Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 14 de 14
Filtrar
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 120(16): e2217551120, 2023 04 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37036965

RESUMEN

Over the past decade, governments and organizations around the world have established behavioral insights teams advocating for randomized experiments. However, recent findings by M. N. Meyer et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 10723-10728 (2019) and P. R. Heck, C. F. Chabris, D. J. Watts, M. N. Meyer, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 117, 18948-18950 (2020) suggest that people often rate randomized experiments as less appropriate than the policies they contain even when approving the implementation of either policy untested and when none of the individual policies is clearly superior. The authors warn that this could cause policymakers to avoid running large-scale field experiments or being transparent about running them and might contribute to an adverse heterogeneity bias in terms of who is participating in experiments. In one direct and six conceptual preregistered replications (total N = 5,200) of the previously published larger-effect studies, using the same main dependent variable but with variations in scenario wordings, recruitment platforms, and countries, and the addition of further measures to assess people's views, we test the generalizability and robustness of these findings. Together, we find that the original results do not appear to generalize. That is, our triangulation reveals insufficient evidence to conclude that people exhibit a common pattern of behavior that would be consistent with relative experiment aversion, thereby supporting recent findings by R. Mislavsky, B. Dietvorst, U. Simonsohn, Mark. Sci. 39, 1092-1104 (2020). Thus, policymakers may not need to be concerned about employing evidence-based practices more so than about universally implementing policies.


Asunto(s)
Ciencias de la Conducta , Ensayos Clínicos Controlados Aleatorios como Asunto , Humanos , Políticas , Proyectos de Investigación
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 119(6)2022 02 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35105809

RESUMEN

Encouraging vaccination is a pressing policy problem. To assess whether text-based reminders can encourage pharmacy vaccination and what kinds of messages work best, we conducted a megastudy. We randomly assigned 689,693 Walmart pharmacy patients to receive one of 22 different text reminders using a variety of different behavioral science principles to nudge flu vaccination or to a business-as-usual control condition that received no messages. We found that the reminder texts that we tested increased pharmacy vaccination rates by an average of 2.0 percentage points, or 6.8%, over a 3-mo follow-up period. The most-effective messages reminded patients that a flu shot was waiting for them and delivered reminders on multiple days. The top-performing intervention included two texts delivered 3 d apart and communicated to patients that a vaccine was "waiting for you." Neither experts nor lay people anticipated that this would be the best-performing treatment, underscoring the value of simultaneously testing many different nudges in a highly powered megastudy.


Asunto(s)
Programas de Inmunización , Vacunas contra la Influenza/administración & dosificación , Farmacias , Vacunación/métodos , Anciano , COVID-19 , Femenino , Humanos , Gripe Humana/prevención & control , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Farmacias/estadística & datos numéricos , Sistemas Recordatorios , Envío de Mensajes de Texto , Vacunación/estadística & datos numéricos
4.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(13): 7103-7107, 2020 03 31.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32179683

RESUMEN

Honest reporting is essential for society to function well. However, people frequently lie when asked to provide information, such as misrepresenting their income to save money on taxes. A landmark finding published in PNAS [L. L. Shu, N. Mazar, F. Gino, D. Ariely, M. H. Bazerman, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 15197-15200 (2012)] provided evidence for a simple way of encouraging honest reporting: asking people to sign a veracity statement at the beginning instead of at the end of a self-report form. Since this finding was published, various government agencies have adopted this practice. However, in this project, we failed to replicate this result. Across five conceptual replications (n = 4,559) and one highly powered, preregistered, direct replication (n = 1,235) conducted with the authors of the original paper, we observed no effect of signing first on honest reporting. Given the policy applications of this result, it is important to update the scientific record regarding the veracity of these results.


Asunto(s)
Contratos , Decepción , Humanos
7.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 109(38): 15197-200, 2012 Sep 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22927408

RESUMEN

Many written forms required by businesses and governments rely on honest reporting. Proof of honest intent is typically provided through signature at the end of, e.g., tax returns or insurance policy forms. Still, people sometimes cheat to advance their financial self-interests-at great costs to society. We test an easy-to-implement method to discourage dishonesty: signing at the beginning rather than at the end of a self-report, thereby reversing the order of the current practice. Using laboratory and field experiments, we find that signing before-rather than after-the opportunity to cheat makes ethics salient when they are needed most and significantly reduces dishonesty.


Asunto(s)
Ética , Fraude , Adulto , Conducta , Comercio , Decepción , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Motivación , Política Pública , Autoinforme , Adulto Joven
8.
Curr Opin Psychol ; 47: 101410, 2022 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35921754

RESUMEN

What types of honesty interventions have been tested and to what extent? We conducted a systematic literature review of single-element intervention studies designed to curtail individual-level dishonesty and classified the obtained interventions in a taxonomy that encompasses three frameworks: nudging, economic, and internal-reward. We find moral reminders that we classify as educative nudges as well as external commitments (pledges, oaths, honor codes) and priming that we classify under the internal-reward framework to be the most frequently studied interventions, whereas architectural nudges (defaults, sludge) have hardly been developed. Most importantly, we identify two areas for improvement essential for our collective ability to successfully translate and scale honesty interventions: a more thorough examination of the interventions' underlying psychological processes and precise description of the experimental designs.


Asunto(s)
Decepción , Aguas del Alcantarillado , Humanos , Principios Morales , Recompensa
9.
Psychol Sci ; 22(7): 843-8, 2011 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21685379

RESUMEN

Why are there national differences in the propensity to bribe? To investigate this question, we conducted a correlational study with cross-national data and a laboratory experiment. We found a significant effect of the degree of collectivism versus individualism present in a national culture on the propensity to offer bribes to international business partners. Furthermore, the effect was mediated by individuals' sense of responsibility for their actions. Together, these results suggest that collectivism promotes bribery through lower perceived responsibility for one's actions.


Asunto(s)
Crimen/psicología , Percepción Social , Comercio/normas , Comparación Transcultural , Cultura , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Principios Morales , Identificación Social , Responsabilidad Social , Valores Sociales , Adulto Joven
10.
Psychol Sci ; 21(4): 494-8, 2010 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20424089

RESUMEN

Consumer choices reflect not only price and quality preferences but also social and moral values, as witnessed in the remarkable growth of the global market for organic and environmentally friendly products. Building on recent research on behavioral priming and moral regulation, we found that mere exposure to green products and the purchase of such products lead to markedly different behavioral consequences. In line with the halo associated with green consumerism, results showed that people act more altruistically after mere exposure to green products than after mere exposure to conventional products. However, people act less altruistically and are more likely to cheat and steal after purchasing green products than after purchasing conventional products. Together, our studies show that consumption is connected to social and ethical behaviors more broadly across domains than previously thought.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Conservación de los Recursos Naturales , Comportamiento del Consumidor , Restauración y Remediación Ambiental/ética , Obligaciones Morales , Conducta Social , Altruismo , Decepción , Femenino , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Masculino , Deseabilidad Social , Responsabilidad Social , Valores Sociales , Robo
11.
Psychiatry Res ; 135(3): 179-83, 2005 Jun 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15993948

RESUMEN

Reward dysfunction may be implicated in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). This study applied a behavioral probe, known to activate brain reward regions, to subjects with PTSD. Male heterosexual Vietnam veterans with (n = 12) or without (n = 11) current PTSD were administered two tasks: (a) key pressing to change the viewing time of average or beautiful female or male facial images, and (b) rating the attractiveness of these images. There were no significant group differences in the attractiveness ratings. However, PTSD patients expended less effort to extend the viewing time of the beautiful female faces. These findings suggest a reward deficit in PTSD.


Asunto(s)
Belleza , Cara , Recompensa , Trastornos por Estrés Postraumático/psicología , Manual Diagnóstico y Estadístico de los Trastornos Mentales , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Heterosexualidad , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Trastornos por Estrés Postraumático/diagnóstico
12.
J Clin Invest ; 125(11): 3993-6, 2015 Nov 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26524587

RESUMEN

Fraudulent business practices, such as those leading to the Enron scandal and the conviction of Bernard Madoff, evoke a strong sense of public outrage. But fraudulent or dishonest actions are not exclusive to the realm of big corporations or to evil individuals without consciences. Dishonest actions are all too prevalent in everyone's daily lives, because people are constantly encountering situations in which they can gain advantages by cutting corners. Whether it's adding a few dollars in value to the stolen items reported on an insurance claim form or dropping outlier data points from a figure to make a paper sound more interesting, dishonesty is part of the human condition. Here, we explore how people rationalize dishonesty, the implications for scientific research, and what can be done to foster a culture of research integrity.


Asunto(s)
Ética en Investigación , Mala Conducta Científica , Conflicto de Intereses , Juegos Experimentales , Humanos , Principios Morales , Motivación , Cultura Organizacional , Investigadores/psicología , Recompensa , Mala Conducta Científica/psicología
13.
Psychol Aging ; 27(4): 801-16, 2012 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23066800

RESUMEN

A prevalent stereotype is that people become less risk taking and more cautious as they get older. However, in laboratory studies, findings are mixed and often reveal no age differences. In the current series of experiments, we examined whether age differences in risk seeking are more likely to emerge when choices include a certain option (a sure gain or a sure loss). In four experiments, we found that age differences in risk preferences only emerged when participants were offered a choice between a risky and a certain gamble but not when offered two risky gambles. In particular, Experiments 1 and 2 included only gambles about potential gains. Here, compared with younger adults, older adults preferred a certain gain over a chance to win a larger gain and thus, exhibited more risk aversion in the domain of gains. But in Experiments 3 and 4, when offered the chance to take a small sure loss rather than risking a larger loss, older adults exhibited more risk seeking in the domain of losses than younger adults. Both their greater preference for sure gains and greater avoidance of sure losses suggest that older adults weigh certainty more heavily than younger adults. Experiment 4 also indicates that older adults focus more on positive emotions than younger adults do when considering their options, and that this emotional shift can at least partially account for age differences in how much people are swayed by certainty in their choices.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Toma de Decisiones , Asunción de Riesgos , Adolescente , Adulto , Afecto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Conducta de Elección , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estrés Psicológico/psicología , Incertidumbre , Adulto Joven
14.
Learn Motiv ; 39(2)2008 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24282336

RESUMEN

Gender may be involved in the motivational processing of facial beauty. This study applied a behavioral probe, known to activate brain motivational regions, to healthy heterosexual subjects. Matched samples of men and women were administered two tasks: (a) key pressing to change the viewing time of average or beautiful female or male facial images, and (b) rating the attractiveness of these images. Men expended more effort (via the key-press task) to extend the viewing time of the beautiful female faces. Women displayed similarly increased effort for beautiful male and female images, but the magnitude of this effort was substantially lower than that of men for beautiful females. Heterosexual facial attractiveness ratings were comparable in both groups. These findings demonstrate heterosexual specificity of facial motivational targets for men, but not for women. Moreover, heightened drive for the pursuit of heterosexual beauty in the face of regular valuational assessments, displayed by men, suggests a gender-specific incentive sensitization phenomenon.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA