Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 22
Filter
1.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 243: 104125, 2024 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38245938

ABSTRACT

To our knowledge, no study has directly examined the link between hypnotic response and the personality trait of transliminality (which is underpinned, for example, by magical ideation, mystical experience, fantasy proneness, absorption, hyperaesthesia). In order to further understand the correlates of suggestibility, the aim of the current project was to investigate whether transliminality is associated with hypnotic and imaginative suggestibility (considering: objective response, subjective response and involuntariness). Another aim was to assess the contribution of transliminality as a predictor of suggestibility when a range of previously studied personality trait measures were considered. Participants completed: the Revised Transliminality Scale, Tellegen Absorption Scale, Creative Experiences Questionnaire, and the Dissociative Experiences Scale II. To avoid context effects, where knowledge or measurement of one trait or ability might influence measurement of another, a separate standalone study was conducted where hypnotic and imaginative (without hypnosis) suggestibility screenings were carried out in-person in small groups using the modified Carleton University Responsiveness to Suggestion Scale. The merging of these two datasets enabled the analyses. Transliminality was weakly correlated with the imaginative suggestibility subjective response measure (r = 0.19). Likewise, weak correlations were found between transliminality and the hypnotic suggestibility response measures (objective, r = 0.21, subjective, r = 0.23, involuntariness, r = 0.24). The multiple regressions (forward selection) reflected the pattern of correlations, with no model for any of the variables, retaining more than a single significant predictor. In summary, this study combination, avoiding context effects, shows transliminality to be a weak predictor of response to suggestion.


Subject(s)
Hypnosis , Imagination , Humans , Suggestion , Fantasy , Personality
2.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29034102

ABSTRACT

This article summarizes key advances in hypnosis research during the past two decades, including (i) clinical research supporting the efficacy of hypnosis for managing a number of clinical symptoms and conditions, (ii) research supporting the role of various divisions in the anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortices in hypnotic responding, and (iii) an emerging finding that high hypnotic suggestibility is associated with atypical brain connectivity profiles. Key recommendations for a research agenda for the next decade include the recommendations that (i) laboratory hypnosis researchers should strongly consider how they assess hypnotic suggestibility in their studies, (ii) inclusion of study participants who score in the middle range of hypnotic suggestibility, and (iii) use of expanding research designs that more clearly delineate the roles of inductions and specific suggestions. Finally, we make two specific suggestions for helping to move the field forward including (i) the use of data sharing and (ii) redirecting resources away from contrasting state and nonstate positions toward studying (a) the efficacy of hypnotic treatments for clinical conditions influenced by central nervous system processes and (b) the neurophysiological underpinnings of hypnotic phenomena. As we learn more about the neurophysiological mechanisms underlying hypnosis and suggestion, we will strengthen our knowledge of both basic brain functions and a host of different psychological functions.

3.
Memory ; 24(4): 455-70, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25751732

ABSTRACT

In the present study we examined whether higher levels of object imagery, a stable characteristic that reflects the ability and preference in generating pictorial mental images of objects, facilitate involuntary and voluntary retrieval of autobiographical memories (ABMs). Individuals with high (High-OI) and low (Low-OI) levels of object imagery were asked to perform an involuntary and a voluntary ABM task in the laboratory. Results showed that High-OI participants generated more involuntary and voluntary ABMs than Low-OI, with faster retrieval times. High-OI also reported more detailed memories compared to Low-OI and retrieved memories as visual images. Theoretical implications of these findings for research on voluntary and involuntary ABMs are discussed.


Subject(s)
Imagination , Memory, Episodic , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adult , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
4.
Psychiatry Res ; 231(2): 151-9, 2015 Feb 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25557062

ABSTRACT

This study explores whether self-reported depth of hypnosis and hypnotic suggestibility are associated with individual differences in neuroanatomy and/or levels of functional connectivity. Twenty-nine people varying in suggestibility were recruited and underwent structural, and after a hypnotic induction, functional magnetic resonance imaging at rest. We used voxel-based morphometry to assess the correlation of grey matter (GM) and white matter (WM) against the independent variables: depth of hypnosis, level of relaxation and hypnotic suggestibility. Functional networks identified with independent components analysis were regressed with the independent variables. Hypnotic depth ratings were positively correlated with GM volume in the frontal cortex and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Hypnotic suggestibility was positively correlated with GM volume in the left temporal-occipital cortex. Relaxation ratings did not correlate significantly with GM volume and none of the independent variables correlated with regional WM volume measures. Self-reported deeper levels of hypnosis were associated with less connectivity within the anterior default mode network. Taken together, the results suggest that the greater GM volume in the medial frontal cortex and ACC, and lower connectivity in the DMN during hypnosis facilitate experiences of greater hypnotic depth. The patterns of results suggest that hypnotic depth and hypnotic suggestibility should not be considered synonyms.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping/methods , Cerebral Cortex/anatomy & histology , Hypnosis , Individuality , Nerve Net/physiology , Adult , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Middle Aged , Suggestion , Young Adult
5.
Cortex ; 49(2): 400-10, 2013 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23026758

ABSTRACT

A controversy in the field of hypnosis has centered on the question of whether there is a uniquely hypnotic state of consciousness and, if so, whether it is causally related to responsiveness to suggestion. Evidence from brain imaging studies has been used to support claims for various altered state hypotheses, without resolving the debate. The designs of many neuroimaging studies confound the induction of hypnosis with the suggestions that can be given in or out of hypnosis, thus rendering them incapable of resolving the controversy. Brain imaging studies that do not have this confound support the hypothesis that hypnotic inductions produce changes in brain activity, but also indicate that these changes are not required for the experience of hypnotic suggestions or their neural correlates. The data remain equivocal as to whether there is a causal relation between the changes in brain activity produced by hypnotic inductions and those produced by other suggestions. It also remains uncertain whether the changes in activation produced by hypnotic inductions reflect a uniquely hypnotic state as opposed to more mundane processes.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Hypnosis , Neuroimaging/methods , Consciousness , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Research Design , Suggestion
6.
Conscious Cogn ; 21(1): 100-16, 2012 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22123187

ABSTRACT

This functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) study investigated high and low suggestible people responding to two visual hallucination suggestions with and without a hypnotic induction. Participants in the study were asked to see color while looking at a grey image, and to see shades of grey while looking at a color image. High suggestible participants reported successful alterations in color perception in both tasks, both in and out of hypnosis, and showed a small benefit if hypnosis was induced. Low suggestible people could not perform the tasks successfully with or without the hypnotic induction. The fMRI results supported the self report data, and changes in brain activity were found in a number of visual areas. The results indicate that a hypnotic induction, although having the potential to enhance the ability of high suggestible people, is not necessary for the effective alteration of color perception by suggestion.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Color Perception/physiology , Hallucinations/psychology , Hypnosis , Suggestion , Adult , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Italy , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged
7.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 139(1): 225-32, 2012 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22112639

ABSTRACT

Suggesting false childhood events produces false autobiographical beliefs, memories and suggestion-consistent behavior. The mechanisms by which suggestion affects behavior are not understood, and whether false beliefs and memories are necessary for suggestions to impact behavior remains unexplored. We examined the relative effects of providing a personalized suggestion (suggesting that an event occurred to the person in the past), and/or a general suggestion (suggesting that an event happened to others in the past). Participants (N=122) received a personalized suggestion, a general suggestion, both or neither, about childhood illness due to spoiled peach yogurt. The personalized suggestion resulted in false beliefs, false memories, and suggestion-consistent behavioral intentions immediately after the suggestion. One week or one month later participants completed a taste test that involved eating varieties of crackers and yogurts. The personalized suggestion led to reduced consumption of only peach yogurt, and those who reported a false memory showed the most eating suppression. This effect on behavior was equally strong after one week and one month, showing a long lived influence of the personalized suggestion. The general suggestion showed no effects. Suggestions that convey personal information about a past event produce false autobiographical memories, which in turn impact behavior.


Subject(s)
Culture , Food Preferences/psychology , Memory, Episodic , Repression, Psychology , Suggestion , Adolescent , Affect , Eating/psychology , Female , Humans , Imagination , Male , Surveys and Questionnaires , Taste , Young Adult
8.
Conscious Cogn ; 18(4): 848-55, 2009 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19782614

ABSTRACT

The 'default mode' network refers to cortical areas that are active in the absence of goal-directed activity. In previous studies, decreased activity in the 'default mode' has always been associated with increased activation in task-relevant areas. We show that the induction of hypnosis can reduce anterior default mode activity during rest without increasing activity in other cortical regions. We assessed brain activation patterns of high and low suggestible people while resting in the fMRI scanner and while engaged in visual tasks, in and out of hypnosis. High suggestible participants in hypnosis showed decreased brain activity in the anterior parts of the default mode circuit. In low suggestible people, hypnotic induction produced no detectable changes in these regions, but instead deactivated areas involved in alertness. The findings indicate that hypnotic induction creates a distinctive and unique pattern of brain activation in highly suggestible subjects.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Hypnosis , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Adult , Arousal/physiology , Attention/physiology , Awareness/physiology , Brain Mapping , Color Perception/physiology , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Female , Humans , Judgment , Male , Middle Aged , Nerve Net/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reference Values , Suggestion , Young Adult
9.
Conscious Cogn ; 18(4): 837-47, 2009 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19709904

ABSTRACT

We examined two potential correlates of hypnotic suggestibility: dissociation and cognitive inhibition. Dissociation is the foundation of two of the major theories of hypnosis and other theories commonly postulate that hypnotic responding is a result of attentional abilities (including inhibition). Participants were administered the Waterloo-Stanford Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form C. Under the guise of an unrelated study, 180 of these participants also completed: a version of the Dissociative Experiences Scale that is normally distributed in non-clinical populations; a latent inhibition task, a spatial negative priming task, and a memory task designed to measure negative priming. The data ruled out even moderate correlations between hypnotic suggestibility and all the measures of dissociation and cognitive inhibition overall, though they also indicated gender differences. The results are a challenge for existing theories of hypnosis.


Subject(s)
Attention , Dissociative Disorders/psychology , Hypnosis , Inhibition, Psychological , Suggestion , Adolescent , Association Learning , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Personality Inventory/statistics & numerical data , Psychological Theory , Psychometrics , Reaction Time , Sex Factors , Verbal Learning , Young Adult
10.
Conscious Cogn ; 18(2): 494-9, 2009 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19289292

ABSTRACT

We administered suggestions to see a gray-scale pattern as colored and a colored pattern in shades of gray to 30 high suggestible and eight low suggestible students. The suggestions were administered twice, once following the induction of hypnosis and once without an induction. Besides rating the degree of color they saw in the stimuli differently, participants also rated their states of consciousness as normal, relaxed, hypnotized, or deeply hypnotized. Reports of being hypnotized were limited to highly suggestible participants and only after the hypnotic induction had been administered. Reports of altered color perception were also limited to high suggestibles, but were roughly comparable regardless of whether hypnosis had been induced. These data indicate that suggestible individuals do not slip into a hypnotic state when given imaginative suggestions without the induction of hypnosis, but nevertheless report experiencing difficult suggestions for profound perceptual alterations that are pheonomenologically similar to what they report in hypnosis.


Subject(s)
Color Perception , Hallucinations/psychology , Hypnosis , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Suggestion , Awareness , Consciousness , Culture , Humans , Imagination , Individuality , Personality Inventory
11.
J Exp Psychol Appl ; 14(3): 255-65, 2008 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18808279

ABSTRACT

"Don't know" (DK) responses to interview questions are conceptually heterogeneous, and may represent uncertainty or clear statements about the contents of memory. A study examined the subjective intent of DK responses in relation to the objective status of information queried, in the context of memory distorting procedures. Participants viewed a video and responded to answerable and unanswerable questions phrased in misleading or nonmisleading formats, while hypnotized or not hypnotized. Subjective meanings of DK responses were queried, and a recognition measure assessed the contents of memory. Lower DK and accuracy rates were consistently associated with unanswerable and misleading questions. One-third of DK responses were statements that the information had no not presented. When these were recoded, accuracy estimates for answerable questions decreased and more so for hypnotized participants. These results demonstrate that DK responses convey different types of information, thus accuracy estimates in studies that permit DK responses may be misestimated. Robust risks associated with asking unanswerable questions and asking questions at all were observed. Implications for working with DK responses during interviews are discussed.


Subject(s)
Culture , Hypnosis , Mental Recall , Suggestion , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Interview, Psychological , Male , Theft/psychology , Video Recording
12.
Int J Clin Exp Hypn ; 56(3): 295-305, 2008 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18569140

ABSTRACT

Portuguese norms for the Waterloo-Stanford Group C (WSGC) scale of hypnotic susceptibility are presented. A Portuguese translation of this scale was given to 625 Portuguese college students. Score distribution, item analysis, and reliability of the WSGC are presented and compared to three North American samples. The findings show that normative data from the Portuguese sample are congruent with the reference samples. The only significant difference obtained was a lower proportion of participants scoring within the high range of hypnotic suggestibility on the WSGC.


Subject(s)
Cross-Cultural Comparison , Hypnosis/methods , Personality Inventory/statistics & numerical data , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Portugal , Psychometrics/statistics & numerical data , Reference Values , Reproducibility of Results , Students/psychology , Suggestion
13.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 128(2): 304-9, 2008 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18417080

ABSTRACT

Previous studies have shown that suggesting childhood events can influence current self-reported attitudes towards future behavior. This study shows that suggesting a false past event (i.e. becoming sick on a specific food during childhood) can modify present behavior (i.e. reduce eating of the food). Participants screened to be normal eaters received or did not receive a suggestion that they likely became sick on spoiled peach yogurt as a child. One week later they took part in an allegedly separate marketing taste-test study, during which they rated preferences for a variety of crackers and yogurts. After completing ratings, participants were invited to freely eat the remaining food while completing questionnaires. Results revealed that the participants receiving the suggestion expressed lower preference specifically for peach yogurt, and ate less yogurt of all the types, while not differing in eating of crackers. These results demonstrate that suggesting false past events influences subsequent behavior.


Subject(s)
Energy Intake , Feeding Behavior , Suggestion , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Culture , Female , Food Preferences , Humans , Surveys and Questionnaires
14.
Int J Clin Exp Hypn ; 56(2): 143-55, 2008 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18307125

ABSTRACT

The effects of implementation intentions and posthypnotic suggestion were investigated in 2 studies. In Experiment 1, participants with high levels of hypnotic suggestibility were instructed to take placebo pills as part of an investigation of how to best enhance compliance with medical instruction. In Experiment 2, participants with high, medium, and low levels of hypnotic suggestibility were asked to run in place, take their pulse rate before, and send an e-mail report to the experimenter each day. Experiment 1 revealed enhanced adherence as a function of both implementation intentions and posthypnotic suggestion. Experiment 2 failed to find any significant main effects but found a significant interaction between suggestibility and the effects of posthypnotic suggestion. Posthypnotic suggestion enhanced adherence among high suggestible participants but lowered it among low suggestibles.


Subject(s)
Goals , Hypnosis , Intention , Patient Compliance , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Suggestion
15.
Cogn Process ; 9(2): 137-43, 2008 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18214564

ABSTRACT

Object imagery refers to the ability to construct pictorial images of objects. Individuals with high object imagery (high-OI) produce more vivid mental images than individuals with low object imagery (low-OI), and they encode and process both mental images and visual stimuli in a more global and holistic way. In the present study, we investigated whether and how level of object imagery may affect the way in which individuals identify visual objects. High-OI and low-OI participants were asked to perform a visual identification task with spatially-filtered pictures of real objects. Each picture was presented at nine levels of filtering, starting from the most blurred (level 1: only low spatial frequencies--global configuration) and gradually adding high spatial frequencies up to the complete version (level 9: global configuration plus local and internal details). Our data showed that high-OI participants identified stimuli at a lower level of filtering than participants with low-OI, indicating that they were better able than low-OI participants to identify visual objects at lower spatial frequencies. Implications of the results and future developments are discussed.


Subject(s)
Identification, Psychological , Imagination/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Vision, Ocular/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Male , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Psychophysics , Statistics, Nonparametric , Surveys and Questionnaires
16.
Am J Clin Hypn ; 49(4): 249-54, 2007 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17444362

ABSTRACT

This article responds to comments on Kirsch, Mazzoni, & Montgomery (2007). Contrary to the perceptions of some commentators, the target article was not aimed at supporting a particular view of hypnosis. Instead, it was a reminder of a long accepted axiom in hypnosis research: the effects of hypnotic suggestions cannot be attributed to hypnosis unless it is demonstrated that the same suggestion does not produce the effect outside of hypnosis.


Subject(s)
Hypnosis/history , History, 20th Century , History, 21st Century , Humans
17.
Ann Behav Med ; 33(1): 112-6, 2007 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17291177

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Expectancy and modeling have been cited as factors in mass psychogenic illness (MPI), which reportedly affects more women than men. PURPOSE: The purpose of the study is to assess the effects of expectancy and modeling in a controlled laboratory analogue of MPI. METHODS: Students were randomly assigned to inhale or not inhale an inert placebo described as a suspected environmental toxin that had been linked to four symptoms typical of reported instances of MPI. Half of the students observed a female confederate inhale the substance and subsequently display the specified symptoms. RESULTS: Students who inhaled the placebo reported greater increases in symptoms, and the increase was significantly greater for the specified symptoms than for other symptoms. Observation of the confederate displaying symptoms increased specified symptoms significantly among women but not among men. Changes in reported symptoms were significantly associated with changes in unobtrusively observed behavior. CONCLUSIONS: Symptoms typical of clinical reports of MPI can be induced by manipulating response expectancies, and the effects are specific rather than generalized. Among women, this effect is enhanced by observing another participant (who in this study is also female) display symptoms. This suggests that the preponderance of women showing symptoms in outbreaks of MPI may be due to gender-linked differences in the effects of modeling on psychogenic symptoms.


Subject(s)
Psychophysiologic Disorders/psychology , Sick Role , Suggestion , Adolescent , Adult , Connecticut , Culture , Environmental Exposure/adverse effects , Female , Humans , Imitative Behavior , Male , Psychophysiologic Disorders/diagnosis , Set, Psychology , Sex Factors
18.
Am J Clin Hypn ; 49(3): 171-8; discussion 179-80, 183-4, 2007 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17265971

ABSTRACT

The history of the most enduring experimental design in hypnosis research is reviewed. More than 75 years of research converge to indicate that: (1) all of the phenomena produced in hypnosis by suggestion also can be produced by suggestion without the induction of hypnosis, (2) the induction of hypnosis produces a relatively small increase in responsiveness to suggestion, and (3) hypnotic and waking suggestion are highly correlated, in many cases rivalling the reliability of the suggestibility measure. The importance of these data to both clinical and experimental hypnosis is emphasized.


Subject(s)
Hypnosis/history , History, 20th Century , History, 21st Century , Humans , Suggestion
19.
Int J Clin Exp Hypn ; 54(3): 340-59, 2006 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16858906

ABSTRACT

In 2002, the first author and colleagues reported data indicating that both hypnosis and misleading questions decreased the accuracy of memory reports and decreased "don't know" response rates, that the effects of misleading questions were significantly greater than those of hypnosis, and that the two effects were additive. Using a sample of 194 undergraduate students, the present study replicated the findings that misleading questions reduce accuracy and "don't know" responding but failed to replicate the negative effect of hypnosis on memory reports. Signal detection analysis indicated that misleading questioning produced decreased sensitivity accompanied by higher response bias, though affecting sensitivity more than producing a criterion shift.


Subject(s)
Communication , Hypnosis/methods , Memory , Signal Detection, Psychological , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Semantics , Suggestion
20.
Pap. psicol ; 25(89): 0-0, sept.-dic. 2004. tab
Article in Spanish | IBECS | ID: ibc-140468

ABSTRACT

La hipnosis es una técnica carente de riesgos per se. Sin embargo, un uso inadecuado de tal técnica puede ser iatrógeno, especialmente en lo referido a la génesis de falsos recuerdos por parte de la persona hipnotizada. Las creencias erróneas y mitos sobre las potencialidades de la hipnosis transmitidos por los medios de comunicación y los propios terapeutas (especialmente los que se hacen llamar hipnoterapeutas) suelen ser, en última instancia, los responsables de los riesgos de la hipnosis. Por lo tanto, una forma de ayudar a prevenir tales riesgos es proporcionar una información rigurosa y veraz al cliente al que se pretende hipnotizar o que solicita la hipnosis (AU)


Hypnosis is a technique that lacks intrinsic risks. Nevertheless, a wrong use of hypnosis can be dangerous due to the increased probability of creating false memories in the hypnotized person. Myths and erroneous conceptions about hypnosis potentialities that are usually transmitted by the media and some therapists (especially the so called "hypnotherapists"), are responsible, among others, of the risks posed by hypnosis. Therefore, one way of preventing such risks is to provide the clients who request hypnosis with accurate and rigorous information about this therapeutic technique (AU)


Subject(s)
Female , Humans , Male , Hypnosis/ethics , Hypnosis/methods , Vascular Headaches/metabolism , Vascular Headaches/psychology , Schizophrenia, Paranoid/complications , Schizophrenia, Paranoid/psychology , Societies/ethics , Societies/legislation & jurisprudence , Hypnosis/instrumentation , Hypnosis/statistics & numerical data , Vascular Headaches/genetics , Vascular Headaches/pathology , Schizophrenia, Paranoid/genetics , Schizophrenia, Paranoid/metabolism , Societies/methods , Societies/policies
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL