the Assurance game, an economic game that has recently been used to explore decision-making in primates. We investigated whether dyads of pet dogs coordinate in the Assurance game. Pairs were presented with two alternatives: they could individually solve an apparatus baited with a low-value reward (Hare) or they could coordinate to solve a cooperative apparatus baited with a high-value reward for each dog (Stag). All individuals matched their partner's choices, but after controlling for side bias, only four out of eleven dyads consistently coordinated on the payoff-dominant strategy (Stag-Stag). Thus, some dogs are capable of finding coordinated outcomes, as do primates, at least when their partner's actions are visible and coordination results in the biggest payoff for both individuals.

5.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 5937, 2024 03 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38467698

RESUMEN

Human cooperation can be facilitated by the ability to create a mental representation of one's own actions, as well as the actions of a partner, known as action co-representation. Even though other species also cooperate extensively, it is still unclear whether they have similar capacities. The Joint Simon task is a two-player task developed to investigate this action co-representation. We tested brown capuchin monkeys (Sapajus [Cebus] apella), a highly cooperative species, on a computerized Joint Simon task and found that, in line with previous research, the capuchins' performance was compatible with co-representation. However, a deeper exploration of the monkeys' responses showed that they, and potentially monkeys in previous studies, did not understand the control conditions, which precludes the interpretation of the results as a social phenomenon. Indeed, further testing to investigate alternative explanations demonstrated that our results were due to low-level cues, rather than action co-representation. This suggests that the Joint Simon task, at least in its current form, cannot determine whether non-human species co-represent their partner's role in joint tasks.


Asunto(s)
Cebus , Señales (Psicología) , Animales , Cebus/fisiología
6.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 10179, 2024 05 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38702498

RESUMEN

Humans are exceptionally flexible in cooperation, partly due to our ability to recognize the roles of cooperative partners. While some non-human animals understand the need for a partner in such interactions, it is unclear whether they grasp the consequences of their partner's actions and adjust accordingly. Previous studies utilizing economic games with non-human animals yielded mixed results. We investigated dogs, known for their close cooperation with humans, in a stag hunt game. Dogs could cooperate for better rewards or defect for lower ones, while their human partners would either cooperate, never cooperate, or act randomly. We control for attraction to food, side bias, and local enhancement. Dogs were more likely to coordinate with their partners when it led to better rewards, suggesting that they understood their partner's actions. By highlighting this cognitive skill in dogs, we advance our knowledge of the intricate mechanisms driving cooperative behavior in non-human animals.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Cooperativa , Animales , Perros , Humanos , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Recompensa , Masculino , Femenino
8.
PLoS One ; 16(7): e0253277, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34260627

RESUMEN

Domestic dogs have been shown to reciprocate help received from conspecifics in food-giving tasks. However, it is not yet known whether dogs also reciprocate help received from humans. Here, we investigated whether dogs reciprocate the receipt of food from humans. In an experience phase, subjects encountered a helpful human who provided them with food by activating a food dispenser, and an unhelpful human who did not provide them with food. Subjects later had the opportunity to return food to each human type, in a test phase, via the same mechanism. In addition, a free interaction session was conducted in which the subject was free to interact with its owner and with whichever human partner it had encountered on that day. Two studies were carried out, which differed in the complexity of the experience phase and the time lag between the experience phase and test phase. Subjects did not reciprocate the receipt of food in either study. Furthermore, no difference was observed in the duration subjects spent in proximity to, or the latency to approach, the two human partners. Although our results suggest that dogs do not reciprocate help received from humans, they also suggest that the dogs did not recognize the cooperative or uncooperative act of the humans during the experience phase. It is plausible that aspects of the experimental design hindered the emergence of any potential reciprocity. However, it is also possible that dogs are simply not prosocial towards humans in food-giving contexts.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Cooperativa , Perros/psicología , Interacción Humano-Animal , Animales , Femenino , Alimentos , Conducta de Ayuda , Humanos , Masculino
9.
Eur J Pharm Biopharm ; 136: 174-183, 2019 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30654016

RESUMEN

Hydrogels are widely studied as drug delivery system. In this work we propose the employment of tetrakis(hydroxymethyl)phosphonium chloride as crosslinking agent to obtain covalent hydrogels based on chitosan. These hydrogels are obtained by Mannich reaction between the amino groups of chitosan with the hydroxymethyl groups of the crosslinker molecule. They show a pH sensitive second order swelling kinetic, have low toxicity, are biocompatible, mucoadhesive and allow a modified release of the encapsulated drug, camptothecin, for 48 h. This antitumor drug has been studied as a drug of interest to develop oral chemotherapy administration strategies. According to the obtained results, oral administration of camptothecin through hydrogels would provide low concentrations of drug at the absorption site, avoiding carrier saturation and reducing its intestinal toxicity.


Asunto(s)
Camptotecina/administración & dosificación , Quitosano/administración & dosificación , Reactivos de Enlaces Cruzados/administración & dosificación , Sistemas de Liberación de Medicamentos/métodos , Hidrogel de Polietilenoglicol-Dimetacrilato/administración & dosificación , Compuestos Organofosforados/administración & dosificación , Administración Oral , Animales , Antineoplásicos Fitogénicos/administración & dosificación , Antineoplásicos Fitogénicos/química , Antineoplásicos Fitogénicos/farmacocinética , Células CACO-2 , Camptotecina/química , Camptotecina/farmacocinética , Quitosano/química , Quitosano/farmacocinética , Reactivos de Enlaces Cruzados/química , Reactivos de Enlaces Cruzados/farmacocinética , Relación Dosis-Respuesta a Droga , Portadores de Fármacos/administración & dosificación , Portadores de Fármacos/química , Portadores de Fármacos/farmacocinética , Humanos , Hidrogel de Polietilenoglicol-Dimetacrilato/química , Hidrogel de Polietilenoglicol-Dimetacrilato/farmacocinética , Masculino , Compuestos Organofosforados/química , Compuestos Organofosforados/farmacocinética , Ratas , Ratas Wistar
10.
Pharmaceutics ; 10(4)2018 Nov 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30400294

RESUMEN

Nanohydrogels based on natural polymers, such as polysaccharides, are gaining interest as vehicles for therapeutic agents, as they can modify the pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of the carried drugs. In this work, hyaluronan-riboflavin nanohydrogels were tested in vivo in healthy rats highlighting their lack of toxicity, even at high doses, and their different biodistribution with respect to that of native hyaluronan. They were also exploited as carriers of a hydrophobic model drug, the anti-inflammatory piroxicam, that was physically embedded within the nanohydrogels by an autoclave treatment. The nanoformulation was tested by intravenous administration showing an improvement of the pharmacokinetic parameters of the molecule. The obtained results indicate that hyaluronan-based self-assembled nanohydrogels are suitable systems for low-soluble drug administration, by increasing the dose as well as the circulation time of poorly available therapeutic agents.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA