Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 308
Filter
Add more filters

Complementary Medicines
Publication year range
1.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 7906, 2021 04 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33846432

ABSTRACT

Transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation (taVNS) is a novel non-invasive brain stimulation technique considered as a potential supplementary treatment option for subjects with refractory epilepsy. Its exact mechanism of action is not yet fully understood. We developed an examination schedule to probe for immediate taVNS-induced modifications of large-scale epileptic brain networks and accompanying changes of cognition and behaviour. In this prospective trial, we applied short-term (1 h) taVNS to 14 subjects with epilepsy during a continuous 3-h EEG recording which was embedded in two standardized neuropsychological assessments. From these EEG, we derived evolving epileptic brain networks and tracked important topological, robustness, and stability properties of networks over time. In the majority of investigated subjects, taVNS induced measurable and persisting modifications in network properties that point to a more resilient epileptic brain network without negatively impacting cognition, behaviour, or mood. The stimulation was well tolerated and the usability of the device was rated good. Short-term taVNS has a topology-modifying, robustness- and stability-enhancing immediate effect on large-scale epileptic brain networks. It has no detrimental effects on cognition and behaviour. Translation into clinical practice requires further studies to detail knowledge about the exact mechanisms by which taVNS prevents or inhibits seizures.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiopathology , Ear Auricle/physiopathology , Epilepsy/physiopathology , Nerve Net/physiopathology , Transcutaneous Electric Nerve Stimulation , Vagus Nerve Stimulation , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Behavior/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Young Adult
2.
Cereb Cortex ; 31(2): 949-960, 2021 01 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33026425

ABSTRACT

Living in rapidly changing environments has shaped the mammalian brain toward high sensitivity to abrupt and intense sensory events-often signaling threats or affordances requiring swift reactions. Unsurprisingly, such events elicit a widespread electrocortical response (the vertex potential, VP), likely related to the preparation of appropriate behavioral reactions. Although the VP magnitude is largely determined by stimulus intensity, the relative contribution of the differential and absolute components of intensity remains unknown. Here, we dissociated the effects of these two components. We systematically varied the size of abrupt intensity increases embedded within continuous stimulation at different absolute intensities, while recording brain activity in humans (with scalp electroencephalography) and rats (with epidural electrocorticography). We obtained three main results. 1) VP magnitude largely depends on differential, and not absolute, stimulus intensity. This result held true, 2) for both auditory and somatosensory stimuli, indicating that sensitivity to differential intensity is supramodal, and 3) in both humans and rats, suggesting that sensitivity to abrupt intensity differentials is phylogenetically well-conserved. Altogether, the current results show that these large electrocortical responses are most sensitive to the detection of sensory changes that more likely signal the sudden appearance of novel objects or events in the environment.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Aged , Animals , Behavior/physiology , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Electrocorticography , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Auditory/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Somatosensory/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Phylogeny , Rats , Rats, Sprague-Dawley , Species Specificity , Young Adult
3.
PLoS One ; 15(10): e0240832, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33119665

ABSTRACT

Hypnosis is a powerful tool to affect the processing and perception of stimuli. Here, we investigated the effects of hypnosis on the processing of auditory stimuli, the time course of event-related-potentials (ERP; N1 and P3b amplitudes) and the activity of cortical sources of the P3b component. Forty-eight participants completed an auditory oddball paradigm composed of standard, distractor, and target stimuli during a hypnosis (HYP), a simulation of hypnosis (SIM), a distraction (DIS), and a control (CON) condition. During HYP, participants were suggested that an earplug would obstruct the perception of tones and during SIM they should pretend being hypnotized and obstructed to hear the tones. During DIS, participants' attention was withdrawn from the tones by focusing participants' attention onto a film. In each condition, subjects were asked to press a key whenever a target stimulus was presented. Behavioral data show that target hit rates and response time became significantly reduced during HYP and SIM and loudness ratings of tones were only reduced during HYP. Distraction from stimuli by the film was less effective in reducing target hit rate and tone loudness. Although, the N1 amplitude was not affected by the experimental conditions, the P3b amplitude was significantly reduced in HYP and SIM compared to CON and DIS. In addition, source localization results indicate that only a small number of neural sources organize the differences of tone processing between the control condition and the distraction, hypnosis, and simulation of hypnosis conditions. These sources belong to brain areas that control the focus of attention, the discrimination of auditory stimuli, and the organization of behavioral responses to targets. Our data confirm that deafness suggestions significantly change auditory processing and perception but complete deafness is hard to achieve during HYP. Therefore, the term 'deafness' may be misleading and should better be replaced by 'hypoacusis'.


Subject(s)
Brain/diagnostic imaging , Cognition/physiology , Deafness/physiopathology , Hypnosis/methods , Acoustic Stimulation , Adolescent , Adult , Attention/physiology , Auditory Perception , Behavior/physiology , Brain/physiology , Brain/physiopathology , Deafness/diagnostic imaging , Deafness/etiology , Electroencephalography/methods , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Auditory/physiology , Female , Hearing/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Speech Perception/physiology , Young Adult
4.
PLoS One ; 15(4): e0226789, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32240174

ABSTRACT

Exploration in reward-based motor learning is observable in experimental data as increased variability. In order to quantify exploration, we compare three methods for estimating other sources of variability: sensorimotor noise. We use a task in which participants could receive stochastic binary reward feedback following a target-directed weight shift. Participants first performed six baseline blocks without feedback, and next twenty blocks alternating with and without feedback. Variability was assessed based on trial-to-trial changes in movement endpoint. We estimated sensorimotor noise by the median squared trial-to-trial change in movement endpoint for trials in which no exploration is expected. We identified three types of such trials: trials in baseline blocks, trials in the blocks without feedback, and rewarded trials in the blocks with feedback. We estimated exploration by the median squared trial-to-trial change following non-rewarded trials minus sensorimotor noise. As expected, variability was larger following non-rewarded trials than following rewarded trials. This indicates that our reward-based weight-shifting task successfully induced exploration. Most importantly, our three estimates of sensorimotor noise differed: the estimate based on rewarded trials was significantly lower than the estimates based on the two types of trials without feedback. Consequently, the estimates of exploration also differed. We conclude that the quantification of exploration depends critically on the type of trials used to estimate sensorimotor noise. We recommend the use of variability following rewarded trials.


Subject(s)
Learning/physiology , Motor Activity/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Behavior/physiology , Biofeedback, Psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Musculoskeletal Physiological Phenomena , Reaction Time/physiology , Research Design , Reward , Statistical Distributions , Young Adult
5.
Clin EEG Neurosci ; 51(2): 87-93, 2020 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31423818

ABSTRACT

Serious (biofeedback) games offer promising ways to supplement or replace more expensive face-to-face interventions in health care. However, studies on the validity and effectiveness of EEG-based serious games remain scarce. In the current study, we investigated whether the conditions of the neurofeedback game "Daydream" indeed trained the brain activity as mentioned in the game manual. EEG activity was assessed in 14 healthy male volunteers while playing the 2 conditions of the game. The participants completed a training of 5 sessions. EEG frequency analyses were performed to verify the claims of the manual. We found significant differences in α- to ß-ratio between the 2 conditions although only in the amplitude data, not in the power data. Within the conditions, mean α-amplitude only differed significantly from the ß-amplitude in the concentration condition. Our analyses showed that neither α nor ß brain activity differed significantly between game levels (higher level requiring increased brain activity) in either of the two conditions. In conclusion, we found only marginal evidence for the proposed claims stated in the manual of the game. Our research emphasizes that it is crucial to validate the claims that serious games make, especially before implementing them in the clinic or as therapeutic devices.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Behavior/physiology , Neurofeedback/physiology , Adult , Electroencephalography/methods , Games, Experimental , Healthy Volunteers , Humans , Male
6.
Psychosom Med ; 81(8): 720-730, 2019 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31343581

ABSTRACT

Research on stress and disease has often afforded an important role to emotion, typically conceptualized in broad categories (e.g., negative emotions), viewed as playing a causal role (e.g., anger contributing to pathophysiology of cardiovascular disease), and measured using self-report inventories. In this article, I argue for the value of evaluating specific emotions, considering bidirectional causal influences, and assessing actual emotional responding when considering the role that emotions play in the stress-disease relationship. In terms of specificity, specific emotions (e.g., anger, sadness, and embarrassment) can be linked with particular health outcomes (e.g., cardiovascular disease and musculoskeletal disease). In terms of bidirectionality, the influences of emotions on disease as well as the influences of disease on emotional functioning can be considered. In terms of assessing actual emotional responding, emotions can be studied in vivo under controlled conditions that allow behavioral, physiological, and subjective responses to be measured during different kinds of emotional functioning (e.g., responding to emotional stimuli, interacting with relationship partners, and downregulating emotional responses). With these considerations in mind, I review early theories and empirical studies in psychosomatic medicine that considered the role of specific emotions and emotion-related behaviors. Studies from our laboratory are presented that illustrate a) differences in patterns of autonomic nervous system responding associated with specific emotions, b) relationships between specific emotions and particular health outcomes in the context of social relationships, c) age as a moderator of the relationship between specific emotions and well-being, d) bidirectional influences (emotions influencing disease and disease influencing emotional functioning), and e) impact of changes in emotional functioning in individuals with neurodegenerative diseases on the health of familial caregivers.


Subject(s)
Emotions/physiology , Social Determinants of Health , Stress, Psychological/physiopathology , Aging/physiology , Aging/psychology , Anger/physiology , Autonomic Nervous System/physiology , Behavior/physiology , Brain/physiology , Burnout, Psychological/physiopathology , Burnout, Psychological/psychology , Caregivers/psychology , Causality , Dementia/physiopathology , Dementia/psychology , Disease Susceptibility , Disgust , Embarrassment , Facial Expression , Female , Hemodynamics , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Life Change Events , Male , Stress, Psychological/psychology
7.
Rev Mal Respir ; 36(3): 342-349, 2019 Mar.
Article in French | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30902446

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The midwife, in taking on a public health role, is one of the most important resources for the prevention of smoking and in helping smoking cessation among women of childbearing age thanks to their numerous contacts with pregnant women. With this in mind, we conducted a study among student midwives to examine their smoking behavior, their attitudes towards smoking, and their participation in prevention. METHOD: This was a descriptive cross-sectional study conducted from 15 January to 15 February 2018 using a self-administered questionnaire and included the student midwives of the University of Health Science at Libreville (Gabon). RESULTS: A total of 188 student midwives completed the questionnaires (70.7% of students of the 1st year, 15.0% of students of the 2nd year and 14.3% of students of the 3rd year). Gaps exist in the knowledge of student midwives regarding the risks of cigarette smoking in pregnancy and its role in the development of complications for the mother and foetus. Overall, 17.1% of student midwives think that smoking is responsible for the occurrence of ectopic pregnancies, 20.3% believe it is responsible for retro-placental haematoma, 17.6% for premature rupture of the membranes. The prevalence of smoking was 11.1% and was most frequent in 2nd year students (25.0%) and 3rd year students (14.8%) (P<0.023). The mean age of beginning smoking was 19.0±4.4 years. The main initiating factors were peer influence (28.6%), pleasure (19.0%) and stress (14.3%). Nicotine dependence was weak to moderate among 48.8% of smokers and absent in 52.2%. CONCLUSION: Gaps exist in the knowledge of student midwives regarding the risks of cigarette smoking to complications of pregnancy. There is need therefore to include formal training on tobacco control strategies at an early stage in the medical curriculum.


Subject(s)
Attitude of Health Personnel , Behavior/physiology , Midwifery , Smoking , Students, Nursing , Adolescent , Adult , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Gabon/epidemiology , Health Knowledge, Attitudes, Practice , Humans , Midwifery/education , Midwifery/statistics & numerical data , Smoking/epidemiology , Smoking/psychology , Smoking Cessation/statistics & numerical data , Students, Nursing/psychology , Students, Nursing/statistics & numerical data , Surveys and Questionnaires , Nicotiana , Young Adult
8.
J Nutr ; 149(4): 687-697, 2019 04 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30926992

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Evidence suggests that iron deficiency (ID) affects cognitive performance, as measured in behavior. Although such effects must be mediated by changes in the brain, very few studies have included measures of brain activity to assess this relation. OBJECTIVE: We tested the hypothesis that provision of iron-biofortified beans would result in improvements in measures of iron status, brain dynamics, and behavior. METHODS: A double-blind, randomized, intervention study was conducted in 55 women aged 18-27 y with low iron status (serum ferritin <20 µg/L). Women were randomly assigned to consume iron-biofortified (86.1 ppm iron) or comparison beans (50.1 ppm iron) daily for 18 wk. Iron status was assessed by hemoglobin, ferritin, transferrin receptor, and body iron; cognitive performance with 5 computerized tasks; and brain dynamics by concurrent electroencephalography (EEG). All measures were taken at baseline and endline. RESULTS: The groups did not differ on any measures at baseline. Intention-to-treat analyses revealed significant (all P < 0.05) improvements in hemoglobin (partial effect size attributable to the independent variable, η2 = 0.16), ferritin (η2 = 0.17), and body iron (η2 = 0.10), speed of responding in attentional and mnemonic tasks (η2 = 0.04-0.29), sensitivity and efficiency of memory retrieval (η2 = 0.12-0.55), and measures of EEG amplitude and spectral power (η2 = 0.08 to 0.49). Mediation models provided evidence in support of the hypothesis that changes in iron status produce changes in behavior by way of changes in brain activity. CONCLUSIONS: Behavioral performance and brain activity, as measured by EEG, are sensitive to iron status, and the consumption of iron-biofortified beans for 18 wk resulted in improvements in measures of both, relative to what was obtained with a comparison bean, in a sample of female university students. Furthermore, the results support the conclusion that changes in brain activity resulting from consumption of biofortified beans mediate the relations between changes in iron biomarkers and changes in cognition. Clinical trial registry: ClinicalTrials.gov Reg No. NCT01594359.


Subject(s)
Behavior/drug effects , Cognition/drug effects , Fabaceae , Food, Fortified , Iron/blood , Adolescent , Adult , Behavior/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Double-Blind Method , Female , Humans , Iron/administration & dosage , Rwanda/epidemiology , Young Adult
9.
Chronobiol Int ; 36(3): 332-342, 2019 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30557062

ABSTRACT

The circadian system influences virtually all biological functions. Understanding the impact of circadian variation on metabolism may provide insight into mechanisms of circadian-associated disorders and guide the implementation of chrono-therapy. Previous research has reported circadian variation in 7-20% of metabolites in human blood. In this study, untargeted metabolomics profiles were measured using blood of two healthy men and one healthy woman, collected every 2 h for up to 48 h under carefully controlled conditions. The pattern of variation of each metabolite over time was examined on each participant separately, using both one- and two-order harmonic models. A total of 100 of 663 metabolites, representing all metabolite categories, showed diurnal rhythmic concentrations that exceeded the Bonferroni threshold (P < 2.5 × 10-5). Overall, peak times of many metabolites were clustered during the afternoon-midnight, including the majority of amino acids, all peptides, all lysolipids and all phospholipids, whereas the majority of steroids peaked in the morning. We observed substantial inter-individual variation for both peak times and amplitudes in these three subjects. In conclusion, at least 15% of blood metabolites, representing a broad group of biological pathways, exhibit diurnal variation in three participants. The average peak times of most of these metabolites are clustered in morning or afternoon-midnight. Further work is needed to validate and extend this work in more individuals.


Subject(s)
Circadian Rhythm/physiology , Hydrocortisone/blood , Steroids/blood , Time Factors , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Behavior/physiology , Female , Humans , Male
10.
Neurosci Lett ; 702: 24-33, 2019 05 29.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30503923

ABSTRACT

Chronic pain is a multidimensional experience with cognitive, affective, and somatosensory components that can be modified by expectations and learning. Individual differences in cognitive and affective processing, as well as contextual aspects of the pain experience, render chronic pain an inherently personal experience. Such individual differences are supported by the heterogeneity of brain representations within and across chronic pain pathologies. In this review, we discuss the complexity of brain representations of pain, and, with respect to this complexity, identify common elements of network-level disruptions in chronic pain. Specifically, we identify prefrontal-limbic circuitry and the default mode network as key elements of functional disruption. We then discuss how these disrupted circuits can be targeted through self-regulation and related cognitive strategies to alleviate chronic pain. We conclude with a proposal for how to develop personalized multivariate models of pain representation in the brain and target them with real-time neurofeedback, so that patients can explore and practice self-regulatory techniques with maximal efficiency.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiopathology , Chronic Pain/physiopathology , Chronic Pain/psychology , Cognitive Behavioral Therapy , Acceptance and Commitment Therapy , Animals , Behavior/physiology , Chronic Pain/therapy , Humans , Meditation , Neurofeedback
11.
Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol ; 316(2): R157-R164, 2019 02 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30521366

ABSTRACT

Measurements of aldosterone for diagnosis of primary aldosteronism are usually made from blood sampled in the morning when aldosterone typically peaks. We tested the relative contributions and interacting influences of the circadian system, ongoing behaviors, and prior sleep to this morning peak in aldosterone. To determine circadian rhythmicity and separate effects of behaviors on aldosterone, 16 healthy participants completed a 5-day protocol in dim light while all behaviors ranging from sleep to exercise were standardized and scheduled evenly across the 24-h circadian period. In another experiment, to test the separate effects of prior nocturnal sleep or the inactivity that accompanies sleep on aldosterone, 10 healthy participants were studied across 2 nights: 1 with sleep and 1 with maintained wakefulness (randomized order). Plasma aldosterone was measured repeatedly in each experiment. Aldosterone had a significant endogenous rhythm ( P < 0.001), rising across the circadian night and peaking in the morning (~8 AM). Activity, including exercise, increased aldosterone, and different behaviors modulated aldosterone differently across the circadian cycle (circadian phase × behavior interaction; P < 0.001). In the second experiment, prior nocturnal sleep and prior rested wakefulness both increased plasma aldosterone ( P < 0.001) in the morning, to the same extent as the change in circadian phases between evening and morning. The morning increase in aldosterone is due to effects of the circadian system plus increased morning activities and not prior sleep or the inactivity accompanying sleep. These findings have implications for the time of and behaviors preceding measurement of aldosterone, especially under conditions of shift work and jet lag.


Subject(s)
Aldosterone/blood , Behavior/physiology , Circadian Rhythm/physiology , Wakefulness/physiology , Adult , Body Temperature/physiology , Exercise/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Sleep/physiology , Time Factors
12.
Brain Topogr ; 32(2): 315-331, 2019 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30498872

ABSTRACT

As we fall sleep, our brain traverses a series of gradual changes at physiological, behavioural and cognitive levels, which are not yet fully understood. The loss of responsiveness is a critical event in the transition from wakefulness to sleep. Here we seek to understand the electrophysiological signatures that reflect the loss of capacity to respond to external stimuli during drowsiness using two complementary methods: spectral connectivity and EEG microstates. Furthermore, we integrate these two methods for the first time by investigating the connectivity patterns captured during individual microstate lifetimes. While participants performed an auditory semantic classification task, we allowed them to become drowsy and unresponsive. As they stopped responding to the stimuli, we report the breakdown of alpha networks and the emergence of theta connectivity. Further, we show that the temporal dynamics of all canonical EEG microstates slow down during unresponsiveness. We identify a specific microstate (D) whose occurrence and duration are prominently increased during this period. Employing machine learning, we show that the temporal properties of microstate D, particularly its prolonged duration, predicts the response likelihood to individual stimuli. Finally, we find a novel relationship between microstates and brain networks as we show that microstate D uniquely indexes significantly stronger theta connectivity during unresponsiveness. Our findings demonstrate that the transition to unconsciousness is not linear, but rather consists of an interplay between transient brain networks reflecting different degrees of sleep depth.


Subject(s)
Behavior/physiology , Brain Mapping/methods , Electroencephalography , Neural Pathways/physiology , Sleepiness , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Alpha Rhythm/physiology , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Female , Humans , Machine Learning , Male , Psychomotor Performance , Reaction Time/physiology , Theta Rhythm/physiology , Young Adult
13.
Eur J Neurosci ; 49(6): 849-858, 2019 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30375083

ABSTRACT

It is well established clinically that rhythmic auditory cues can improve gait and other motor behaviors in Parkinson's disease (PD) and other disorders. However, the neural systems underlying this therapeutic effect are largely unknown. To investigate this question we scanned people with PD and age-matched healthy controls using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). All subjects performed a rhythmic motor behavior (right hand finger tapping) with and without simultaneous auditory rhythmic cues at two different speeds (1 and 4 Hz). We used spatial independent component analysis (ICA) and regression to identify task-related functional connectivity networks and assessed differences between groups in intra- and inter-network connectivity. Overall, the control group showed greater intra-network connectivity in perceptual and motor related networks during motor tapping both with and without rhythmic cues. The PD group showed greater inter-network connectivity between the auditory network and the executive control network, and between the executive control network and the motor/cerebellar network associated with the motor task performance. We interpret our results as indicating that the temporal rhythmic auditory information may assist compensatory mechanisms through network-level effects, reflected in increased interaction between auditory and executive networks that in turn modulate activity in cortico-cerebellar networks.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Cues , Neural Pathways/physiopathology , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Behavior/physiology , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Middle Aged
14.
PLoS One ; 13(11): e0207439, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30462695

ABSTRACT

The purpose of the current study was to examine the relationship between expertise, performance, and gaze behavior in a complex error-detection cockpit task. Twenty-four pilots and 26 non-pilots viewed video-clips from a pilot's viewpoint and were asked to detect malfunctions in the cockpit instrument panel. Compared to non-pilots, pilots detected more malfunctioning instruments, had shorter dwell times on the instruments, made more transitions, visited task-relevant areas more often, and dwelled longer on the areas between the instruments. These results provide evidence for three theories that explain underlying processes for expert performance: The long-term working memory theory, the information-reduction hypothesis, and the holistic model of image perception. In addition, the results for generic attentional skills indicated a higher capability to switch between global and local information processing in pilots compared to non-pilots. Taken together, the results suggest that gaze behavior as well as other generic skills may provide important information concerning underlying processes that can explain successful performance during flight in expert pilots.


Subject(s)
Aerospace Medicine , Attention/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Task Performance and Analysis , Adult , Aircraft , Aviation , Behavior/physiology , Electronic Data Processing , Humans , Male , Pilots
15.
Nestle Nutr Inst Workshop Ser ; 89: 131-142, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29991038

ABSTRACT

The perspective shared here is that of a nutritionist who has been collaborating with a behavioral scientist for 20 years. Examples will be related to long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids, the subject of our collaboration. While it is well accepted that nutrition is key to optimal human health and development, nutrition intervention trials in populations and randomized controlled trials of specific nutrients that have measured these outcomes have occurred relatively recently. Studies of nutrition and behavior are even less common - the first appears to have been a protein intervention that began in 1969 in Guatemala that involved developmental follow-up to adulthood. When results of multiple trials are available, findings of individual trials frequently range from no effect to benefit, making it difficult to make decisions about policy and practice. A meta-analysis that combines the results of all randomized trials of a nutrient is considered the highest level of evidence for drug trials. For studies of nutrient supplementation, however, meta-analyses can err on the side of no effect and lead to assumptions that a nutrient is adequate in populations with deficient or marginal status. In studies that assess behavior, collaboration with a behavioral scientist is necessary to determine the behavioral outcome(s) to assess, ensure the proper administration of the outcome, and analyze and interpret the results. The goal of the paper is to offer insight into issues common to all nutrition research, but especially issues unique to studies that assess behavior. As noted, industry, governments, health organizations, journals, as well as scientists have roles to play.


Subject(s)
Behavior/physiology , Interdisciplinary Communication , Nutritional Physiological Phenomena , Animals , Arachidonic Acid/administration & dosage , Biomedical Research , Breast Feeding , Clinical Trials as Topic , Docosahexaenoic Acids/administration & dosage , Female , Humans , Infant , Infant, Newborn , Infant, Premature , Nutrition Therapy , Nutritional Requirements , Nutritional Sciences , Nutritionists , Pregnancy , Quality Control
16.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 88: 73-83, 2018 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29548931

ABSTRACT

There is mounting scientific evidence showing the importance of innate biological rhythms on disease onset and progression. Perhaps the most important of these is the circadian rhythm, a cycle of oscillations lasting approximately 24 h. Recent work has shown that circadian rhythms are intrinsically linked to the immune system in a bidirectional fashion, and that disruption of these cycles can contribute to changes in pathology and quality of life (including fatigue, mood, and disability). This is particularly true in diseases of the nervous and immune systems. We review here the current preclinical and clinical literature to highlight interactions between circadian rhythms and multiple sclerosis, as well as its animal model, experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis. We highlight potential benefits of chronotherapy (the temporal administration of immunomodulatory drugs) in an effort to increase treatment efficacy and reduce the negative side-effects of the drugs that often burden those suffering from the disease.


Subject(s)
Chronobiology Phenomena/physiology , Circadian Rhythm/physiology , Drug-Related Side Effects and Adverse Reactions/physiopathology , Multiple Sclerosis/physiopathology , Animals , Behavior/physiology , Drug-Related Side Effects and Adverse Reactions/psychology , Humans , Multiple Sclerosis/psychology , Quality of Life
17.
Nature ; 554(7692): 368-372, 2018 02 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29414944

ABSTRACT

Many models of cognition and of neural computations posit the use and estimation of prior stimulus statistics: it has long been known that working memory and perception are strongly impacted by previous sensory experience, even when that sensory history is not relevant to the current task at hand. Nevertheless, the neural mechanisms and regions of the brain that are necessary for computing and using such prior experience are unknown. Here we report that the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) is a critical locus for the representation and use of prior stimulus information. We trained rats in an auditory parametric working memory task, and found that they displayed substantial and readily quantifiable behavioural effects of sensory-stimulus history, similar to those observed in humans and monkeys. Earlier proposals that the PPC supports working memory predict that optogenetic silencing of this region would impair behaviour in our working memory task. Contrary to this prediction, we found that silencing the PPC significantly improved performance. Quantitative analyses of behaviour revealed that this improvement was due to the selective reduction of the effects of prior sensory stimuli. Electrophysiological recordings showed that PPC neurons carried far more information about the sensory stimuli of previous trials than about the stimuli of the current trial. Furthermore, for a given rat, the more information about previous trial sensory history in the neural firing rates of the PPC, the greater the behavioural effect of sensory history, suggesting a tight link between behaviour and PPC representations of stimulus history. Our results indicate that the PPC is a central component in the processing of sensory-stimulus history, and could enable further neurobiological investigation of long-standing questions regarding how perception and working memory are affected by prior sensory information.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Behavior/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Touch Perception/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Animals , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Neurons/physiology , Optogenetics , Parietal Lobe/cytology , Psychometrics , Rats , Rats, Long-Evans , Young Adult
18.
J Neurosci Res ; 96(4): 671-687, 2018 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28801920

ABSTRACT

In 2000, a landmark case report described the concurrent restoration of consciousness and thalamo-frontal connectivity after severe brain injury (Laureys et al., ). Being a single case however, this study could not disambiguate whether the result was specific to the restoration of consciousness per se as opposed to the return of complex cognitive function in general or simply the temporal evolution of post-injury pathophysiological events. To test whether the restoration of thalamo-cortical connectivity is specific to consciousness, 20 moderate-to-severe brain injury patients (from a recruited sample of 42) underwent resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging within a week after injury and again six months later. As described in the single case report, we find thalamo-frontal connectivity to be increased at the chronic, compared with the acute, time-point. The increased connectivity was independent of whether patients had already recovered consciousness prior to the first assessment or whether they recovered consciousness in-between the two. Conversely, we did find an association between restoration of thalamo-frontal connectivity and the return of complex cognitive function. While we did replicate the findings of Laureys et al. (), our data suggests that the restoration of thalamo-frontal connectivity is not as tightly linked to the reemergence of consciousness per se. However, the degree to which the return of connectivity is linked to the return of complex cognitive function, or to the evolution of other time-dependent post-injury mechanisms, remains to be understood.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/pathology , Consciousness/physiology , Thalamus/pathology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Behavior/physiology , Brain Injuries , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Female , Glasgow Coma Scale , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Nerve Regeneration , Prospective Studies , Thalamus/physiology , Young Adult
19.
PLoS Biol ; 15(12): e2003143, 2017 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29206225

ABSTRACT

The brain is thought to generate internal predictions to optimize behaviour. However, it is unclear whether predictions signalling is an automatic brain function or depends on task demands. Here, we manipulated the spatial/temporal predictability of visual targets, and the relevance of spatial/temporal information provided by auditory cues. We used magnetoencephalography (MEG) to measure participants' brain activity during task performance. Task relevance modulated the influence of predictions on behaviour: spatial/temporal predictability improved spatial/temporal discrimination accuracy, but not vice versa. To explain these effects, we used behavioural responses to estimate subjective predictions under an ideal-observer model. Model-based time-series of predictions and prediction errors (PEs) were associated with dissociable neural responses: predictions correlated with cue-induced beta-band activity in auditory regions and alpha-band activity in visual regions, while stimulus-bound PEs correlated with gamma-band activity in posterior regions. Crucially, task relevance modulated these spectral correlates, suggesting that current goals influence PE and prediction signalling.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Behavior/physiology , Brain Mapping , Neural Pathways/physiology , Reaction Time , Acoustic Stimulation , Adolescent , Adult , Alpha Rhythm/physiology , Bayes Theorem , Brain Mapping/methods , Cues , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Female , Humans , Magnetoencephalography , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
20.
HERD ; 10(4): 74-86, 2017 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28643564

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: This study is to examine how viewing a Japanese garden affects Japanese patients with dementia. BACKGROUND: In a previous study, authors explored the effect on individuals with Alzheimer's disease of viewing an indoor Japanese garden at a nursing home in the United States and reported that viewing the garden significantly reduced the heart rate, evoked short-term and long-term memories, and improved behavioral symptoms. However, it was unclear whether these effects were caused by the design of Japanese garden or unfamiliarity of the design to Caucasians. METHODS: We constructed a Japanese garden on the rooftop of a hospital in Japan and assessed with a total of 25 subjects on the following categories: (1) eye movement, (2) heart rate, and (3) behavior under four different conditions: (a) open view of the site before construction of the Japanese garden (the control space), (b) open view of the Japanese garden, (c) view of the Japanese garden through closed door, and (d) view of Japanese garden through closed door with the chrysanthemum scent. Findings/Results: Viewers' eyes scanned larger area while viewing the Japanese garden, and viewing the Japanese garden significantly reduced heart rate and improved behavioral symptoms than the control space. We also found that the effect of viewing the same Japanese garden differed across three conditions: the view through an open door, a closed door, and a closed door with added scent.


Subject(s)
Dementia/physiopathology , Gardens , Horticultural Therapy/psychology , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Behavior/physiology , Cognitive Dysfunction/physiopathology , Eye Movements/physiology , Female , Heart Rate/physiology , Horticultural Therapy/methods , Hospital Design and Construction , Hospitals , Humans , Japan , Male , Mental Status and Dementia Tests
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL