Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 7 de 7
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Schizophr Res ; 224: 141-150, 2020 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33268158

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Abnormalities in fronto-striatal-thalamic (FST) sub-circuits are present in schizophrenia and are associated with cognitive impairments. However, it remains unknown whether abnormalities in FST sub-circuits are present before psychosis onset. This may be elucidated by investigating 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11DS), a genetic syndrome associated with a 30% risk for developing schizophrenia in adulthood and a decline in Verbal IQ (VIQ) preceding psychosis onset. Here, we examined white matter (WM) tracts in FST sub-circuits, especially those in the dorsolateral (DLPFC) and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) sub-circuits, and their associations with VIQ in young adults with 22q11DS. METHODS: Diffusion MRI scans were acquired from 21 individuals with 22q11DS with prodromal symptoms of schizophrenia, 30 individuals with 22q11DS without prodromal symptoms, and 30 healthy controls (mean age: 21 ± 2 years). WM tracts were reconstructed between striatum and thalamus with rostral middle frontal gyrus (rMFG) and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), representing DLPFC and VLPFC respectively. Fractional anisotropy (FA) and radial diffusivity (RD) were used for group comparisons. VIQ was assessed and associations with the diffusion measures were evaluated. RESULTS: FA was significantly increased and RD decreased in most tracts of the DLPFC and VLPFC sub-circuits in 22q11DS. Verbal IQ scores correlated negatively with FA and, at trend level, positively with RD in the right thalamus-IFG tract in 22q11DS with prodromal symptoms. CONCLUSIONS: While abnormalities in FST sub-circuits are associated with schizophrenia, we observed that these abnormalities are also present in 22q11DS individuals with prodromal symptoms and are associated with verbal performance in the right thalamus-IFG tract.


Asunto(s)
Síndrome de DiGeorge , Sustancia Blanca , Adulto , Anisotropía , Imagen de Difusión Tensora , Humanos , Tálamo/diagnóstico por imagen , Sustancia Blanca/diagnóstico por imagen , Adulto Joven
2.
Hear Res ; 296: 131-40, 2013 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23168357

RESUMEN

We investigated gender differences in the identification of personally familiar voices in a gender-balanced sample of 40 listeners. From various types of utterances, listeners had to identify by name 20 speakers (10 female) among a set of 70 possible classmates who were all 12th grade pupils from the same local secondary school. Mean identification rates were 67% from sentences, and around 35% for an isolated /Hello/ or a VCV syllable. Even from non-verbal harrumphs, speakers were identified with an accuracy of 18%, i.e. highly above chance levels. Substantial individual differences were observed between listeners. Importantly, superior overall performance of female listeners was qualified by an interaction between voice gender and listener gender. Male listeners exhibited an own-gender bias (i.e. better identification for male than female voices), whereas female listeners identified voices of both genders at similar levels. Individual own-gender identification biases were correlated with differences in reported contact to a speaker's voice and voice distinctiveness. Overall, the present study establishes a number of factors that account for substantial individual differences in personal voice identification.


Asunto(s)
Fonética , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Acústica del Lenguaje , Percepción del Habla , Calidad de la Voz , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Audiometría del Habla , Discriminación en Psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Factores Sexuales , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
3.
Hear Res ; 282(1-2): 283-8, 2011 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21771649

RESUMEN

High-level adaptation not only biases the perception of faces, but also causes transient distortions in auditory perception of non-linguistic voice information about gender, identity, and emotional intonation. Here we report a novel auditory aftereffect in perceiving vocal age: age estimates were elevated in age-morphed test voices when preceded by adaptor voices of young speakers (∼20 yrs), compared to old adaptor voices (∼70 yrs). This vocal age aftereffect (VAAE) complements a recently reported face aftereffect (Schweinberger et al., 2010) and points to selective neuronal coding of vocal age. Intriguingly, post-adaptation assessment revealed that VAAEs could persist for minutes after adaptation, although reduced in magnitude. As an important qualification, VAAEs during post-adaptation were modulated by gender congruency between speaker and listener. For both male and female listeners, VAAEs were much reduced for test voices of opposite gender. Overall, this study establishes a new auditory aftereffect in the perception of vocal age. We offer a tentative sociobiological explanation for the differential, gender-dependent recovery from vocal age adaptation.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Señales (Psicología) , Acústica del Lenguaje , Percepción del Habla , Calidad de la Voz , Estimulación Acústica , Adaptación Psicológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Envejecimiento/psicología , Análisis de Varianza , Audiometría del Habla , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Psicoacústica , Factores Sexuales , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
4.
Hear Res ; 268(1-2): 38-45, 2010 Sep 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20430084

RESUMEN

While adaptation to complex auditory stimuli has traditionally been reported for linguistic properties of speech, the present study demonstrates non-linguistic high-level aftereffects in the perception of voice identity, following adaptation to voices or faces of personally familiar speakers. In Exp. 1, prolonged exposure to speaker A's voice biased the perception of identity-ambiguous voice morphs between speakers A and B towards speaker B (and vice versa). Significantly biased voice identity perception was also observed in Exp. 2 when adaptors were videos of speakers' silently articulating faces, although effects were reduced in magnitude relative to those seen in Exp. 1. By contrast, adaptation to an unrelated speaker C elicited an intermediate proportion of speaker A identifications in both experiments. While crossmodal aftereffects on auditory identification (Exp. 2) dissipated rapidly, unimodal aftereffects (Exp. 1) were still measurable a few minutes after adaptation. These novel findings suggest contrastive coding of voice identity in long-term memory, with at least two perceptual mechanisms of voice identity adaptation: one related to auditory coding of voice characteristics, and another related to multimodal coding of familiar speaker identity.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento en Psicología , Acústica del Lenguaje , Percepción del Habla , Voz , Estimulación Acústica , Adaptación Psicológica , Adulto , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria , Detección de Señal Psicológica , Factores de Tiempo , Grabación en Video , Adulto Joven
5.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 22(10): 2345-56, 2010 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19702459

RESUMEN

The perception of facial gender has been found to be adaptively recalibrated: adaptation to male faces causes participants to perceive subsequent faces as more feminine and vice versa [Webster, M. A., Kaping, D., Mizokami, Y., & Duhamel, P. Adaptation to natural facial categories. Nature, 428, 557-561, 2004]. In an event-related brain potential (ERP) study, Kovács et al. [Kovács, G., Zimmer, M., Banko, E., Harza, I., Antal, A., & Vidnyanszky, Z. Electrophysiological correlates of visual adaptation to faces and body parts in humans. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 742-753, 2006] reported reduced N170 amplitudes and increased latencies for test faces following female gender adaptation compared to control stimulus (a phase randomized face) adaptation. We examined whether this N170 attenuation to test faces was related to the adaptor's gender, or to adaptation to face exposure in general. We compared N170 effects after adaptation to either male or androgynous faces. Additionally, we investigated cross-modal adaptation for the same test faces following male or androgynous voice adaptors. Visual adaptation to face gender replicated previously reported aftereffects in classifying androgynous faces, and a similar trend was observed following adaptation to voice gender. Strikingly, N170 amplitudes were dramatically reduced for faces following face adaptors (relative to those following voice adaptors), whereas only minimal gender-specific adaptation effects were seen in the N170. By contrast, strong gender-specific adaptation effects appeared in a centroparietal P3-like component (approximately 400-600 msec), which in the context of adaptation may reflect a neural correlate of the detection of perceptual novelty.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/genética , Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Cara , Caracteres Sexuales , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
6.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 21(4): 625-41, 2009 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18702593

RESUMEN

We used ERPs to investigate neural correlates of face learning. At learning, participants viewed video clips of unfamiliar people, which were presented either with or without voices providing semantic information. In a subsequent face-recognition task (four trial blocks), learned faces were repeated once per block and presented interspersed with novel faces. To disentangle face from image learning, we used different images for face repetitions. Block effects demonstrated that engaging in the face-recognition task modulated ERPs between 170 and 900 msec poststimulus onset for learned and novel faces. In addition, multiple repetitions of different exemplars of learned faces elicited an increased bilateral N250. Source localizations of this N250 for learned faces suggested activity in fusiform gyrus, similar to that found previously for N250r in repetition priming paradigms [Schweinberger, S. R., Pickering, E. C., Jentzsch, I., Burton, A. M., & Kaufmann, J. M. Event-related brain potential evidence for a response of inferior temporal cortex to familiar face repetitions. Cognitive Brain Research, 14, 398-409, 2002]. Multiple repetitions of learned faces also elicited increased central-parietal positivity between 400 and 600 msec and caused a bilateral increase of inferior-temporal negativity (>300 msec) compared with novel faces. Semantic information at learning enhanced recognition rates. Faces that had been learned with semantic information elicited somewhat less negative amplitudes between 700 and 900 msec over left inferior-temporal sites. Overall, the findings demonstrate a role of the temporal N250 ERP in the acquisition of new face representations across different images. They also suggest that, compared with visual presentation alone, additional semantic information at learning facilitates postperceptual processing in recognition but does not facilitate perceptual analysis of learned faces.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Cara , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Semántica , Adulto Joven
7.
Neuropsychologia ; 47(1): 123-31, 2009 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18765243

RESUMEN

Phonagnosia, the inability to recognize familiar voices, has been studied in brain-damaged patients but no cases due to developmental problems have been reported. Here we describe the case of KH, a 60-year-old active professional woman who reports that she has always experienced severe voice recognition difficulties. Her hearing abilities are normal, and an MRI scan showed no evidence of brain damage in regions associated with voice or auditory perception. To better understand her condition and to assess models of voice and high-level auditory processing, we tested KH on behavioural tasks measuring voice recognition, recognition of vocal emotions, face recognition, speech perception, and processing of environmental sounds and music. KH was impaired on tasks requiring the recognition of famous voices and the learning and recognition of new voices. In contrast, she performed well on nearly all other tasks. Her case is the first report of developmental phonagnosia, and the results suggest that the recognition of a speaker's vocal identity depends on separable mechanisms from those used to recognize other information from the voice or non-vocal auditory stimuli.


Asunto(s)
Agnosia/fisiopatología , Percepción Auditiva , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Voz , Estimulación Acústica , Agnosia/patología , Discriminación en Psicología , Progresión de la Enfermedad , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA