Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 21
Filtrar
1.
Aust Vet J ; 100(1-2): 63-66, 2022 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34528244

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Brachycephalic obstructive airway syndrome (BOAS) is common in brachycephalic breeds and comprises multiple anatomic characteristics. Computed tomography (CT) is increasingly used in the preoperative assessment of dogs presenting with BOAS. We describe the case of a French Bulldog with a presumed congenital hyoid malformation and anatomic comparison of hyoid morphology between French Bulldogs and mesaticephalic dogs using CT. METHODS: Skull CT studies of French Bulldogs and mesaticephalic dogs presenting to two referral hospitals between 2013 and 2020 were collected. The degree of curvature of the basihyoid bone and the relative ventrodorsal thickness (RVD) were determined using a novel measurement technique. A circle of best fit was applied to the inside arc of the basihyoid bone, and the radius and sector angle within the basihyoid bone were recorded. The RVD was defined by the ventrodorsal thickness divided by the width in the frontal plane. RESULTS: The basihyoid bone of French Bulldogs had a greater mean ± SD RVD of 0.23 ± 0.04 compared to 0.16 ± 0.03 in mesaticephalic dogs (P < 0.01). The sector angle was also greater in French Bulldogs, measuring 144.08 ± 20.88° compared to 103.94 ± 16.49° in mesaticephalic dogs (P < 0.01). CONCLUSION: This case of hyoid malformation in a French Bulldog reports a rarely documented condition that complicated the management of BOAS. Comparative assessment of hyoid conformation revealed more acute curvature and greater RVD in French Bulldogs than in mesaticephalic dogs. This may represent an additional distorted anatomic component of brachycephalic skull morphology.


Asunto(s)
Obstrucción de las Vías Aéreas , Craneosinostosis , Enfermedades de los Perros , Hueso Hioides/anomalías , Obstrucción de las Vías Aéreas/veterinaria , Animales , Craneosinostosis/diagnóstico por imagen , Craneosinostosis/veterinaria , Enfermedades de los Perros/diagnóstico por imagen , Perros , Tomografía Computarizada por Rayos X/veterinaria
2.
Behav Brain Res ; 39(1): 1-17, 1990 Jun 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2390193

RESUMEN

We present the first evidence that a non-human species (the cat) is able to discriminate the orientation of illusory contours. Following Vogels and Orban45, we used two types of illusory contours. In one type, the illusory contour was defined by a number of contour-inducing semicircles, of which the endpoints were separated by a gap. In the other pattern, the inducing semicircles were shifted in phase along their diameter and their endpoints were aligned along the contour. Just noticeable differences in orientation were measured (at the 73.5% correct level), using a Wetherill and Levitt49 staircase procedure. Values in the order of 11 degrees were obtained when using the first type of illusory contour. Just noticeable differences with the second type were in the order of 17 degrees. Reducing the salience of the illusory contour, whether by scrambling the contour, or by decreasing the number or the contrast of inducing semicircles, systematically increased discrimination thresholds.


Asunto(s)
Gatos/psicología , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Percepción de Forma , Ilusiones , Ilusiones Ópticas , Orientación , Animales , Atención , Umbral Diferencial , Femenino , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos
3.
Behav Brain Res ; 76(1-2): 215-23, 1996 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8734055

RESUMEN

In this review we contrast passive, attribute driven processing in the visual system with an active, task-dependent view and summarize the evidence from our Positron Emission Tomography (PET) work supporting the task-dependent view. The PET studies involved comparison of regional Cerebral Blood Flow (rCBF) in closely related detection and discrimination tasks. The major finding reported is that the same retinal input or input containing only a single cue activates different extrastriate areas depending on the task.


Asunto(s)
Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Lóbulo Occipital/anatomía & histología , Lóbulo Occipital/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Animales , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Encéfalo/irrigación sanguínea , Encéfalo/fisiología , Humanos , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Lóbulo Occipital/irrigación sanguínea , Estimulación Luminosa , Primates , Factores de Tiempo , Tomografía Computarizada de Emisión , Vías Visuales/anatomía & histología , Vías Visuales/fisiología
4.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 19(5): 1014-27, 1993 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8228836

RESUMEN

The transparency phenomenon, which arises when a radial and a rotational motion pattern are spatially superimposed, suggests that these motion patterns are processed independently. Indeed, for unrestricted stimulus durations, observers could identify the rotational pattern as clockwise or counterclockwise, and the radial pattern as expansion or contraction, even under uncertainty. However, when the time available to process the compound stimulus was equal to the minimum duration required to identify each of the patterns when presented in isolation, identification was impaired. Whereas for spirallike motion patterns the radial and rotational components could be identified, radial and rotational motion patterns were not processed independently when superimposed. Although radial and rotational transformations could not be identified simultaneously, a coherent optic flow pattern could be segregated from another superimposed optic flow component given definite foreknowledge.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Enmascaramiento Perceptual , Percepción Espacial , Área de Dependencia-Independencia , Humanos , Masculino , Orientación , Psicofísica
5.
Vision Res ; 35(11): 1541-5, 1995 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7667912

RESUMEN

For a continuous flow field depicting a combined translation and expansion, there exists a natural distribution of local motion directions whose mean direction corresponds to the direction of translation. A random sampling of this distribution may introduce spatial asymmetries and thus alter the mean direction. This statistical phenomenon has a perceptual parallel: the spatial distribution of the dots becomes relevant with respect to the perceived global direction. The perceived direction of motion corresponds to the mean direction, not to the actual direction of translation.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Humanos , Psicofísica , Distribución Aleatoria , Estadística como Asunto
6.
Vision Res ; 37(5): 645-8, 1997 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9156208

RESUMEN

Human observers are very good at segmenting visual scenes consisting of multiple moving objects. Segregation of transparent motions, however, turns out to be a predominantly central vision process. In peripheral vision transparent motions are blended to form a single novel pattern whose coherent motion corresponds to the average of the separate motions, whilst sensitivity to small differences in coherent motion is maintained. These results point to distinct peripheral processing mechanisms with the advantage of being able to detect changes in motion fields quickly and accurately.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Humanos , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología
7.
Vision Res ; 28(12): 1323-35, 1988.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3256150

RESUMEN

In the present experiments three different motion discrimination tasks were studied using a random dot pattern as stimulus: velocity discrimination, direction discrimination and discrimination of opposite directions. The analysis of the motion of random dot patterns is based on motion sensitive mechanisms without the confounding interference of position sensitive mechanisms (Nakayama and Tyler, 1981). Furthermore, since isotropic random dot patterns contain no dominant orientation, a change in the direction of motion does not parallel a change in orientation. Hence the use of a random dot pattern as stimulus allows velocity and direction discrimination to be compared. Human velocity discrimination displays a U-shaped dependence on the stimulus velocity: the JNDs, expressed as Weber-fractions, are minimal for velocities ranging from 4 to 64 deg.sec-1. The Weber-fractions in velocity, determined with a staircase procedure tracking a 84% correct response level, were about 7% at the optimal speeds. The velocity discrimination curve obtained with the random dot pattern is similar to that obtained with light bars. Human direction discrimination, defined as the smallest difference in direction which can be resolved, also displays a U-shaped dependence on the stimulus velocity. Direction discrimination thresholds decrease up to a velocity of 4 deg.sec-1, they then stay at a constant level up to 128 deg.sec-1. Beyond this velocity the thresholds increase again. The mean direction discrimination threshold was 1.8 deg at optimal speeds. Discrimination of opposite directions, determined for the same conditions as those for which velocity and direction discrimination thresholds were determined, was better than the 90% response level at all speeds. However at low contrast, opposite directions are reliably discriminated only at intermediate speeds. Perceiving a coherent moving random dot pattern is supposed to be based on a cooperation between a large number of local motion detectors. In order to evaluate the importance of detector output pooling, the influence of the size of the pattern and of the presentation time on the three discrimination tasks was measured. The results indicate that the pooling requirements are task dependent. A somewhat larger pooling is required for velocity discrimination than for direction discrimination, whereas for discrimination of opposite directions only a few local motion detectors are involved.


Asunto(s)
Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Rotación , Umbral Diferencial , Humanos , Psicofísica , Factores de Tiempo
8.
Water Sci Technol ; 43(3): 339-46, 2001.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11381925

RESUMEN

BP Refinery (Bulwer Island) Ltd (BP) located on the eastern Australian coast is currently undergoing a major expansion as a part of the Queensland Clean Fuels Project. The associated wastewater treatment plant upgrade will provide a better quality of treated effluent than is currently possible with the existing infrastructure, and which will be of a sufficiently high standard to meet not only the requirements of imposed environmental legislation but also BP's environmental objectives. A number of challenges were faced when considering the upgrade, particularly; cost constraints and limited plot space, highly variable wastewater, toxicity issues, and limited hydraulic head. Sequencing Batch Reactor (SBR) Technology was chosen for the lagoon upgrade based on the following; SBR technology allowed a retro-fit of the existing earthen lagoon without the need for any additional substantial concrete structures, a dual lagoon system allowed partial treatment of wastewaters during construction, SBRs give substantial process flexibility, SBRs have the ability to easily modify process parameters without any physical modifications, and significant cost benefits. This paper presents the background to this application, an outline of laboratory studies carried out on the wastewater and details the full scale design issues and methods for providing a cost effective, efficient treatment system using the existing lagoon system.


Asunto(s)
Reactores Biológicos , Residuos Industriales , Petróleo , Eliminación de Residuos Líquidos/métodos , Reactores Biológicos/economía , Conservación de los Recursos Naturales , Costos y Análisis de Costo , Nitrógeno/aislamiento & purificación , Queensland , Eliminación de Residuos Líquidos/economía
9.
Perception ; 28(6): 703-9, 1999.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10664765

RESUMEN

To compare transparent motion and kinetic boundaries with unidirectional motion, in many studies the relative motion is generated by superimposing or adjoining unidirectional motions oriented in opposite directions. The presumption, tacitly underlying this comparison, is that the two oppositely directed velocities are independent of one another as far as their speed is concerned, i.e. the speed of the relative motion is presumed to be equivalent to the speed of the unidirectional components. Here we report that the relative motion between dots moving in opposite directions augments perceived speed. A constant-stimuli procedure was used to pair transparent-motion or kinetic-boundary displays with unidirectional motion, and human observers were asked to match the speed of the relative and unidirectional motions. The results show that transparency and kinetic boundaries increase the perceived visual speed by about 50%, compared with the speed of the individual components.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Gráficos por Computador , Humanos , Pruebas Psicológicas
10.
Percept Psychophys ; 47(5): 433-8, 1990 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2349055

RESUMEN

Theoretically, optic flow, an important source of information for the perception of locomotion and three-dimensional structure of the environment, is described in terms of divergence, curl, and shear components. We measured how the detection of the type of flow field depends on directional information. We manipulated the local directions by rotating them through an angle x relative to the original direction (i.e., the direction of motion at that locus in an unaltered flow field). The results of the first experiments showed that divergence, curl, and shear can be detected even if the directional range of the individual motion vectors is as broad as 180 degrees. Subsequent experiments revealed that the detection of the geometric components of the optic flow field is merely based on the integration of a few (10% of vectors) local directions correctly (within 10 degrees of original direction) specifying the type of flow field. Other directions are irrelevant to this process. This is actually what one would expect if the optic flow is analyzed by special purpose mechanisms that detect and process the geometric components on the basis of the integration of motion information. The results indicate that as far as they integrate motion information, detectors for divergence, curl, and shear operate in a similar manner. Implications of the results for modeling such mechanisms are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Ilusiones , Percepción de Movimiento , Ilusiones Ópticas , Orientación , Adulto , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Humanos , Psicofísica
11.
Perception ; 19(1): 21-7, 1990.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2336331

RESUMEN

Sequential presentation of a number of random-dot patterns which when super-imposed yield an expanding flow field leads to the perception of a coherent motion towards the observer. The motion vectors in this type of flow field all radiate from the origin. This percept of a global coherent expanding flow results only when the local speeds (magnitude of the local motion vectors) are zero at the centre and increase linearly towards the periphery. If all the dots radiate outwards but have the same speed, a clear percept of three-dimensional nonrigidity arises.


Asunto(s)
Aceleración , Percepción de Profundidad , Percepción de Forma , Percepción de Movimiento , Orientación , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Atención , Humanos , Psicofísica
12.
J Opt Soc Am A ; 6(2): 323-8, 1989 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2926529

RESUMEN

The aim of the present experiments was to investigate the maximum displacement over which coherent motion can be perceived (Dmax) for a stroboscopically illuminated moving random-dot pattern, both as a function of the size of the pattern and as a function of the spatial-frequency content. Perception of coherent motion was defined operationally as the ability to discriminate opposite directions. The maximum velocity (Vmax) for which the observer could discriminate the direction of motion for a given strobe rate was obtained by using a forced-choice staircase procedure. Vmax was measured as a function of the strobe rate for four different sizes of the pattern. For each pattern size Vmax was limited by the spatial asynchrony between the flashes (Dmax). For a given size of the pattern, Dmax did not depend on the temporal asynchronies tested (10-100 msec). Dmax values were increased with the size of the pattern. Bandpass rectangular filtering was used to change the spatial-frequency content of the pattern. There was a gradual increase in Dmax as the spatial-frequency content shifted to low spatial frequencies. Interestingly, the Dmax that was obtained with the unfiltered pattern was larger than that obtained with the high-pass pattern but also smaller than that obtained with the low-pass pattern. The present results are in agreement with results obtained with a two-frame display. Dmax is not a fixed value: it depends on the size of the pattern as well on the spatial-frequency content.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)


Asunto(s)
Discriminación en Psicología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Orientación , Fusión de Flicker , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
13.
Perception ; 24(7): 749-59, 1995.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8710438

RESUMEN

Naive observers of random-dot stereograms depicting complex surfaces often find that they require several tens of seconds before the impression of depth emerges. With practice, however, perception times often decrease markedly: perceptual learning occurs. Current explanations of these effects were assessed in two experiments. In the first experiment the perception times of naive observers for random-dot stereograms which depicted the same complex shape but contained different ranges of disparity were measured. In the second experiment the minimum times required by experienced observers to perceive a given complex shape in stereograms that contained different ranges of disparity were determined. Perception times for the naive observers were all very fast (<3 s) and showed no evidence of perceptual learning. There was no effect of disparity range on perception times in either experiment. It was found that very large-disparity (80 min arc) complex stereograms could be perceived quickly, even by naive observers. It is concluded that the long initial latencies previously reported are not due to surface complexity nor to the range of disparities present. Other factors. such as dot size, dot density, and the correlation of the stereo images, appear to be important determinants of efficient stereoscopic performance when viewing complex random-dot stereograms.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Profundidad , Humanos , Disparidad Visual
14.
Int J Cogn Ergon ; 3(3): 189-202, 1999.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11543418

RESUMEN

The effect of different visual depth cues presented through a head-mounted display in a dark (no pictorial cue) environment was investigated. The relative effects of binocular disparity, motion parallax, and a combination of the 2, were assessed for 3 tasks at 2 viewing distances. These tasks (which varied in the minimum amount of information they required) were a nulling task, setting a triangle to be equilateral and matching the base-to-apex magnitude of 2 triangles at different distances. Performance within the tasks varied considerably but was most accurate for the nulling task. Differences between viewing conditions may be due to a failure in the assessment of absolute viewing distance. It is argued that these results are task specific. Although there was some variation between different cue types, they appear to be largely interchangeable within the tasks. These results have implications for system designers selecting an appropriate display device for a telepresence system.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Profundidad , Percepción de Movimiento , Interfaz Usuario-Computador , Disparidad Visual , Visión Binocular , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Cibernética , Presentación de Datos , Ergonomía , Femenino , Percepción de Forma , Humanos , Masculino , Sistemas Hombre-Máquina , Psicofísica , Robótica , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
15.
J Neurophysiol ; 72(3): 1420-4, 1994 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7807222

RESUMEN

1. The regions of the human brain responsive to motion were mapped using the H2(15)O position emission tomography (PET) activation technique and compared by viewing a moving random dot pattern with a stationary dot pattern. The stimulus was optimized in dot density and 3 degrees in diameter. 2. In addition to bilateral foci at the border between Brodmann areas 19 and 37, a V1/V2 focus and a focus in the cuneus reported earlier, we observed activations in other visual areas (lower BA 19 and the parieto-occipital fissure) in the cerebellum and in two other, presumed vestibular areas, the posterior bank of lateral sulcus and at the border of BA 2/40. 3. Homologies between monkey and human cortex are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Cerebelo/fisiología , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Dominancia Cerebral/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Lóbulo Occipital/fisiología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Especificidad de la Especie , Tomografía Computarizada de Emisión , Núcleos Vestibulares/fisiología
16.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 92(4): 993-7, 1995 Feb 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7862680

RESUMEN

We have localized an area in the human brain involved in the processing of contours defined by motion differences (kinetic contours) by comparing with positron emission tomography the regional cerebral blood flow in tasks performed with kinetic and luminance-defined gratings. These tasks included passive viewing, counting the total number of grating stimuli, and counting the number of gratings of a given orientation. Comparison between the counting tasks and passive viewing with a given type of contour revealed a set of active areas that were similar for both luminance-defined and kinetic contours. Comparisons between these two types of contours revealed a single focus in the right hemisphere that did not overlap with the many regions activated by uniform motion. In particular this "kinetic focus" was clearly separated from the area previously defined as the human homologue of V5/middle temporal. Activity in this kinetic focus was stronger when orientation had to be processed than in the other two tasks. These results and control experiments with uniformly moving random dot patterns suggest the existence of an area in the human visual system that is activated much more by kinetic contours than by luminance contours or uniformly moving random dots. Up to now, such an area has not been described in the monkey visual system.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Animales , Femenino , Humanos , Cinética , Masculino , Flujo Sanguíneo Regional , Tomografía Computarizada de Emisión , Corteza Visual/irrigación sanguínea
17.
J Opt Soc Am A ; 2(11): 1836-47, 1985 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-4067694

RESUMEN

Just-noticeable differences (jnd's) in velocity were measured as a function of reference velocity for central and peripheral vision. The velocity discrimination curves plotting jnd's in velocity, expressed as Weber fractions, as a function of reference velocity were U shaped at all eccentricities. Under almost every stimulus condition the increase in jnd in velocity with increasing eccentricity was significantly larger at low reference velocities than at high reference velocities. Consequently the shift toward higher velocities with increasing eccentricity was much clearer for the lower end of the velocity-discrimination curve than for the upper end. These results are in agreement with the predictions derived from the response characteristics of velocity-tuned cells. Control experiments involving direction discrimination have shown that the impossibility of making fine velocity judgments at high speeds is due not to too weak a contrast for the stimulus motion to be visible but to a limitation in the neural apparatus analyzing velocity.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Percepción de Movimiento , Campos Visuales , Adulto , Umbral Diferencial , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Psicofísica , Retina/fisiología
18.
Exp Brain Res ; 122(1): 9-22, 1998 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9772107

RESUMEN

The regional cerebral blood-flow (rCBF) pattern of the human brain was measured using positron emission tomography (PET) while subjects viewed, detected, judged the speed of a moving random dot pattern (RDP) or compared speeds of successive RDPs. In all four conditions, retinal input was identical. Two additional conditions, continuous presentation of a moving and a stationary RDP, were included to identify human MT/V5 (hMT/V5). Both speed discrimination tasks involved the right cuneus and right lingual gyrus and to a lesser degree the left lingual gyrus and a more anterior lingual region in the right hemisphere. There was, however, little or no differential activity over hMT/V5 during either speed discrimination. Direct comparison of the two speed discrimination tasks revealed higher activity in the right middle fusiform gyrus, a result reminiscent of that obtained in earlier studies using orientation and direction as the attribute to be discriminated. These results confirm that processing in the human visual cortex is task dependent and underscore the role of the middle fusiform gyrus in temporal comparison of simple attributes.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Circulación Cerebrovascular/fisiología , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Procesos Mentales/fisiología , Movimiento (Física) , Valores de Referencia , Tomografía Computarizada de Emisión , Corteza Visual/fisiología
19.
Brain ; 119 ( Pt 4): 1263-76, 1996 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8813289

RESUMEN

This PET study concerns changes in regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) associated with orientation discrimination tasks and with simple detection of the stimulus. The difference in rCBF between discrimination and detection represents the discrimination, or 'task', component of the response. We have examined how such differences are influenced by the position of the visual stimulus and also how they change when a peripheral distractor is added to a relevant central stimulus. We first determined the regions in which the task produced the main effect regardless of stimulus position or distraction with an additional stimulus. In these selected regions, we determined the interactions between task and stimulus position and the interactions between task and stimulus addition. Five regions were more active during orientation discrimination than during stimulus detection: the inferior occipital cortex, the right putamen, the superior parietal lobule, the anterior cingulate cortex and the left lower and the right upper premotor area. Stimulus addition interacted with task only in the lower premotor area. Interactions between the task and stimulus position occurred in the occipital and parietal cortex and in the putamen. When a central stimulus was presented instead of a peripheral one the difference between orientation discrimination and stimulus detection was significantly larger in the inferior occipital lobe and in the right putamen. Conversely, the difference between orientation discrimination and stimulus detection in the superior parietal lobule was significantly larger when a peripheral stimulus was presented instead of a central one.


Asunto(s)
Velocidad del Flujo Sanguíneo/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Circulación Cerebrovascular/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Tomografía Computarizada de Emisión
20.
Rev Fr Transfus Hemobiol ; 34(3): 199-211, 1991 May.
Artículo en Francés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2064687

RESUMEN

The present study is a therapeutic trial of phase I, based on the principle of passive immunotherapy in acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS). Eighteen patients with full blown AIDS (stage IV C2 of CDC) were subdivided into two groups: nine receiving every two weeks 300 ml of plasma collected from HIV-1 seropositive symptomless (stage II or III of CDC) individuals, and nine (control group) receiving 300 ml of seronegative plasma at the same rythm and for the same period. Each patient received seven transfusions. Clinical and biological results during the transfusional and post-transfusional periods are reported.


Asunto(s)
Síndrome de Inmunodeficiencia Adquirida/terapia , Productos del Gen gag/inmunología , VIH/inmunología , Síndrome de Inmunodeficiencia Adquirida/fisiopatología , Evaluación de Medicamentos , Seropositividad para VIH , Humanos , Inmunización Pasiva , Inmunoterapia , Factores de Tiempo , Productos del Gen gag del Virus de la Inmunodeficiencia Humana
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA