Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 98
Filtrar
Más filtros

Medicinas Complementárias
Bases de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 41(13): 3680-3695, 2020 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32583940

RESUMEN

Previous research in young adults has demonstrated that both motor learning and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) trigger decreases in the levels of gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) in the sensorimotor cortex, and these decreases are linked to greater learning. Less is known about the role of GABA in motor learning in healthy older adults, a knowledge gap that is surprising given the established aging-related reductions in sensorimotor GABA. Here, we examined the effects of motor learning and subsequent tDCS on sensorimotor GABA levels and resting-state functional connectivity in the brains of healthy older participants. Thirty-six older men and women completed a motor sequence learning task before receiving anodal or sham tDCS to the sensorimotor cortex. GABA-edited magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the sensorimotor cortex and resting-state (RS) functional magnetic resonance imaging data were acquired before and after learning/stimulation. At the group level, neither learning nor anodal tDCS significantly modulated GABA levels or RS connectivity among task-relevant regions. However, changes in GABA levels from the baseline to post-learning session were significantly related to motor learning magnitude, age, and baseline GABA. Moreover, the change in functional connectivity between task-relevant regions, including bilateral motor cortices, was correlated with baseline GABA levels. These data collectively indicate that motor learning-related decreases in sensorimotor GABA levels and increases in functional connectivity are limited to those older adults with higher baseline GABA levels and who learn the most. Post-learning tDCS exerted no influence on GABA levels, functional connectivity or the relationships among these variables in older adults.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Conectoma , Espectroscopía de Resonancia Magnética , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología , Corteza Sensoriomotora/fisiología , Aprendizaje Seriado/fisiología , Estimulación Transcraneal de Corriente Directa , Ácido gamma-Aminobutírico/metabolismo , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Corteza Motora/diagnóstico por imagen , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Corteza Sensoriomotora/diagnóstico por imagen , Corteza Sensoriomotora/metabolismo
2.
Neuropsychologia ; 134: 107204, 2019 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31562864

RESUMEN

Here we report a detailed analysis of the fast network dynamics underlying P3a and P3b event-related potential (ERP) subcomponents generated during an unconventional serial auditory search paradigm. We dissect the electroencephalographic (EEG) data from an earlier study of ours, using a variety of advanced signal processing techniques, in order to discover how the brain is processing auditory targets differently when they possess a rare, salient, unpredictable feature not shared with distractors than when targets lack this feature but distractors have it. We find that brain regions associated with the Ventral Attention Network (VAN) are the primary neural generators of the P3a subcomponent in response to feature-present targets, whereas regions associated with the Dorsal Attention Network (DAN), as well as regions associated with detecting auditory oddball stimuli (ODD), may be the primary neural generators of the P3b, in the context of our study, and perhaps in search paradigms in general. Moreover, measurements of the time courses of oscillatory power changes and inter-regional synchronization in theta and low-gamma frequency bands were consistent with the early activation and synchronization within the VAN associated with the P3a subcomponent, and with the later activation and synchronization within the DAN and ODD networks associated with the P3b subcomponent. Implications of these finding for the mechanisms underlying search asymmetry phenomena are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Aprendizaje Seriado/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos/fisiología , Potenciales Postsinápticos Excitadores/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Procesamiento de Señales Asistido por Computador , Adulto Joven
3.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 72(10): 2541-2553, 2019 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31272296

RESUMEN

The method of loci is arguably the most famous mnemonic strategy and is highly effective for memorising lists of non-spatial information in order. As described and instructed, this strategy apparently relies on a spatial/navigational metaphor. The user imagines moving through an environment, placing (study) and reporting (recall) list items along the way. However, whether the method relies critically on this spatial/navigation metaphor is unknown. An alternative hypothesis is that the navigation component is superfluous to memory success, and the method of loci is better viewed as a special case of a larger class of imagery-based peg strategies. Training participants on three virtual environments varying in their characteristics (an apartment, an open field, and a radial-arm maze), we asked participants to use each trained environment as the basis of the method of loci to learn five 11-word lists. Performance varied significantly across environment. However, the effects were small in magnitude. Further tests suggested that navigation-relevant knowledge and ability were not major determinants of success in verbal memory, even for participants who were confirmed to have been compliant with the strategy. These findings echo neuroimaging findings that navigation-based cognition does occur during application of the method of loci, but imagined navigation is unlikely to be directly responsible for its effectiveness. Instead, the method of loci may be best viewed as a variant of peg methods.


Asunto(s)
Imaginación/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Aprendizaje Seriado/fisiología , Navegación Espacial/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Realidad Virtual , Adulto Joven
4.
Neuroimage ; 169: 419-430, 2018 04 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29277652

RESUMEN

Sleep benefits motor memory consolidation. This mnemonic process is thought to be mediated by thalamo-cortical spindle activity during NREM-stage2 sleep episodes as well as changes in striatal and hippocampal activity. However, direct experimental evidence supporting the contribution of such sleep-dependent physiological mechanisms to motor memory consolidation in humans is lacking. In the present study, we combined EEG and fMRI sleep recordings following practice of a motor sequence learning (MSL) task to determine whether spindle oscillations support sleep-dependent motor memory consolidation by transiently synchronizing and coordinating specialized cortical and subcortical networks. To that end, we conducted EEG source reconstruction on spindle epochs in both cortical and subcortical regions using novel deep-source localization techniques. Coherence-based metrics were adopted to estimate functional connectivity between cortical and subcortical structures over specific frequency bands. Our findings not only confirm the critical and functional role of NREM-stage2 sleep spindles in motor skill consolidation, but provide first-time evidence that spindle oscillations [11-17 Hz] may be involved in sleep-dependent motor memory consolidation by locally reactivating and functionally binding specific task-relevant cortical and subcortical regions within networks including the hippocampus, putamen, thalamus and motor-related cortical regions.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía/métodos , Neuroimagen Funcional/métodos , Hipocampo/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Consolidación de la Memoria/fisiología , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Putamen/fisiología , Fases del Sueño/fisiología , Tálamo/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Hipocampo/diagnóstico por imagen , Humanos , Masculino , Putamen/diagnóstico por imagen , Aprendizaje Seriado/fisiología , Tálamo/diagnóstico por imagen , Adulto Joven
5.
J Bodyw Mov Ther ; 21(3): 626-632, 2017 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28750975

RESUMEN

Sequential visual isometric pinch task (SVIPT) has been recently used as a visuomotor sequence task in clinical research. The influence of varying intervals between sequenced trials on the acquisition of implicit sequence learning is not yet determined for SVIPT. The aim of this study was to investigate the effects of inter-trial interval (ITI) on implicit motor sequence learning using SVIPT. A total of 32 healthy participants with mean age 31.3 ± 4.5 years participated in this study. Participants were randomly assigned to one of four ITI groups; (1, 2, 3 and 4 s). They were instructed to control their force on a force transducer to reach a number of targets which appeared on the computer screen by changing the pinch force exerted onto the transducer. In this study, outcome measures were movement time, error rate and skill, which were measured before and after training. Our results indicated that motor sequence learning similarly affected various ITIs. Indeed, all participants exhibited same improvement in implicit learning of SVIPT even though the ITIs varied from 1 to 4 s. Our findings suggest that implicit learning of SVIPT is independent of ITI within this range in healthy individuals.


Asunto(s)
Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Aprendizaje Seriado/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Destreza Motora , Tiempo de Reacción , Método Simple Ciego , Factores de Tiempo
6.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 70(4): 664-674, 2017 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26848780

RESUMEN

How can we grasp the temporal structure of events? A few studies have indicated that representations of temporal structure are acquired when there is an intention to learn, but not when learning is incidental. Response-to-stimulus intervals, uncorrelated temporal structures, unpredictable ordinal information, and lack of metrical organization have been pointed out as key obstacles to incidental temporal learning, but the literature includes piecemeal demonstrations of learning under all these circumstances. We suggest that the unacknowledged effects of ordinal load may help reconcile these conflicting findings, ordinal load referring to the cost of identifying the sequence of events (e.g., tones, locations) where a temporal pattern is embedded. In a first experiment, we manipulated ordinal load into simple and complex levels. Participants learned ordinal-simple sequences, despite their uncorrelated temporal structure and lack of metrical organization. They did not learn ordinal-complex sequences, even though there were no response-to-stimulus intervals nor unpredictable ordinal information. In a second experiment, we probed learning of ordinal-complex sequences with strong metrical organization, and again there was no learning. We conclude that ordinal load is a key obstacle to incidental temporal learning. Further analyses showed that the effect of ordinal load is to mask the expression of temporal knowledge, rather than to prevent learning.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Aprendizaje Seriado/fisiología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Análisis de Varianza , Femenino , Humanos , Intención , Masculino , Fonética , Psicoacústica , Adulto Joven
7.
Neuropsychologia ; 85: 80-90, 2016 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26972966

RESUMEN

Beat perception is the ability to perceive temporal regularity in musical rhythm. When a beat is perceived, predictions about upcoming events can be generated. These predictions can influence processing of subsequent rhythmic events. However, statistical learning of the order of sounds in a sequence can also affect processing of rhythmic events and must be differentiated from beat perception. In the current study, using EEG, we examined the effects of attention and musical abilities on beat perception. To ensure we measured beat perception and not absolute perception of temporal intervals, we used alternating loud and soft tones to create a rhythm with two hierarchical metrical levels. To control for sequential learning of the order of the different sounds, we used temporally regular (isochronous) and jittered rhythmic sequences. The order of sounds was identical in both conditions, but only the regular condition allowed for the perception of a beat. Unexpected intensity decrements were introduced on the beat and offbeat. In the regular condition, both beat perception and sequential learning were expected to enhance detection of these deviants on the beat. In the jittered condition, only sequential learning was expected to affect processing of the deviants. ERP responses to deviants were larger on the beat than offbeat in both conditions. Importantly, this difference was larger in the regular condition than in the jittered condition, suggesting that beat perception influenced responses to rhythmic events in addition to sequential learning. The influence of beat perception was present both with and without attention directed at the rhythm. Moreover, beat perception as measured with ERPs correlated with musical abilities, but only when attention was directed at the stimuli. Our study shows that beat perception is possible when attention is not directed at a rhythm. In addition, our results suggest that attention may mediate the influence of musical abilities on beat perception.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos/fisiología , Música , Periodicidad , Aprendizaje Seriado/fisiología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Psicoacústica , Estadística como Asunto , Adulto Joven
8.
Hum Mov Sci ; 45: 71-83, 2016 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26605966

RESUMEN

We examined the effects of a bandwidth feedback manipulation on motor learning. Effects on movement accuracy, as well as on movement consistency, have been addressed in earlier studies. We have additionally investigated the effects on motor automatization. Because providing error feedback is believed to induce attentional control processes, we suppose that a bandwidth method should facilitate motor automatization. Participants (N=48) were assigned to four groups: one control group and three intervention groups. Participants of the intervention groups practiced an arm movement sequence with 760 trials. The BW0-Group practiced with 100% frequency of feedback. For the BW10-Group, feedback was provided when the errors were larger than 10°. The YokedBW10-Group participants were matched to the feedback schedule of research twins from the BW10-Group. All groups performed pre-tests and retention tests with a secondary task paradigm to test for automaticity. The BW10-Group indicated a higher degree of automatization compared with the BW0-Group, which did not exhibit a change in automaticity. The comparison of the YokedBW10-Group, which also exhibited automatization, and the BW10-Group leads to the proposal that reduction of quantitative feedback frequency and additional positive feedback are responsible for the bandwidth effect. Differences in movement accuracy and consistency were not evident.


Asunto(s)
Automatismo , Retroalimentación Psicológica , Conocimiento Psicológico de los Resultados , Destreza Motora , Desempeño Psicomotor , Aprendizaje Seriado , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Atención , Percepción de Color , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Práctica Psicológica , Retención en Psicología , Privación Sensorial , Adulto Joven
9.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 123: 18-27, 2015 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25914128

RESUMEN

Atropine sulfate is a muscarinic cholinergic antagonist which impairs acquisition and retention performance on a variety of cognitive tasks. The present study examined the effects of atropine on acquisition and retention of a highly-structured serial pattern in a serial multiple choice (SMC) task. Rats were given daily intraperitoneal injections of either saline or atropine sulfate (50mg/kg) and trained in an octagonal operant chamber equipped with a lever on each wall. They learned to press the levers in a particular order (the serial pattern) for brain-stimulation reward in a discrete-trial procedure with correction. The two groups learned a pattern composed of eight 3-element chunks ending with a violation element: 123-234-345-456-567-678-781-818 where the digits represent the clock-wise positions of levers in the chamber, dashes indicate 3-s pauses, and other intertrial intervals were 1s. Central muscarinic cholinergic blockade by atropine caused profound impairments during acquisition, specifically in the encoding of chunk-boundary elements (the first element of chunks) and the violation element of the pattern, but had a significant but negligible effect on the encoding of within-chunk elements relative to saline-injected rats. These effects persisted when atropine was removed, and similar impairments were also observed in retention performance. The results indicate that intact central muscarinic cholinergic systems are necessary for learning and producing appropriate responses at places in sequences where pattern structure changes. The results also provide further evidence that multiple cognitive systems are recruited to learn and perform within-chunk, chunk-boundary, and violation elements of a serial pattern.


Asunto(s)
Atropina/farmacología , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Antagonistas Muscarínicos/farmacología , Desempeño Psicomotor/efectos de los fármacos , Aprendizaje Seriado/efectos de los fármacos , Animales , Conducta Animal/efectos de los fármacos , Señales (Psicología) , Estimulación Eléctrica , Hipotálamo/fisiología , Inyecciones Intraperitoneales , Ratas , Ratas Long-Evans , Recompensa
10.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 41(5): 1570-8, 2015 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25730304

RESUMEN

Serial ordering mechanisms have been investigated extensively in psychology and psycholinguistics. It has also been demonstrated repeatedly that long-term phonological knowledge contributes to serial ordering. However, the mechanisms that contribute to serial ordering have yet to be fully understood. To understand these mechanisms, we demonstrate 2 effects using triples of Japanese nonwords in immediate serial recall. One, a type of bielement frequency effect, is a retrograde compatibility effect. Bielement frequency effects are well-established phenomena whereby a 2-element sequence (e.g., "ka-re") that frequently appears in a language instantiates better recall of any sequence that includes this element (e.g., "ka-re-su-mo"). We demonstrate that bielement frequency affected both the first (e.g., "ka" for "ka-re"; retrograde compatibility effect) and second part of a sequence, indicating the existence of minicontext representations of 2-element sequences. The other effects are the position-element(s) frequency effects, whereby an element (e.g., the mora "ka") that more frequently appears in 1 position of a sequence (e.g., in the first mora of a word) than in other positions facilitates better recall of that element (i.e., the first mora). The effects demonstrated in this article indicate the long-term associations of position representations and elements. These effects are discussed in terms of the extensive learning hypothesis, which assumes that phonological structures are learned gradually. Implications for computational models are also discussed.


Asunto(s)
Conocimiento , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Fonética , Aprendizaje Seriado/fisiología , Aprendizaje Verbal/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental , Persona de Mediana Edad , Probabilidad , Psicolingüística , Tiempo de Reacción , Vocabulario , Adulto Joven
11.
Exp Brain Res ; 233(4): 1125-36, 2015 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25567086

RESUMEN

The present study sought to investigate the neural basis of implicit learning of task-irrelevant perceptual sequence. A novel SRT task, the serial syllable identification task (SSI task), was used in which the participants were asked to recognize which one of two Chinese syllables was presented. The tones of the syllables were irrelevant to the task but followed an underlying structured sequence. Participants were scanned while they performed the SSI task. Results showed that, at the behavioral level, faster RTs for the sequential material indicated that task-irrelevant sequence knowledge could be learned. In the subsequent prediction test of knowledge of the tonal cues using subjective measures, we found that the knowledge was obtained unconsciously. At the neural level, the left caudate, bilateral hippocampus and bilateral superior parietal lobule were engaged during the sequence condition relative to the random condition. Further analyses revealed that greater learning-related activation (relative to random) in the right caudate nucleus, bilateral hippocampus and left superior parietal lobule were found during the second half of the training phase compared with the first half. When people reported that they were guessing, the magnitude of the right hippocampus and left superior parietal lobule activations was positively related to the accuracy of prediction test, which was significantly better than chance. Together, the present results indicated that the caudate, hippocampus and superior parietal lobule played critical roles in the implicit perceptual sequence learning even when the perceptual features were task irrelevant.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Encéfalo/fisiología , Fonética , Aprendizaje Seriado/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Concienciación , Encéfalo/irrigación sanguínea , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Oxígeno/sangre , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto Joven
12.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 27(4): 819-31, 2015 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25313656

RESUMEN

Speech is perhaps the most sophisticated example of a species-wide movement capability in the animal kingdom, requiring split-second sequencing of approximately 100 muscles in the respiratory, laryngeal, and oral movement systems. Despite the unique role speech plays in human interaction and the debilitating impact of its disruption, little is known about the neural mechanisms underlying speech motor learning. Here, we studied the behavioral and neural correlates of learning new speech motor sequences. Participants repeatedly produced novel, meaningless syllables comprising illegal consonant clusters (e.g., GVAZF) over 2 days of practice. Following practice, participants produced the sequences with fewer errors and shorter durations, indicative of motor learning. Using fMRI, we compared brain activity during production of the learned illegal sequences and novel illegal sequences. Greater activity was noted during production of novel sequences in brain regions linked to non-speech motor sequence learning, including the BG and pre-SMA. Activity during novel sequence production was also greater in brain regions associated with learning and maintaining speech motor programs, including lateral premotor cortex, frontal operculum, and posterior superior temporal cortex. Measures of learning success correlated positively with activity in left frontal operculum and white matter integrity under left posterior superior temporal sulcus. These findings indicate speech motor sequence learning relies not only on brain areas involved generally in motor sequencing learning but also those associated with feedback-based speech motor learning. Furthermore, learning success is modulated by the integrity of structural connectivity between these motor and sensory brain regions.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Encéfalo/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Aprendizaje Seriado/fisiología , Habla/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Encéfalo/irrigación sanguínea , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Oxígeno/sangre , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
13.
Conscious Cogn ; 28: 141-50, 2014 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25062120

RESUMEN

Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence suggest that mindfulness exerts its salutary effects by disengaging habitual processes supported by subcortical regions and increasing effortful control processes supported by the frontal lobes. Here we investigated whether individual differences in dispositional mindfulness relate to performance on implicit sequence learning tasks in which optimal learning may in fact be impeded by the engagement of effortful control processes. We report results from two studies where participants completed a widely used questionnaire assessing mindfulness and one of two implicit sequence learning tasks. Learning was quantified using two commonly used measures of sequence learning. In both studies we detected a negative relationship between mindfulness and sequence learning, and the relationship was consistent across both learning measures. Our results, the first to show a negative relationship between mindfulness and implicit sequence learning, suggest that the beneficial effects of mindfulness do not extend to all cognitive functions.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje , Atención Plena , Adolescente , Cognición , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción , Aprendizaje Seriado , Adulto Joven
14.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 67(11): 2207-17, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24796760

RESUMEN

Phonological working memory is known be (a) inversely related to the duration of the items to be learned (word-length effect), and (b) impaired by the presence of irrelevant speech-like sounds (irrelevant-speech effect). As it is discussed controversially whether these memory disruptions are subject to attentional control, both effects were studied in sighted participants and in a sample of early blind individuals who are expected to be superior in selectively attending to auditory stimuli. Results show that, while performance depended on word length in both groups, irrelevant speech interfered with recall only in the sighted group, but not in blind participants. This suggests that blind listeners may be able to effectively prevent irrelevant sound from being encoded in the phonological store, presumably due to superior auditory processing. The occurrence of a word-length effect, however, implies that blind and sighted listeners are utilizing the same phonological rehearsal mechanism in order to maintain information in the phonological store.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Ceguera/fisiopatología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Aprendizaje Seriado/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Anciano , Análisis Factorial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Sensibilidad y Especificidad , Aprendizaje Verbal/fisiología , Adulto Joven
15.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 26(10): 2210-23, 2014 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24673407

RESUMEN

"Tip-of-the-tongue" (TOT) is the phenomenon associated with the inaccessibility of a known word from memory. It is universally experienced, increases in frequency with age, and is most common for proper nouns. It is a good model for the symptom of anomia experienced much more frequently by some aphasic patients following brain injury. Here, we induced the TOT state in older participants while they underwent brain scanning with magnetoencephalography to investigate the changes in oscillatory brain activity associated with failed retrieval of known words. Using confrontation naming of pictures of celebrities, we successfully induced the TOT state in 29% of trials and contrasted it with two other states: "Know" where the participants both correctly recognized the celebrity's face and retrieved their name and "Don't Know" when the participants did not recognize the celebrity. We wished to test Levelt's influential model of speech output by carrying out two analyses, one epoching the data to the point in time when the picture was displayed and the other looking back in time from when the participants first articulated their responses. Our main findings supported the components of Levelt's model, but not their serial activation over time as both semantic and motor areas were identified in both analyses. We also found enduring decreases in the alpha frequency band in the left ventral temporal region during the TOT state, suggesting ongoing semantic search. Finally, we identified reduced beta power in classical peri-sylvian language areas for the TOT condition, suggesting that brain regions that encode linguistic memories are also involved in their attempted retrieval.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Ondas Encefálicas/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Pensamiento/fisiología , Vocabulario , Estimulación Acústica , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Magnetoencefalografía , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Nombres , Estimulación Luminosa , Semántica , Aprendizaje Seriado , Factores de Tiempo , Aprendizaje Verbal/fisiología
16.
Learn Mem ; 20(12): 674-85, 2013 Nov 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24241750

RESUMEN

Pairing a previously neutral conditioned stimulus (CS; e.g., a tone) to an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US; e.g., a footshock) leads to associative learning such that the tone alone comes to elicit a conditioned response (e.g., freezing). We have previously shown that an extinction session that occurs within the reconsolidation window attenuates fear responding and prevents the return of fear in pure tone Pavlovian fear conditioning. Here we sought to examine whether this effect also applies to a more complex fear memory. First, we show that after fear conditioning to the simultaneous presentation of a tone and a light (T+L) coterminating with a shock, the compound memory that ensues is more resistant to fear extinction than simple tone-shock pairings. Next, we demonstrate that the compound memory can be disrupted by interrupting the reconsolidation of the two individual components using a sequential retrieval+extinction paradigm, provided the stronger compound component is retrieved first. These findings provide insight into how compound memories are encoded, and could have important implications for PTSD treatment.


Asunto(s)
Condicionamiento Clásico/fisiología , Extinción Psicológica/fisiología , Miedo , Memoria a Largo Plazo/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica/efectos adversos , Análisis de Varianza , Animales , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Señales (Psicología) , Reacción Cataléptica de Congelación , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/efectos adversos , Proteínas Proto-Oncogénicas c-fos/metabolismo , Psicofísica , Ratas , Ratas Sprague-Dawley , Aprendizaje Seriado/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo
17.
Cereb Cortex ; 23(4): 801-5, 2013 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22459017

RESUMEN

Human learning and memory depend on multiple cognitive systems related to dissociable brain structures. These systems interact not only in cooperative but also sometimes competitive ways in optimizing performance. Previous studies showed that manipulations reducing the engagement of frontal lobe-mediated explicit attentional processes could lead to improved performance in striatum-related procedural learning. In our study, hypnosis was used as a tool to reduce the competition between these 2 systems. We compared learning in hypnosis and in the alert state and found that hypnosis boosted striatum-dependent sequence learning. Since frontal lobe-dependent processes are primarily affected by hypnosis, this finding could be attributed to the disruption of the explicit attentional processes. Our result sheds light not only on the competitive nature of brain systems in cognitive processes but also could have important implications for training and rehabilitation programs, especially for developing new methods to improve human learning and memory performance.


Asunto(s)
Hipnosis , Aprendizaje Seriado/fisiología , Aprendizaje Verbal/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Función Ejecutiva , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto Joven
18.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 39(2): 494-509, 2013 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22963232

RESUMEN

Evidence suggests that people with autism rely less on holistic visual information than typical adults. The current studies examine this by investigating core visual processes that contribute to holistic processing--namely, individuation and element grouping--and how they develop in participants with autism and typically developing (TD) participants matched for age, IQ, and gender. Individuation refers to the ability to "see" approximately four elements simultaneously; grouping elements can modify how many elements can be individuated. We examined these processes using two well-established paradigms, rapid enumeration and multiple object tracking (MOT). In both tasks, a performance limit of four elements in typical adults is thought to reflect individuation capacity. Participants with autism displayed a smaller individuation capacity than TD controls, regardless of whether they were enumerating static elements or tracking moving ones. To manipulate the holistic information available via element grouping, elements were arranged into a design in rapid enumeration, or moved together in MOT. Performance in participants with autism was affected to a similar degree as TD participants by element grouping, whether the manipulation helped or hurt performance, consistent with evidence that some types of gestalt/grouping information are processed typically in autism. There was substantial development from childhood to adolescence in the speed of individuation in those with autism, but not from adolescence to adulthood, a pattern distinct from TD participants. These results reveal how core visual processes function in autism, and provide insight into the architecture of vision (i.e., individuation appears distinct from visual strengths in autism, such as visual search).


Asunto(s)
Asociación , Atención , Trastorno Autístico/psicología , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Orientación , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Femenino , Teoría Gestáltica , Humanos , Masculino , Matemática , Solución de Problemas , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tiempo de Reacción , Aprendizaje Seriado , Conducta Verbal
19.
Neurosci Lett ; 540: 37-42, 2013 Apr 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23206748

RESUMEN

Both motor imagery and action observation have been shown to play a role in learning or re-learning complex motor tasks. According to a well accepted view they share a common neurophysiological basis in the mirror neuron system. Neurons within this system discharge when individuals perform a specific action and when they look at another individual performing the same or a motorically related action. In the present paper, after a short review of literature on the role of action observation and motor imagery in motor learning, we report the results of a kinematics study where we directly compared motor imagery and action observation in learning a novel complex motor task. This involved movement of the right hand and foot in the same angular direction (in-phase movement), while at the same time moving the left hand and foot in an opposite angular direction (anti-phase movement), all at a frequency of 1Hz. Motor learning was assessed through kinematics recording of wrists and ankles. The results showed that action observation is better than motor imagery as a strategy for learning a novel complex motor task, at least in the fast early phase of motor learning. We forward that these results may have important implications in educational activities, sport training and neurorehabilitation.


Asunto(s)
Imaginación , Aprendizaje , Percepción de Movimiento , Movimiento , Adulto , Animales , Aprendizaje por Asociación , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Encéfalo/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Conducta Imitativa , Masculino , Neuronas Espejo/fisiología , Modelos Psicológicos , Práctica Psicológica , Rango del Movimiento Articular , Aprendizaje Seriado , Adulto Joven
20.
Cogn Process ; 13(3): 267-76, 2012 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22618605

RESUMEN

In this study, we investigated the interactions between temporal and spatial information in auditory working memory. In two experiments, participants were presented with sequences of sounds originating from different locations in space and were then asked to recall either their position or their serial order. In Experiment 1, attention during encoding was manipulated by contrasting 'pure' blocks (i.e., location-only or serial-order-only trials) to 'mixed' blocks (i.e., different percentages of spatial and serial-order trials). In Experiment 2, 'pure' blocks were contrasted to blocks in which spatial and serial-order trials were intermixed with a third task requiring a semantic categorization of sounds. Results from both experiments showed that, whereas serial-order recall is linearly affected by the simultaneous encoding of a concurrent feature, the recall of position is mostly unaffected by concurrent feature encoding. Contrastingly, overall performance level was lower for spatial recall than serial recall. We concluded that serial order and location of items appear to be independently encoded in auditory working memory. Serial order is easier to recall, but strongly affected by the processing of concurrent item dimensions, while item location is more difficult to recall, but relatively automatic, as shown by its strong resistance to interfering dimensions in encoding.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Aprendizaje Seriado/fisiología , Localización de Sonidos/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Psicoacústica , Tiempo de Reacción , Semántica , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA