Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 10 de 10
Filtrar
1.
Psychol Res ; 88(2): 307-337, 2024 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37847268

RESUMEN

Accounting for how the human mind represents the internal and external world is a crucial feature of many theories of human cognition. Central to this question is the distinction between modal as opposed to amodal representational formats. It has often been assumed that one but not both of these two types of representations underlie processing in specific domains of cognition (e.g., perception, mental imagery, and language). However, in this paper, we suggest that both formats play a major role in most cognitive domains. We believe that a comprehensive theory of cognition requires a solid understanding of these representational formats and their functional roles within and across different domains of cognition, the developmental trajectory of these representational formats, and their role in dysfunctional behavior. Here we sketch such an overarching perspective that brings together research from diverse subdisciplines of psychology on modal and amodal representational formats so as to unravel their functional principles and their interactions.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Humanos
2.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 221: 103428, 2021 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34775274

RESUMEN

In the context of the embodied cognition debate, an effect of motion verb associated speed information has previously been detected using eye-tracking, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and reaction times (RT). The latter, for instance, was implemented by Wender and Weber (1982), who observed that participants were faster in detecting motion in sentences associated with fast motion compared to sentences associated with slow motion after having formed mental images of the sentences' content. It remains open whether the reported effects of speed are associated with automatic lexical-semantic retrieval processes or whether they reflect higher top-down cognitive processes. To answer this question, the paradigm by Wender and Weber (1982) was adopted and further elaborated in the present study. In Experiment 1 visualization instructions were eliminated. Additionally, the stimulus material was manipulated in regards to the agent of the described movement (human vs. object motion) in order to determine the representation's modality (visual vs. motoric). In Experiment 2, the task to detect motion was replaced by the task to judge sensicality. The results suggest that the prompt to perform mental imagery is not a precondition for the engagement of modal representations in this speed of motion paradigm and that the involved representations' modality is visual rather than motoric. However, the modal representations' involvement is dependent on the task. They thus do not seem to be part of the invariant semantic representation of manner of motion verbs.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje , Semántica , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Movimiento (Física) , Tiempo de Reacción
3.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 45(6): 969-979, 2019 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30160503

RESUMEN

When people judge the duration of stimuli, judgments are influenced by the physical size of these stimuli. Specifically, people tend to judge the duration of large stimuli longer than the duration of small stimuli. However, some authors (Bottini & Casasanto, 2010; Ma, Yang, & Zhang, 2012) have reported that even implicit size can affect duration estimates. The present study extends the results of these studies. Specifically, we examined whether the imagined size of objects would also influence duration estimates. In each trial, participants reproduced the duration of an animal word. During the presentation of the word, they were asked to imagine the word's referent. The animals employed in all experiments varied in size. In Experiment 1, participants were instructed to imagine the animals in a fixed context. Reproduced duration increased not only with the numbers of letters of the animal word (i.e., physical size) but also with the size of the word's referent (i.e., implicit size of the animal). Experiments 2 and 3 examined whether the effect of size would increase when the size of the animals was made more salient. The results of all experiments showed that reproduced duration increases with implicit stimulus size. In addition, Experiments 2 and 3 provide some evidence that the imagined size effect becomes more pronounced when the participant's attention is drawn to the size differences among the imagined animals. It is argued that the implicit size effect is mediated by mental imagery that operates on pre-existing space-time associations. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Imaginación , Percepción del Tamaño , Percepción del Tiempo , Adolescente , Adulto , Animales , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Psicofísica , Lectura , Percepción Visual , Adulto Joven
4.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 77(5): 1737-49, 2015 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25832186

RESUMEN

Three experiments assessed whether participants perceive infrequent stimuli (oddballs) to last longer than frequent ones (standards). The classical oddball paradigm was modified so that participants judged the duration of a target stimulus which could either be a standard or an oddball. The target was always the fifth stimulus in the stream and all stimuli were presented at predefined spatial positions. These modifications enabled a direct comparison of duration judgments for oddballs and standards. In Experiments 1 and 2 not only the duration of oddballs but also the duration of standards was overestimated by virtually the same amount. In other words, the overestimation of oddballs was not due to their oddness but reflected a different temporal dilation such as the negative time-order error. In Experiment 3, all stimuli were presented at the same spatial position. Again, both oddballs and standards were overestimated, however, oddballs more so. The present results highlight the importance of comparing the judged duration of oddballs and standards when evaluating the size of the genuine oddball effect. Furthermore, the findings suggest that the occurrence of temporal oddball effects can depend on spatial features of stimulus presentation.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Incertidumbre , Adulto Joven
5.
Int J Sport Nutr Exerc Metab ; 24(6): 623-31, 2014 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24901677

RESUMEN

Nutritional supplements (NS) are defined as concentrated sources of nutrients and other substances that have a nutritional or physiological effect and that are used in high frequency among athletes. The study aimed to create a prediction profile for young elite athletes to identify those athletes who have a higher relative risk for using NS. The second objective was to examine the hypothesis that the consumption of NS paves a gateway for the use of illicit drugs and doping substances. A self-designed anonymous paper-and-pencil questionnaire was used to examine the prevalence of NS consumption, doping, and illicit drug use in elite athletes with a mean age of 17 years (SD = 4 years). Logistic regression analysis was employed to assess whether NS consumption can be predicted by independent variables (e.g., biographical data, training characteristics, drug consumption behavior) to create the prediction profile for NS use. 55% and 5% of the athletes (n = 536) responded positively to having used NS and illicit drugs, respectively. Nutritional supplement consumption was positively correlated with age (OR: 1.92; CI: 1.21 to 3.05), the desire to enhance performance to become an Olympic or World Champion (OR: 3.72; CI: 2.33 to 6.01), and being educated about NS (OR: 2.76; CI: 1.73 to 4.45). It was negatively correlated with training frequency (OR: 0.55; CI: 0.35 to 0.86) and the use of nicotine (OR: 0.29; CI: 0.1 to 0.74) but did not correlate with illicit drug use and alcohol consumption. The present results show that NS are used on a large scale in elite sports. The prediction profile presented in this article may help to identify those athletes who have a high risk for using NS to plan potential education and prevention models more individually.


Asunto(s)
Atletas/psicología , Suplementos Dietéticos/estadística & datos numéricos , Doping en los Deportes/estadística & datos numéricos , Predicción , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Atletas/estadística & datos numéricos , Rendimiento Atlético/psicología , Niño , Doping en los Deportes/tendencias , Femenino , Alemania , Humanos , Masculino , Oportunidad Relativa , Sustancias para Mejorar el Rendimiento/administración & dosificación , Análisis de Regresión , Asunción de Riesgos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
6.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 67(6): 1121-50, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24295428

RESUMEN

In typical discrimination experiments, participants are presented with a constant standard and a variable comparison stimulus and their task is to judge which of these two stimuli is larger (comparative judgement). In these experiments, discrimination sensitivity depends on the temporal order of these stimuli (Type B effect) and is usually higher when the standard precedes rather than follows the comparison. Here, we outline how two models of stimulus discrimination can account for the Type B effect, namely the weighted difference model (or basic Sensation Weighting model) and the Internal Reference Model. For both models, the predicted psychometric functions for comparative judgements as well as for equality judgements, in which participants indicate whether they perceived the two stimuli to be equal or not equal, are derived and it is shown that the models also predict a Type B effect for equality judgements. In the empirical part, the models' predictions are evaluated. To this end, participants performed a duration discrimination task with comparative judgements and with equality judgements. In line with the models' predictions, a Type B effect was observed for both judgement types. In addition, a time-order error, as indicated by shifts of the psychometric functions, and differences in response times were observed only for the equality judgement. Since both models entail distinct additional predictions, it seems worthwhile for future research to unite the two models into one conceptual framework.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Modelos Psicológicos , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicometría , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Sensibilidad y Especificidad , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
7.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 11(1): 68-84, 2011 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21264649

RESUMEN

In two experiments, each including a simple reaction time (RT) task, a localization task, and a passive oddball paradigm, the physical similarity between two dichotically presented auditory stimuli was manipulated. In both experiments, a redundant signals effect (RSE), high localization performance, and a reliable mismatch negativity (MMN) was observed for largely differing stimuli, suggesting that these are coded separately in auditory memory. In contrast, no RSE and a localization rate close to chance level (experiment 1) or at chance (experiment 2) were observed for stimuli differing to a lesser degree. Crucially, for such stimuli a small (experiment 1) or no (experiment 2) MMN were observed. These MMN results indicate that such stimuli tend to fuse into a single percept and that this fusion occurs rather early within information processing.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Variación Contingente Negativa/fisiología , Pruebas de Audición Dicótica , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Pruebas de Audición Dicótica/métodos , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Electrooculografía/métodos , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Psicoacústica , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
8.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 63(12): 2432-51, 2010 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20544563

RESUMEN

A clock paradigm was employed to assess whether temporal preparation decreases the time to detect the onset of a stimulus-that is, perceptual latency. In four experiments participants watched a revolving clock hand while listening to soft or loud target tones under high or low temporal preparation. At the end of each trial, participants reported the clock hand position at the onset of the target tone. The deviation of the reported clock hand position from the actual position indexed perceptual latency. As expected, perceptual latency decreased with target tone intensity. Most importantly, however, greater temporal preparation decreased perceptual latency in all four experiments, especially for soft tones, which supports rather directly the idea that temporal preparation diminishes the duration of perceptual processing.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Percepción del Tiempo , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
9.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 134(1): 94-104, 2010 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20096395

RESUMEN

When a warning signal (WS) precedes an imperative signal (IS) by a certain amount of time (the foreperiod, FP), responses are speeded. Moreover, this effect is modulated by the FP length in the previous trial. This sequential FP effect has lately been attributed to a trace-conditioning mechanism according to which individuals learn (and re-learn) temporal relationships between the WS and the IS. Recent evidence suggests that sensory WS attributes are critical to trigger time-related response activation. Specifically, when WS modality is shifted in subsequent trials (e.g., from auditory to visual modality), the sequential FP effect becomes attenuated. This study examined whether the sequential FP effect is reduced only by between-modality shifts or whether this attenuation generalizes to cross-trial shifts of WS attributes within modalities. We compared dimensional (low vs. high tone frequency) and qualitative shifts (pure tone vs. noise) of equal-intense auditory WS events. The results of four experiments revealed that shifts of tone frequency did not, whereas shifts of qualitative tone characteristics did attenuate the sequential FP effect. These results support the view that the WS acts as a trigger cue that unintentionally activates responses at previously reinforced critical moments.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Acústica , Aprendizaje por Asociación , Atención , Percepción Auditiva , Señales (Psicología) , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Tiempo de Reacción , Percepción del Tiempo , Adulto , Conducta de Elección , Condicionamiento Psicológico , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional , Generalización Psicológica , Humanos , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor , Psicofísica , Adulto Joven
10.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 16(2): 382-9, 2009 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19293111

RESUMEN

Perceptual learning was used to study potential transfer effects in a duration discrimination task. Subjects were trained to discriminate between two empty temporal intervals marked with auditory beeps, using a two-alternative forced choice paradigm. The major goal was to examine whether perceptual learning would generalize to empty intervals that have the same duration but are marked by visual flashes. The experiment also included longer intervals marked with auditory beeps and filled auditory intervals of the same duration as the trained interval, in order to examine whether perceptual learning would generalize to these conditions within the same sensory modality. In contrast to previous findings showing a transfer from the haptic to the auditory modality, the present results do not indicate a transfer from the auditory to the visual modality; but they do show transfers within the auditory modality.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Percepción del Tiempo , Transferencia de Experiencia en Psicología , Percepción Visual , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Femenino , Generalización Psicológica , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Valorización y Adquisición Práctica , Psicofísica , Retención en Psicología , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA