Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 632
Filter
Add more filters

Complementary Medicines
Publication year range
1.
PLoS One ; 16(12): e0260859, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34874970

ABSTRACT

Musicians tend to have better auditory and motor performance than non-musicians because of their extensive musical experience. In a previous study, we established that loudness discrimination acuity is enhanced when sound is produced by a precise force generation task. In this study, we compared the enhancement effect between experienced pianists and non-musicians. Without the force generation task, loudness discrimination acuity was better in pianists than non-musicians in the condition. However, the force generation task enhanced loudness discrimination acuity similarly in both pianists and non-musicians. The reaction time was also reduced with the force control task, but only in the non-musician group. The results suggest that the enhancement of loudness discrimination acuity with the precise force generation task is independent of musical experience and is, therefore, a fundamental function in auditory-motor interaction.


Subject(s)
Acoustic Stimulation , Cognition/physiology , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Loudness Perception , Music/psychology , Psychomotor Performance , Sound , Adult , Humans , Reaction Time , Young Adult
2.
PLoS One ; 16(12): e0261001, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34941913

ABSTRACT

Loudness judgments of sounds varying in level across time show a non-uniform temporal weighting, with increased weights assigned to the beginning of the sound (primacy effect). In addition, higher weights are observed for temporal components that are higher in level than the remaining components (loudness dominance). In three experiments, sounds consisting of 100- or 475-ms Gaussian wideband noise segments with random level variations were presented and either none, the first, or a central temporal segment was amplified or attenuated. In Experiment 1, the sounds consisted of four 100-ms segments that were separated by 500-ms gaps. Previous experiments did not show a primacy effect in such a condition. In Experiment 2, four- or ten-100-ms-segment sounds without gaps between the segments were presented to examine the interaction between the primacy effect and level dominance. As expected, for the sounds with segments separated by gaps, no primacy effect was observed, but weights on amplified segments were increased and weights on attenuated segments were decreased. For the sounds with contiguous segments, a primacy effect as well as effects of relative level (similar to those in Experiment 1) were found. For attenuation, the data indicated no substantial interaction between the primacy effect and loudness dominance, whereas for amplification an interaction was present. In Experiment 3, sounds consisting of either four contiguous 100-ms or 475-ms segments, or four 100-ms segments separated by 500-ms gaps were presented. Effects of relative level were more pronounced for the contiguous sounds. Across all three experiments, the effects of relative level were more pronounced for attenuation. In addition, the effects of relative level showed a dependence on the position of the change in level, with opposite direction for attenuation compared to amplification. Some of the results are in accordance with explanations based on masking effects on auditory intensity resolution.


Subject(s)
Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Loudness Perception , Psychoacoustics , Adult , Female , Humans , Judgment , Male , Noise , Sound , Young Adult
3.
Hear Res ; 391: 107969, 2020 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32320925

ABSTRACT

Speech recognition in noisy environments remains a challenge for cochlear implant (CI) recipients. Unwanted charge interactions between current pulses, both within and between electrode channels, are likely to impair performance. Here we investigate the effect of reducing the number of current pulses on speech perception. This was achieved by implementing a psychoacoustic temporal-masking model where current pulses in each channel were passed through a temporal integrator to identify and remove pulses that were less likely to be perceived by the recipient. The decision criterion of the temporal integrator was varied to control the percentage of pulses removed in each condition. In experiment 1, speech in quiet was processed with a standard Continuous Interleaved Sampling (CIS) strategy and with 25, 50 and 75% of pulses removed. In experiment 2, performance was measured for speech in noise with the CIS reference and with 50 and 75% of pulses removed. Speech intelligibility in quiet revealed no significant difference between reference and test conditions. For speech in noise, results showed a significant improvement of 2.4 dB when removing 50% of pulses and performance was not significantly different between the reference and when 75% of pulses were removed. Further, by reducing the overall amount of current pulses by 25, 50, and 75% but accounting for the increase in charge necessary to compensate for the decrease in loudness, estimated average power savings of 21.15, 40.95, and 63.45%, respectively, could be possible for this set of listeners. In conclusion, removing temporally masked pulses may improve speech perception in noise and result in substantial power savings.


Subject(s)
Cochlear Implantation/instrumentation , Cochlear Implants , Hearing Loss/therapy , Noise/adverse effects , Perceptual Masking , Persons With Hearing Impairments/rehabilitation , Speech Perception , Acoustic Stimulation , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Electric Stimulation , Hearing , Hearing Loss/diagnosis , Hearing Loss/physiopathology , Hearing Loss/psychology , Humans , Loudness Perception , Male , Middle Aged , Persons With Hearing Impairments/psychology , Speech Intelligibility
4.
Hear Res ; 390: 107949, 2020 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32200300

ABSTRACT

Phantom electrode (PE) stimulation can extend the lower limit of pitch perception with cochlear implants (CIs) by using simultaneous out-of-phase stimulation of the most apical primary electrode and the adjacent basal compensating electrode. The total electrical field may push the excitation pattern beyond the most apical electrode to elicit a lower pitch, depending on the ratio of current between the compensating and primary electrodes (i.e., the compensation coefficient σ). This study tested the hypothesis that dynamic current steering of PE stimuli can be implemented by varying σ over time to encode spectral details in low frequencies. To determine the range of σ for current steering and the corresponding current levels, Experiment 1 tested CI users' loudness balance and pitch ranking of static PE stimuli with σ from 0 to 0.6 in steps of 0.2. It was found that the equal-loudness most comfortable level significantly increased with σ and can be modeled by a piecewise linear function of σ. Consistent with the previous findings, higher σ elicited either lower or similar pitches without salient pitch reversals than lower σ. Based on the results of Experiment 1, Experiment 2 created flat, rising, and falling pitch contours of 300-1000 ms using dynamic PE stimuli with time-varying σ from 0 to 0.6 and equal-loudness current levels. In a pitch contour identification (PCI) task, CI users scored 80% and above on average. Increasing the stimulus duration from 300 to 1000 ms slightly but did not significantly improve the PCI scores. Across subjects, the 1000-ms PCI scores in Experiment 2 were significantly correlated with the cumulative pitch-ranking sensitivity in Experiment 1. It is thus feasible to use dynamic current steering with PE to encode low-frequency pitch cues for CI users.


Subject(s)
Cochlear Implantation/instrumentation , Cochlear Implants , Cues , Deafness/rehabilitation , Persons With Hearing Impairments/rehabilitation , Pitch Perception , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Aged , Deafness/diagnosis , Deafness/physiopathology , Deafness/psychology , Electric Stimulation , Female , Hearing , Humans , Loudness Perception , Male , Middle Aged , Persons With Hearing Impairments/psychology , Pitch Discrimination
5.
Hear Res ; 389: 107921, 2020 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32097828

ABSTRACT

The goal of the present study was to compare forward masking patterns by stimulation of low and high rates in cochlear implant users. Postlingually deafened Cochlear Nucleus® device users participated in the study. In experiment 1, two maskers of different rates (250 and 1000 pulses per second) were set at levels that produced equal masking for a probe presented at the same electrode as the maskers. This aligned the two masking functions at the on-site probe location. Then their forward masking patterns for the far probes were compared. Results showed that slope of the masked probe-threshold decay as a function of probe-masker separation was steeper for the high-rate than the low-rate masker. A linear model indicated that this difference in spread of neural excitation (SOE) was accounted for by two factors that were not correlated with each other. One factor was that the low-rate masker required a considerably higher current level to be equally effective in masking as the high-rate masker. The second factor was the effect of stimulation rate on loudness, i.e., integration of multiple pulses. This was consistent with our hypothesis that if an increase in stimulation rate does not result in an increased total neural response, then it is unlikely that the change in rate would change spatial distribution of the neural activity. Interestingly, the difference in masking effectiveness of the maskers predicted subjects' speech recognition. Poorer performers were those who showed more comparable masking effects by maskers of different rates. The difference in the masking effectiveness may indirectly measure the auditory neurons' excitability, which predicts speech recognition. In experiment 2, SOE of the high-rate and low-rate maskers were compared at a level that is clinically relevant, i.e., equal loudness. At equal loudness, high-rate stimulation not only produced an overall greater amount of forward masking, but also a shallower decay of masking with probe-masker separation (wider SOE), compared to low rate. The difference in SOE was the opposite to the findings from experiment 1. Whether the maskers were calibrated for equal masking or loudness, the absolute current level was always higher for the low-rate masker, which suggests that the SOE patterns cannot be explained by current spread alone. The fact that high-rate stimulation produced greater masking and wider SOE at equal loudness may explain why using high stimulation rates has not produced consistent benefits for speech recognition, and why lowering stimulation rate from the manufacturer's default sometimes results in improved speech recognition for subjects.


Subject(s)
Cochlea/physiopathology , Cochlear Implantation/instrumentation , Cochlear Implants , Deafness/rehabilitation , Perceptual Masking , Persons With Hearing Impairments/rehabilitation , Speech Perception , Acoustic Stimulation , Aged , Deafness/diagnosis , Deafness/physiopathology , Deafness/psychology , Electric Stimulation , Female , Hearing , Humans , Loudness Perception , Male , Middle Aged , Noise/adverse effects , Persons With Hearing Impairments/psychology , Recognition, Psychology , Speech Intelligibility
6.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 147(1): 108, 2020 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32006968

ABSTRACT

This paper investigates multidimensional characteristics of tonal noise from heating, ventilation, and air-conditioning systems, besides loudness and tonality, to improve prediction of annoyance. Two studies were conducted: multidimensional scaling (MDS) analysis to determine what other perceptual signal characteristics are important and perceptual weight analysis (PWA) to understand the impact of multiple tones in a signal. In the MDS study, paired comparison tasks were conducted to gather similarity and annoyance data. Results show that the latent perceptual dimensions are related to the signal's tonality, loudness, sharpness, and roughness. Including metrics for these perceptions, except roughness, improves the performance of earlier annoyance prediction models. Including both sharpness and tonal audibility does not further improve prediction performance, though. In the PWA study, noise stimuli with five-tone complexes between 125 Hz and 2 kHz were generated for subjective testing to obtain a perceptual weighting function. The levels of each tone were randomly adjusted for every trial, and both harmonic and inharmonic tone complexes were utilized. The PWA result was applied as a spectral weighting function to calculate a proposed weighted-sum tonal audibility metric. Utilizing the proposed metric instead of the traditional tonal audibility metric improves annoyance prediction to a similar degree as including sharpness.


Subject(s)
Acoustics , Auditory Perception , Noise , Psychoacoustics , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Female , Humans , Loudness Perception , Male , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted , Young Adult
7.
Hear Res ; 389: 107908, 2020 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32062293

ABSTRACT

Hyperacusis is a debilitating hearing condition in which normal everyday sounds are perceived as exceedingly loud, annoying, aversive or even painful. The prevalence of hyperacusis approaches 10%, making it an important, but understudied medical condition. To noninvasively identify the neural correlates of hyperacusis in an animal model, we used sound-evoked functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to locate regions of abnormal activity in the central nervous system of rats with behavioral evidence of hyperacusis induced with an ototoxic drug (sodium salicylate, 250 mg/kg, i.p.). Reaction time-intensity measures of loudness-growth revealed behavioral evidence of salicylate-induced hyperacusis at high intensities. fMRI revealed significantly enhanced sound-evoked responses in the auditory cortex (AC) to 80 dB SPL tone bursts presented at 8 and 16 kHz. Sound-evoked responses in the inferior colliculus (IC) were also enhanced, but to a lesser extent. To confirm the main results, electrophysiological recordings of spike discharges from multi-unit clusters were obtained from the central auditory pathway. Salicylate significantly enhanced tone-evoked spike-discharges from multi-unit clusters in the AC from 4 to 30 kHz at intensities ≥60 dB SPL; less enhancement occurred in the medial geniculate body (MGB), and even less in the IC. Our results demonstrate for the first time that non-invasive sound-evoked fMRI can be used to identify regions of neural hyperactivity throughout the brain in an animal model of hyperacusis.


Subject(s)
Auditory Pathways/diagnostic imaging , Behavior, Animal , Brain Mapping , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Hyperacusis/diagnostic imaging , Loudness Perception , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Acoustic Stimulation , Animals , Auditory Pathways/physiopathology , Brain/physiopathology , Disease Models, Animal , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials, Auditory , Hyperacusis/physiopathology , Hyperacusis/psychology , Male , Predictive Value of Tests , Rats, Sprague-Dawley , Reaction Time , Time Factors
8.
Ear Hear ; 41(5): 1187-1195, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31985534

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) is a brain imaging technique particularly suitable for hearing studies. However, the nature of fNIRS responses to auditory stimuli presented at different stimulus intensities is not well understood. In this study, we investigated whether fNIRS response amplitude was better predicted by stimulus properties (intensity) or individually perceived attributes (loudness). DESIGN: Twenty-two young adults were included in this experimental study. Four different stimulus intensities of a broadband noise were used as stimuli. First, loudness estimates for each stimulus intensity were measured for each participant. Then, the 4 stimulation intensities were presented in counterbalanced order while recording hemoglobin saturation changes from cortical auditory brain areas. The fNIRS response was analyzed in a general linear model design, using 3 different regressors: a non-modulated, an intensity-modulated, and a loudness-modulated regressor. RESULTS: Higher intensity stimuli resulted in higher amplitude fNIRS responses. The relationship between stimulus intensity and fNIRS response amplitude was better explained using a regressor based on individually estimated loudness estimates compared with a regressor modulated by stimulus intensity alone. CONCLUSIONS: Brain activation in response to different stimulus intensities is more reliant upon individual loudness sensation than physical stimulus properties. Therefore, in measurements using different auditory stimulus intensities or subjective hearing parameters, loudness estimates should be examined when interpreting results.


Subject(s)
Auditory Cortex , Spectroscopy, Near-Infrared , Acoustic Stimulation , Brain , Hearing , Humans , Loudness Perception , Sound , Young Adult
9.
Trends Hear ; 24: 2331216519900595, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31994456

ABSTRACT

Loudness context effects comprise differences in judgments of the loudness of a target stimulus depending on the presence of a preceding inducer tone. Interstimulus intervals (ISIs) between inducer tone and target tone of about 200 ms and above cause an induced loudness reduction (ILR) of the target tone. As the ILR increases, respectively, the perceived loudness of the target stimuli decreases with increasing ISI. This in turn means that identical stimuli in a different context have a differently perceived loudness. A correlation between specific characteristics in the electroencephalography responses and perceived loudness in an ILR experiment would therefore provide a neurophysiological indication of loudness processing beyond a neural representation of stimulus intensity only. To examine if such a correlation exists, we investigated cortical electroencephalography responses in a latency range from 75 to 510 ms during a psychoacoustical ILR experiment with different ISIs. With increasing ISI, the strength of the N1-P2 deflection of the respective electroencephalography response decreases similarly to the loudness perception of the target tone pulse. This indicates a representation based on loudness rather than on intensity at the corresponding processing stage.


Subject(s)
Evoked Potentials , Loudness Perception , Acoustic Stimulation , Electroencephalography , Humans , Judgment
10.
PLoS One ; 14(11): e0223075, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31689327

ABSTRACT

Previous research has consistently shown that for sounds varying in intensity over time, the beginning of the sound is of higher importance for the perception of loudness than later parts (primacy effect). However, in all previous studies, the target sounds were presented in quiet, and at a fixed average sound level. In the present study, temporal loudness weights for a time-varying narrowband noise were investigated in the presence of a continuous bandpass-filtered background noise and the average sound levels of the target stimuli were varied across a range of 60 dB. Pronounced primacy effects were observed in all conditions and there were no significant differences between the temporal weights observed in the conditions in quiet and in background noise. Within the conditions in background noise, there was a significant effect of the sound level on the pattern of weights, which was mainly caused by a slight trend for increased weights at the end of the sounds ("recency effect") in the condition with lower average level. No such effect was observed for the in-quiet conditions. Taken together, the observed primacy effect is largely independent of masking as well as of sound level. Compatible with this conclusion, the observed primacy effects in quiet and in background noise can be well described by an exponential decay function using parameters based on previous studies. Simulations using a model for the partial loudness of time-varying sounds in background noise showed that the model does not predict the observed temporal loudness weights.


Subject(s)
Loudness Perception/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adolescent , Adult , Auditory Threshold/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Psychological , Noise , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Psychoacoustics , Sound , Time Factors , Young Adult
11.
Hear Res ; 381: 107776, 2019 09 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31401433

ABSTRACT

Chronic tinnitus, a symptom of high prevalence, is a persistent hearing sensation in the absence of an external sound source. Recent electrophysiological studies indicate that tinnitus generation is to a high degree the result of maladaptive plasticity in the central auditory pathway. The pitch of the tinnitus sensation can be assessed by performing a pitch matching procedure. In the most frequent "tonal tinnitus" type pure tones are used as test stimuli. However, in the case of tonal tinnitus not a single malfunctioning neuron, but rather a population of neighbouring neurons is involved in the generation process of tinnitus and patients typically perceive their tinnitus as a sound having a prominent centre frequency with some spectral extent. Thus, the question arises, why not to use narrow band noise (NBN) instead of pure tones as test stimuli in pitch matching procedures? To investigate this, we first evaluated the pitch matching performance of healthy subjects. In a recursive two alternative choice testing, driven by a computer based automated procedure, the subjects were asked to match the pitch of two sounds. In a crosswise design, NBNs and pure tones were used both as target and as test stimuli. We were able to show that across all four possible combinations the pitch matching performance was least favourable when a sinusoidal sound had to be matched to an NBN target. Even though matching two sinusoidal sounds results in the lowest error, considering that the tinnitus percept typically includes some spectral extent, an NBN should be preferably used as a test stimulus against a pure tone.


Subject(s)
Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Audiometry, Pure-Tone , Auditory Pathways/physiopathology , Hearing , Pitch Perception , Tinnitus/diagnosis , Adult , Female , Humans , Loudness Perception , Male , Middle Aged , Predictive Value of Tests , Tinnitus/physiopathology , Tinnitus/psychology , Young Adult
12.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 145(5): 3208, 2019 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31153337

ABSTRACT

The aim of this study is to explore the performance of binaural and monaural recordings in soundscape evaluation. Twelve sites with different acoustic scenarios were chosen, where binaural and monaural recordings were simultaneously made. Nine soundscape indicators were assessed by residents through a laboratory-based auditory test. The results showed that the two recording methods present good agreement on most soundscape evaluation indicators including overall impression, acoustic comfort, pleasantness, annoyance, eventfulness, and loudness. The two recording methods were found to be correlated with different indicators in a similar way. For most sites, the two recording methods were significantly correlated excluding for directionality. For both recording methods, the A-weighted sound pressure level was found to have a weak impact on soundscape evaluation. Reverberation time significantly affects reverberance through binaural recordings. Overall, for most soundscape indicators, it is feasible to use both recording methods, although when "realism," "reverberance," and "directivity" are involved in evaluation, binaural recordings will render corresponding perception more consistently than the monaural.


Subject(s)
Auditory Threshold/physiology , Loudness Perception/physiology , Sound , Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Acoustics , Hearing Tests/methods , Humans
13.
PLoS One ; 14(5): e0216499, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31051008

ABSTRACT

Learning, attention and action play a crucial role in determining how stimulus predictions are formed, stored, and updated. Years-long experience with the specific repertoires of sounds of one or more musical styles is what characterizes professional musicians. Here we contrasted active experience with sounds, namely long-lasting motor practice, theoretical study and engaged listening to the acoustic features characterizing a musical style of choice in professional musicians with mainly passive experience of sounds in laypersons. We hypothesized that long-term active experience of sounds would influence the neural predictions of the stylistic features in professional musicians in a distinct way from the mainly passive experience of sounds in laypersons. Participants with different musical backgrounds were recruited: professional jazz and classical musicians, amateur musicians and non-musicians. They were presented with a musical multi-feature paradigm eliciting mismatch negativity (MMN), a prediction error signal to changes in six sound features for only 12 minutes of electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography (MEG) recordings. We observed a generally larger MMN amplitudes-indicative of stronger automatic neural signals to violated priors-in jazz musicians (but not in classical musicians) as compared to non-musicians and amateurs. The specific MMN enhancements were found for spectral features (timbre, pitch, slide) and sound intensity. In participants who were not musicians, the higher preference for jazz music was associated with reduced MMN to pitch slide (a feature common in jazz music style). Our results suggest that long-lasting, active experience of a musical style is associated with accurate neural priors for the sound features of the preferred style, in contrast to passive listening.


Subject(s)
Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Loudness Perception/physiology , Pitch Perception/physiology , Adult , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Magnetoencephalography , Male , Music , Young Adult
14.
PLoS One ; 14(2): e0212940, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30817808

ABSTRACT

Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) is a non-invasive brain imaging technique that measures changes in oxygenated and de-oxygenated hemoglobin concentration and can provide a measure of brain activity. In addition to neural activity, fNIRS signals contain components that can be used to extract physiological information such as cardiac measures. Previous studies have shown changes in cardiac activity in response to different sounds. This study investigated whether cardiac responses collected using fNIRS differ for different loudness of sounds. fNIRS data were collected from 28 normal hearing participants. Cardiac response measures evoked by broadband, amplitude-modulated sounds were extracted for four sound intensities ranging from near-threshold to comfortably loud levels (15, 40, 65 and 90 dB Sound Pressure Level (SPL)). Following onset of the noise stimulus, heart rate initially decreased for sounds of 15 and 40 dB SPL, reaching a significantly lower rate at 15 dB SPL. For sounds at 65 and 90 dB SPL, increases in heart rate were seen. To quantify the timing of significant changes, inter-beat intervals were assessed. For sounds at 40 dB SPL, an immediate significant change in the first two inter-beat intervals following sound onset was found. At other levels, the most significant change appeared later (beats 3 to 5 following sound onset). In conclusion, changes in heart rate were associated with the level of sound with a clear difference in response to near-threshold sounds compared to comfortably loud sounds. These findings may be used alone or in conjunction with other measures such as fNIRS brain activity for evaluation of hearing ability.


Subject(s)
Hearing/physiology , Heart Rate/physiology , Loudness Perception/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Auditory Threshold/physiology , Brain/physiology , Female , Functional Neuroimaging , Heart Sounds/physiology , Humans , Male , Spectroscopy, Near-Infrared , Young Adult
15.
Hear Res ; 377: 24-33, 2019 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30884368

ABSTRACT

Cochlear implant users require fitting of electrical threshold and comfort levels for optimal access to sound. In this study, we used single-channel cortical auditory evoked responses (CAEPs) obtained from 20 participants using a Nucleus device. A fully objective method to estimate threshold levels was developed, using growth function fitting and the peak phase-locking value feature. Results demonstrated that growth function fitting is a viable method for estimating threshold levels in cochlear implant users, with a strong correlation (r = 0.979, p < 0.001) with behavioral thresholds. Additionally, we compared the threshold estimates using CAEPs acquired from a standard montage (Cz to mastoid) against using a montage of recording channels near the cochlear implant, simulating recording from the device itself. The correlation between estimated and behavioural thresholds remained strong (r = 0.966, p < 0.001), however the recording time needed to be increased to produce a similar estimate accuracy. Finally, a method for estimating comfort levels was investigated, and showed that the comfort level estimates were mildly correlated with behavioral comfort levels (r = 0.50, p = 0.024).


Subject(s)
Auditory Threshold , Cochlear Implantation/instrumentation , Cochlear Implants , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials, Auditory , Loudness Perception , Persons With Hearing Impairments/rehabilitation , Prosthesis Fitting , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Electric Stimulation , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Persons With Hearing Impairments/psychology , Predictive Value of Tests , Prosthesis Design , Treatment Outcome
16.
Neuroscience ; 407: 93-107, 2019 05 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30292765

ABSTRACT

The central gain model of hyperacusis proposes that loss of auditory input can result in maladaptive neuronal gain increases in the central auditory system, leading to the over-amplification of sound-evoked activity and excessive loudness perception. Despite the attractiveness of this model, and supporting evidence for it, a critical test of the central gain theory requires that changes in sound-evoked activity be explicitly linked to perceptual alterations of loudness. Here we combined an operant conditioning task that uses a subject's reaction time to auditory stimuli to produce reliable measures of loudness growth with chronic electrophysiological recordings from the auditory cortex and inferior colliculus of awake, behaviorally-phenotyped animals. In this manner, we could directly correlate daily assessments of loudness perception with neurophysiological measures of sound encoding within the same animal. We validated this novel psychophysical-electrophysiological paradigm with a salicylate-induced model of hearing loss and hyperacusis, as high doses of sodium salicylate reliably induce temporary hearing loss, neural hyperactivity, and auditory perceptual disruptions like tinnitus and hyperacusis. Salicylate induced parallel changes to loudness growth and evoked response-intensity functions consistent with temporary hearing loss and hyperacusis. Most importantly, we found that salicylate-mediated changes in loudness growth and sound-evoked activity were correlated within individual animals. These results provide strong support for the central gain model of hyperacusis and demonstrate the utility of using an experimental design that allows for within-subject comparison of behavioral and electrophysiological measures, thereby making inter-subject variability a strength rather than a limitation.


Subject(s)
Hearing Loss/physiopathology , Hyperacusis/physiopathology , Loudness Perception/physiology , Sodium Salicylate/pharmacology , Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Animals , Auditory Cortex/drug effects , Auditory Cortex/physiopathology , Evoked Potentials, Auditory/drug effects , Female , Hearing/drug effects , Hearing/physiology , Hearing Loss/drug therapy , Inferior Colliculi/drug effects , Inferior Colliculi/physiopathology , Loudness Perception/drug effects , Male , Rats, Sprague-Dawley , Rodentia
17.
Audiol., Commun. res ; 24: e2156, 2019. tab, graf
Article in Portuguese | LILACS | ID: biblio-1038756

ABSTRACT

RESUMO Objetivo Verificar a associação entre musicalização infantil e habilidades auditivas de ordenação temporal e localização sonora em pré-escolares de 5 e 6 anos. Métodos Participaram do estudo 60 crianças de ambos os sexos, de 5 anos a 6 anos e 11 meses, sendo 30 do grupo com treinamento musical e 30 do grupo sem treinamento musical. Os participantes de ambos os grupos foram submetidos à triagem auditiva, avaliação simplificada do processamento auditivo e ao teste Padrão de Frequência, em campo livre. Os desempenhos de cada um dos procedimentos foram tabulados, analisando-se as possíveis correlações e associações entre eles, como variáveis dependentes e variáveis independentes, como grupo, sexo e idade. Resultados O grupo com treinamento musical apresentou média de acertos superior ao grupo sem treinamento musical, nos testes de memória sequencial não verbal e verbal, teste Padrão de Frequência não verbal e verbal. Sujeitos de 5 anos do grupo com treinamento musical obtiveram melhor desempenho, em relação aos sujeitos de 5 anos do grupo sem treinamento musical, acertando mais sequências. No teste de localização sonora, não houve diferença entre a idade e o grupo. Conclusão Pré-escolares de 5 e 6 anos que participavam de musicalização infantil apresentaram melhor desempenho nos testes que avaliaram as habilidades de memória sequencial não verbal e verbal e de ordenação temporal de três sons, quando comparados aos pré-escolares que não participavam de musicalização. Portanto, a musicalização infantil influenciou positivamente as habilidades auditivas de pré-escolares de 5 e 6 anos.


ABSTRACT Purpose Verify the association between children's music learning and listening skills of temporal ordering and sound localization in preschoolers from 5 to 6 years old. Methods 60 children of both genders, from 5 to 6 years and 11 months, participated in the study, 30 of the group with music training and the other 30 of the group with no music training. The participants of both groups were submitted to hearing screening, simplified auditory processing assessment and to the Pitch Pattern Sequence. The performance of each of the procedures was tabulated, being analyzed the possible correlations and associations between them, as, for example, dependent and independent variables such as group, gender and age. Results The group with music training presented higher mean of scores than the one with no music training in the verbal and nonverbal sequential memory tests and on the verbal and nonverbal Pitch Pattern Sequence. The 5-year-old children of the group with music training got better results than the 5-year-old ones with no music training, getting right in more sequences. In the sound localization test, there was no difference between ages and groups. Conclusion Preschoolers from 5 to 6 years old who participated in children's music learning presented better performance in the tests that evaluate the abilities of the verbal and non-verbal sequential memory and of the temporal ordering of three sounds when compared to the preschoolers who did not participate in the music learning. Therefore, the children's music learning positively influenced the listening skills of preschoolers from 5 to 6 years old.


Subject(s)
Humans , Child, Preschool , Child , Auditory Perception , Triage , Hearing Tests , Music/psychology , Auditory Cortex , Sound Localization , Acoustic Stimulation , Loudness Perception
18.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 144(5): 2751, 2018 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30522299

ABSTRACT

The symmetric biphasic pulses used in contemporary cochlear implants (CIs) consist of both cathodic and anodic currents, which may stimulate different sites on spiral ganglion neurons and, potentially, interact with each other. The effect on the order of anodic and cathodic stimulation on loudness at short inter-pulse intervals (IPIs; 0-800 µs) is investigated. Pairs of opposite-polarity pseudomonophasic (PS) pulses were used and the amplitude of each pulse was manipulated independently. In experiment 1 the two PS pulses differed in their current level in order to elicit the same loudness when presented separately. Six users of the Advanced Bionics CI (Valencia, CA) loudness-ranked trains of the pulse pairs using a midpoint-comparison procedure. Stimuli with anodic-leading polarity were louder than those with cathodic-leading polarity for IPIs shorter than 400 µs. This effect was small-about 0.3 dB-but consistent across listeners. When the same procedure was repeated with both PS pulses having the same current level (experiment 2), anodic-leading stimuli were still louder than cathodic-leading stimuli at very short intervals. However, when using symmetric biphasic pulses (experiment 3) the effect disappeared at short intervals and reversed at long intervals. Possible peripheral sources of such polarity interactions are discussed.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Cochlear Implants/adverse effects , Loudness Perception/physiology , Spiral Ganglion/physiopathology , Acoustic Stimulation , Aged , Cochlear Implantation/methods , Cochlear Implants/statistics & numerical data , Electric Stimulation/adverse effects , Electrodes, Implanted/standards , Humans , Middle Aged , Pitch Discrimination/physiology , Prosthesis Design , Spiral Ganglion/surgery
19.
Hear Res ; 370: 168-180, 2018 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30390569

ABSTRACT

This study examines temporal effects both at threshold and at supra-threshold levels. The level needed to detect a short-duration 4.0-kHz signal was measured for signals presented with different onset delays relative to a 300-ms broadband noise masker: 100 ms and 5 ms before the onset of the masker and 5 ms and 100 ms after the onset of the masker. Loudness matches between the signal in quiet and the signal at the same four onset delays were obtained for five presentation levels of the short-duration signal and for three masker levels. The temporal effect was defined as the level difference between the signals near masker onset and the signals well before or well after masker onset, needed to reach threshold and/or achieve equal loudness. Both at threshold and at supra-threshold levels temporal effects were observed consistent with a decrease in gain at the masker frequency during the course of the masker. The temporal effect was not restricted to simultaneous masking, but was also found for backward masking. In both cases the temporal effects were stronger at supra-threshold levels than at threshold. This may be caused by a transient effect at masker onset. The almost simultaneous onset of the signal and the masker makes it difficult for subjects to separate signal from the masker, especially when the signal level is close to masked threshold.


Subject(s)
Auditory Threshold , Loudness Perception , Noise/adverse effects , Perceptual Masking , Acoustic Stimulation , Humans , Psychoacoustics , Signal Detection, Psychological , Time Factors
20.
Trends Hear ; 22: 2331216518807535, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30378468

ABSTRACT

Cochlear-implant users who have experienced both analog and pulsatile sound coding strategies often have strong preferences for the sound quality of one over the other. This suggests that analog and pulsatile stimulation may provide different information or sound quality to an implant listener. It has been well documented that many implant listeners both prefer and perform better with multichannel analog than multichannel pulsatile strategies, although the reasons for these differences remain unknown. Here, we examine the perceptual differences between analog and pulsatile stimulation on a single electrode. A multidimensional scaling task, analyzed across two dimensions, suggested that pulsatile stimulation was perceived to be considerably different from analog stimulation. Two associated tasks using single-dimensional scaling showed that analog stimulation was perceived to be less Clean on average than pulsatile stimulation and that the perceptual differences were not related to pitch. In a follow-up experiment, it was determined that the perceptual differences between analog and pulsatile stimulation were not dependent on the interpulse gap present in pulsatile stimulation. Although the results suggest that there is a large perceptual difference between analog and pulsatile stimulation, further work is needed to determine the nature of these differences.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception , Cochlear Implantation/instrumentation , Cochlear Implants , Deafness/rehabilitation , Persons With Hearing Impairments/rehabilitation , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Aged , Deafness/diagnosis , Deafness/physiopathology , Deafness/psychology , Electric Stimulation , Female , Hearing , Humans , Loudness Perception , Male , Middle Aged , Persons With Hearing Impairments/psychology , Pitch Perception , Prosthesis Design , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL