Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 3.425
Filtrar
Más filtros

Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Neuropsychologia ; 197: 108850, 2024 May 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38467371

RESUMEN

Neurodevelopmental disorders are traditionally characterised by a range of associated cognitive impairments in, for example, sensory processing, facial recognition, visual imagery, attention, and coordination. In this critical review, we propose a major reframing, highlighting the variety of unique cognitive strengths that people with neurodevelopmental differences can exhibit. These include enhanced visual perception, strong spatial, auditory, and semantic memory, superior empathy and theory of mind, along with higher levels of divergent thinking. Whilst we acknowledge the heterogeneity of cognitive profiles in neurodevelopmental conditions, we present a more encouraging and affirmative perspective of these groups, contrasting with the predominant, deficit-based position prevalent throughout both cognitive and neuropsychological research. In addition, we provide a theoretical basis and rationale for these cognitive strengths, arguing for the critical role of hereditability, behavioural adaptation, neuronal-recycling, and we draw on psychopharmacological and social explanations. We present a table of potential strengths across conditions and invite researchers to systematically investigate these in their future work. This should help reduce the stigma around neurodiversity, instead promoting greater social inclusion and significant societal benefits.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Dislexia , Trastornos del Neurodesarrollo , Humanos , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Percepción Visual , Cognición
2.
Proc Biol Sci ; 291(2018): 20232867, 2024 Mar 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38471562

RESUMEN

A delayed foveal mask affects perception of peripheral stimuli. The effect is determined by the timing of the mask and by the similarity with the peripheral stimulus. A congruent mask enhances performance, while an incongruent one impairs it. It is hypothesized that foveal masks disrupt a feedback mechanism reaching the foveal cortex. This mechanism could be part of a broader circuit associated with mental imagery, but this hypothesis has not as yet been tested. We investigated the link between mental imagery and foveal feedback. We tested the relationship between performance fluctuations caused by the foveal mask-measured in terms of discriminability (d') and criterion (C)-and the scores from two questionnaires designed to assess mental imagery vividness (VVIQ) and another exploring object imagery, spatial imagery and verbal cognitive styles (OSIVQ). Contrary to our hypotheses, no significant correlations were found between VVIQ and the mask's impact on d' and C. Neither the object nor spatial subscales of OSIVQ correlated with the mask's impact. In conclusion, our findings do not substantiate the existence of a link between foveal feedback and mental imagery. Further investigation is needed to determine whether mask interference might occur with more implicit measures of imagery.


Asunto(s)
Imaginación , Percepción Visual , Fóvea Central , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Personalidad
3.
Autism Res ; 17(2): 280-310, 2024 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38334251

RESUMEN

Autistic individuals show substantially reduced benefit from observing visual articulations during audiovisual speech perception, a multisensory integration deficit that is particularly relevant to social communication. This has mostly been studied using simple syllabic or word-level stimuli and it remains unclear how altered lower-level multisensory integration translates to the processing of more complex natural multisensory stimulus environments in autism. Here, functional neuroimaging was used to examine neural correlates of audiovisual gain (AV-gain) in 41 autistic individuals to those of 41 age-matched non-autistic controls when presented with a complex audiovisual narrative. Participants were presented with continuous narration of a story in auditory-alone, visual-alone, and both synchronous and asynchronous audiovisual speech conditions. We hypothesized that previously identified differences in audiovisual speech processing in autism would be characterized by activation differences in brain regions well known to be associated with audiovisual enhancement in neurotypicals. However, our results did not provide evidence for altered processing of auditory alone, visual alone, audiovisual conditions or AV- gain in regions associated with the respective task when comparing activation patterns between groups. Instead, we found that autistic individuals responded with higher activations in mostly frontal regions where the activation to the experimental conditions was below baseline (de-activations) in the control group. These frontal effects were observed in both unisensory and audiovisual conditions, suggesting that these altered activations were not specific to multisensory processing but reflective of more general mechanisms such as an altered disengagement of Default Mode Network processes during the observation of the language stimulus across conditions.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Trastorno Autístico , Percepción del Habla , Adulto , Niño , Humanos , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Narración , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/diagnóstico por imagen , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
4.
Neurosci Res ; 201: 27-30, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38311033

RESUMEN

The inability to visualise was given the name aphantasia in 2015 by Zeman and colleagues. In 2018 we published research showing that fifteen individuals who self-identified as having aphantasia also demonstrated a lack of sensory visual imagery when undergoing the binocular rivalry imagery paradigm, suggesting more than just a metacognitive difference. Here we update these findings with over fifty participants with aphantasia and show that there is evidence for a lack of sensory imagery in aphantasia. How the binocular rivalry paradigm scores relate to the vividness of visual imagery questionnaire (VVIQ) and how aphantasia can be confirmed is discussed.


Asunto(s)
Imágenes en Psicoterapia , Imaginación , Humanos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Percepción Visual
5.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 45(3): e26590, 2024 Feb 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38401134

RESUMEN

It has been suggested that visual images are memorized across brief periods of time by vividly imagining them as if they were still there. In line with this, the contents of both working memory and visual imagery are known to be encoded already in early visual cortex. If these signals in early visual areas were indeed to reflect a combined imagery and memory code, one would predict them to be weaker for individuals with reduced visual imagery vividness. Here, we systematically investigated this question in two groups of participants. Strong and weak imagers were asked to remember images across brief delay periods. We were able to reliably reconstruct the memorized stimuli from early visual cortex during the delay. Importantly, in contrast to the prediction, the quality of reconstruction was equally accurate for both strong and weak imagers. The decodable information also closely reflected behavioral precision in both groups, suggesting it could contribute to behavioral performance, even in the extreme case of completely aphantasic individuals. Our data thus suggest that working memory signals in early visual cortex can be present even in the (near) absence of phenomenal imagery.


Asunto(s)
Memoria a Corto Plazo , Corteza Visual , Humanos , Percepción Visual , Corteza Visual/diagnóstico por imagen , Imágenes en Psicoterapia , Recuerdo Mental , Imaginación
7.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 3262, 2024 02 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38332159

RESUMEN

The McGurk effect refers to an audiovisual speech illusion where the discrepant auditory and visual syllables produce a fused percept between the visual and auditory component. However, little is known about how individual differences contribute to the McGurk effect. Here, we examined whether music training experience-which involves audiovisual integration-can modulate the McGurk effect. Seventy-three participants completed the Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index (Gold-MSI) questionnaire to evaluate their music expertise on a continuous scale. Gold-MSI considers participants' daily-life exposure to music learning experiences (formal and informal), instead of merely classifying people into different groups according to how many years they have been trained in music. Participants were instructed to report, via a 3-alternative forced choice task, "what a person said": /Ba/, /Ga/ or /Da/. The experiment consisted of 96 audiovisual congruent trials and 96 audiovisual incongruent (McGurk) trials. We observed no significant correlations between the susceptibility of the McGurk effect and the different subscales of the Gold-MSI (active engagement, perceptual abilities, music training, singing abilities, emotion) or the general musical sophistication composite score. Together, these findings suggest that music training experience does not modulate audiovisual integration in speech as reflected by the McGurk effect.


Asunto(s)
Música , Percepción del Habla , Humanos , Percepción Visual , Habla , Oro , Percepción Auditiva , Estimulación Acústica
8.
PLoS Biol ; 22(2): e3002494, 2024 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38319934

RESUMEN

Effective interactions with the environment rely on the integration of multisensory signals: Our brains must efficiently combine signals that share a common source, and segregate those that do not. Healthy ageing can change or impair this process. This functional magnetic resonance imaging study assessed the neural mechanisms underlying age differences in the integration of auditory and visual spatial cues. Participants were presented with synchronous audiovisual signals at various degrees of spatial disparity and indicated their perceived sound location. Behaviourally, older adults were able to maintain localisation accuracy. At the neural level, they integrated auditory and visual cues into spatial representations along dorsal auditory and visual processing pathways similarly to their younger counterparts but showed greater activations in a widespread system of frontal, temporal, and parietal areas. According to multivariate Bayesian decoding, these areas encoded critical stimulus information beyond that which was encoded in the brain areas commonly activated by both groups. Surprisingly, however, the boost in information provided by these areas with age-related activation increases was comparable across the 2 age groups. This dissociation-between comparable information encoded in brain activation patterns across the 2 age groups, but age-related increases in regional blood-oxygen-level-dependent responses-contradicts the widespread notion that older adults recruit new regions as a compensatory mechanism to encode task-relevant information. Instead, our findings suggest that activation increases in older adults reflect nonspecific or modulatory mechanisms related to less efficient or slower processing, or greater demands on attentional resources.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Anciano , Teorema de Bayes , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética
9.
Neuroreport ; 35(4): 269-276, 2024 Mar 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38305131

RESUMEN

This study explored how the human brain perceives stickiness through tactile and auditory channels, especially when presented with congruent or incongruent intensity cues. In our behavioral and functional MRI (fMRI) experiments, we presented participants with adhesive tape stimuli at two different intensities. The congruent condition involved providing stickiness stimuli with matching intensity cues in both auditory and tactile channels, whereas the incongruent condition involved cues of different intensities. Behavioral results showed that participants were able to distinguish between the congruent and incongruent conditions with high accuracy. Through fMRI searchlight analysis, we tested which brain regions could distinguish between congruent and incongruent conditions, and as a result, we identified the superior temporal gyrus, known primarily for auditory processing. Interestingly, we did not observe any significant activation in regions associated with somatosensory or motor functions. This indicates that the brain dedicates more attention to auditory cues than to tactile cues, possibly due to the unfamiliarity of conveying the sensation of stickiness through sound. Our results could provide new perspectives on the complexities of multisensory integration, highlighting the subtle yet significant role of auditory processing in understanding tactile properties such as stickiness.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Humanos , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Encéfalo/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal , Percepción Visual/fisiología
10.
J Neurosci ; 44(10)2024 Mar 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38199864

RESUMEN

During communication in real-life settings, our brain often needs to integrate auditory and visual information and at the same time actively focus on the relevant sources of information, while ignoring interference from irrelevant events. The interaction between integration and attention processes remains poorly understood. Here, we use rapid invisible frequency tagging and magnetoencephalography to investigate how attention affects auditory and visual information processing and integration, during multimodal communication. We presented human participants (male and female) with videos of an actress uttering action verbs (auditory; tagged at 58 Hz) accompanied by two movie clips of hand gestures on both sides of fixation (attended stimulus tagged at 65 Hz; unattended stimulus tagged at 63 Hz). Integration difficulty was manipulated by a lower-order auditory factor (clear/degraded speech) and a higher-order visual semantic factor (matching/mismatching gesture). We observed an enhanced neural response to the attended visual information during degraded speech compared to clear speech. For the unattended information, the neural response to mismatching gestures was enhanced compared to matching gestures. Furthermore, signal power at the intermodulation frequencies of the frequency tags, indexing nonlinear signal interactions, was enhanced in the left frontotemporal and frontal regions. Focusing on the left inferior frontal gyrus, this enhancement was specific for the attended information, for those trials that benefitted from integration with a matching gesture. Together, our results suggest that attention modulates audiovisual processing and interaction, depending on the congruence and quality of the sensory input.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Percepción del Habla , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Encéfalo/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Magnetoencefalografía , Habla/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Estimulación Luminosa
11.
J Sports Med Phys Fitness ; 64(3): 301-310, 2024 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38261333

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: This systematic review aimed to analyze the available body of published peer-reviewed studies on the effects of combat sports compared with active/passive control on cognitive function and electrophysiological markers of brain activity in older people. EVIDENCE ACQUISITION: The studies were searched in Scopus, Web of Science, PubMed, MEDLINE, and PsycINFO databases from deadline to June 2023. The PRISMA, TESTEX, RoB, and GRADE scales assessed the evidence's methodological quality and certainty of evidence. The protocol was registered in PROSPERO (code: CRD42022361695). EVIDENCE SYNTHESIS: After reviewing 3768 studies, seven combat sports interventions (score ≥60% in methodological quality) were selected, composed of 381 older people (63% female), with a mean age of 66 years. In the selected studies, interventions based on judo, karate, and taekwondo were carried out, where it was not possible to verify the benefits of combat sports in cognitive function and electrophysiological markers of brain activity regarding active/passive control groups, although the individual results of the analyzed studies indicate that the practice of combat sports favor selective attention, divided attention, executive function, visual perception, and cognitive processing speed in older people. CONCLUSIONS: The available evidence does not allow a definite recommendation regarding combat sports as an effective cognitive function intervention in older people.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Artes Marciales , Anciano , Humanos , Función Ejecutiva , Artes Marciales/fisiología , Percepción Visual
12.
Phys Life Rev ; 48: 113-131, 2024 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38217888

RESUMEN

Theories of Visual Mental Imagery (VMI) emphasize the processes of retrieval, modification, and recombination of sensory information from long-term memory. Yet, only few studies have focused on the behavioral mechanisms and neural correlates supporting VMI of stimuli from different semantic domains. Therefore, we currently have a limited understanding of how the brain generates and maintains mental representations of colors, faces, shapes - to name a few. Such an undetermined scenario renders unclear the organizational structure of neural circuits supporting VMI, including the role of the early visual cortex. We aimed to fill this gap by reviewing the scientific literature of five semantic domains: visuospatial, face, colors, shapes, and letters imagery. Linking theory to evidence from over 60 different experimental designs, this review highlights three main points. First, there is no consistent activity in the early visual cortex across all VMI domains, contrary to the prediction of the dominant model. Second, there is consistent activity of the frontoparietal networks and the left hemisphere's fusiform gyrus during voluntary VMI irrespective of the semantic domain investigated. We propose that these structures are part of a domain-general VMI sub-network. Third, domain-specific information engages specific regions of the ventral and dorsal cortical visual pathways. These regions partly overlap with those found in visual perception studies (e.g., fusiform face area for faces imagery; lingual gyrus for color imagery). Altogether, the reviewed evidence suggests the existence of domain-general and domain-specific mechanisms of VMI selectively engaged by stimulus-specific properties (e.g., colors or faces). These mechanisms would be supported by an organizational structure mixing vertical and horizontal connections (heterarchy) between sub-networks for specific stimulus domains. Such a heterarchical organization of VMI makes different predictions from current models of VMI as reversed perception. Our conclusions set the stage for future research, which should aim to characterize the spatiotemporal dynamics and interactions among key regions of this architecture giving rise to visual mental images.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Encéfalo , Percepción Visual , Lóbulo Temporal , Lóbulo Occipital
13.
Commun Biol ; 7(1): 118, 2024 01 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38253781

RESUMEN

Neuroscientific research has consistently shown more extensive non-visual activity in the visual cortex of congenitally blind humans compared to sighted controls; a phenomenon known as crossmodal plasticity. Whether or not crossmodal activation of the visual cortex retracts if sight can be restored is still unknown. The present study, involving a rare group of sight-recovery individuals who were born pattern vision blind, employed visual event-related potentials to investigate persisting crossmodal modulation of the initial visual cortical processing stages. Here we report that the earliest, stimulus-driven retinotopic visual cortical activity (<100 ms) was suppressed in a spatially specific manner in sight-recovery individuals when concomitant sounds accompanied visual stimulation. In contrast, sounds did not modulate the earliest visual cortical response in two groups of typically sighted controls, nor in a third control group of sight-recovery individuals who had suffered a transient phase of later (rather than congenital) visual impairment. These results provide strong evidence for persisting crossmodal activity in the visual cortex after sight recovery following a period of congenital visual deprivation. Based on the time course of this modulation, we speculate on a role of exuberant crossmodal thalamic input which may arise during a sensitive phase of brain development.


Asunto(s)
Ceguera , Corteza Visual , Humanos , Percepción Visual , Sonido , Tálamo
14.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 86(1): 22-27, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36627474

RESUMEN

Previous research demonstrated effects of visual imagery on search speed in visual search paradigms. However, these effects were rather small, questioning their ecological validity. Thus, our present study aimed to generalize these effects to more naturalistic material (i.e., a paradigm that allows for top-down strategies in highly complex visual search displays that include overlapping stimuli while simultaneously avoiding possibly confounding search instructions). One hundred and four participants with aphantasia (= absence of voluntary mental imagery) and 104 gender and age-matched controls were asked to find hidden objects in several hidden object pictures with search times recorded. Results showed that people with aphantasia were significantly slower than controls, even when controlling for age and general processing speed. Thus, effects of visual imagery might be strong enough to influence the perception of our real-life surroundings, probably because of the involvement of visual imagery in several top-down strategies.


Asunto(s)
Imaginación , Percepción Visual , Humanos
15.
Conscious Cogn ; 117: 103598, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38086154

RESUMEN

Little is known about the perceptual characteristics of mental images nor how they vary across sensory modalities. We conducted an exhaustive survey into how mental images are experienced across modalities, mainly targeting visual and auditory imagery of a single stimulus, the letter "O", to facilitate direct comparisons. We investigated temporal properties of mental images (e.g. onset latency, duration), spatial properties (e.g. apparent location), effort (e.g. ease, spontaneity, control), movement requirements (e.g. eye movements), real-imagined interactions (e.g. inner speech while reading), beliefs about imagery norms and terminologies, as well as respondent confidence. Participants also reported on the five traditional senses and their prominence during thinking, imagining, and dreaming. Overall, visual and auditory experiences dominated mental events, although auditory mental images were superior to visual mental images on almost every metric tested except regarding spatial properties. Our findings suggest that modality-specific differences in mental imagery may parallel those of other sensory neural processes.


Asunto(s)
Imaginación , Sensación , Humanos , Percepción Visual , Imágenes en Psicoterapia , Percepción Auditiva
16.
Perception ; 53(1): 31-43, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37872670

RESUMEN

We present an experimental research aiming to explore how spatial attention may be biased through auditory stimuli. In particular, we investigate how synchronous sound and image may affect attention and increase the saliency of the audiovisual event. We have designed and implemented an experimental study where subjects, wearing an eye-tracking system, were examined regarding their gaze toward the audiovisual stimuli being displayed. The audiovisual stimuli were specifically tailored for this experiment, consisting of videos contrasting in terms of Synch Points (i.e., moments where a visual event is associated with a visible trigger movement, synchronous with its correspondent sound). While consistency across audiovisual sensory modalities revealed to be an attention-drawing feature, when combined with synchrony, it clearly emphasized the biasing, triggering orienting, that is, focal attention towards the particular scene that contains the Synch Point. Consequently, results revealed synchrony to be a saliency factor, contributing to the strengthening of the focal attention.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Sonido , Movimiento , Tecnología de Seguimiento Ocular , Estimulación Acústica , Estimulación Luminosa
17.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 36(2): 272-289, 2024 Feb 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38010290

RESUMEN

Mental imagery (MI) is the ability to generate visual phenomena in the absence of sensory input. MI is often likened to visual working memory (VWM): the ability to maintain and manipulate visual representations. How MI is recruited during VWM is yet to be established. In a modified orientation change-discrimination task, we examined how behavioral (proportion correct) and neural (contralateral delay activity [CDA]) correlates of precision and capacity map onto subjective ratings of vividness and number of items in MI within a VWM task. During the maintenance period, 17 participants estimated the vividness of their MI or the number of items held in MI while they were instructed to focus on either precision or capacity of their representation and to retain stimuli at varying set sizes (1, 2, and 4). Vividness and number ratings varied over set sizes; however, subjective ratings and behavioral performance correlated only for vividness rating at set size 1. Although CDA responded to set size as was expected, CDA did not reflect subjective reports on high and low vividness and on nondivergent (reported the probed number of items in mind) or divergent (reported number of items diverged from probed) rating trials. Participants were more accurate in low set sizes compared with higher set sizes and in coarse (45°) orientation changes compared with fine (15°) orientation changes. We failed to find evidence for a relationship between the subjective sensory experience of precision and capacity of MI and the precision and capacity of VWM.


Asunto(s)
Memoria a Corto Plazo , Metacognición , Humanos , Percepción Visual
18.
Neurosci Res ; 201: 50-59, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38029861

RESUMEN

Cognitive neuroscience research on mental imagery has largely focused on the visual imagery modality in unimodal task contexts. Recent studies have uncovered striking individual differences in visual imagery capacity, with some individuals reporting a subjective absence of conscious visual imagery ability altogether ("aphantasia"). However, naturalistic mental imagery is often multi-sensory, and preliminary findings suggest that many individuals with aphantasia also report a subjective lack of mental imagery in other sensory domains (such as auditory or olfactory imagery). In this paper, we perform a series of cluster analyses on the multi-sensory imagery questionnaire scores of two large groups of aphantasic subjects, defining latent sub-groups in this sample population. We demonstrate that aphantasia is a heterogenous phenomenon characterised by dominant sub-groups of individuals with visual aphantasia (those who report selective visual imagery absence) and multi-sensory aphantasia (those who report an inability to generate conscious mental imagery in any sensory modality). We replicate our findings in a second large sample and show that more unique aphantasia sub-types also exist, such as individuals with selectively preserved mental imagery in only one sensory modality (e.g. intact auditory imagery). We outline the implications of our findings for network theories of mental imagery, discussing how unique aphantasia aetiologies with distinct self-report patterns might reveal alterations to various levels of the sensory processing hierarchy implicated in mental imagery.


Asunto(s)
Imaginación , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Imágenes en Psicoterapia
19.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 86(1): 49-61, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37872433

RESUMEN

Mental imagery attracts attention to imagery-matching stimuli. However, it remains unknown whether voluntarily imagined atypical color also attracts attention to a stimulus that matches the imagery when the imagined stimuli are color-diagnostic objects, which are strongly associated with typical color. This study investigated whether people can voluntarily imagine atypical colors of such objects and attend to imagery-matching stimuli. Participants in the imagery group were instructed to imagine an atypical color of the black-white objects according to the instructed color or voluntarily selected color, whereas participants in the control group were instructed to attend to the objects without any instruction of imagery. Thereafter, they detected a color target in a visual search task. Results revealed that participants in the imagery group directed attention to the imagery-matching atypical color, not to the original color of the object in the search. Meanwhile, participants in the control group did not demonstrate any attentional guidance. These results suggest that voluntarily imagining atypical color can attenuate mental representations of the original color imagery and change attention to a stimulus that matches imagery.


Asunto(s)
Imaginación , Percepción Visual , Humanos
20.
Cortex ; 170: 26-31, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37926612

RESUMEN

The famous "Piazza del Duomo" paper, published in Cortex in 1978, inspired a considerable amount of research on visual mental imagery in brain-damaged patients. As a consequence, single-case reports featuring dissociations between perceptual and imagery abilities challenged the prevailing model of visual mental imagery. Here we focus on mental imagery for colors. A case study published in Cortex showed perfectly preserved color imagery in a patient with acquired achromatopsia after bilateral lesions at the borders between the occipital and temporal cortex. Subsequent neuroimaging findings in healthy participants extended and specified this result; color imagery elicited activation in both a domain-general region located in the left fusiform gyrus and the anterior color-biased patch within the ventral temporal cortex, but not in more posterior color-biased patches. Detailed studies of individual neurological patients, as those often published in Cortex, are still critical to inspire and constrain neurocognitive research and its theoretical models.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Encefálicas , Imaginación , Humanos , Imaginación/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral , Imágenes en Psicoterapia , Percepción Visual/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA