Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 6 de 6
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
Tipo de documento
País de afiliação
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 129(1): 190-7, 2008 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18620329

RESUMO

Do false beliefs last? To explore this question, this study planted false beliefs or memories of a childhood experience with asparagus. We found that these false beliefs had consequences for subjects, when assessed directly after the suggestive manipulation. Moreover, subjects were brought back two weeks later to see if their false beliefs persisted. After two weeks, subjects' confidence in their new memories, and the consequences of those memories were diminished, but not extinguished. These false beliefs were found to be somewhat weaker than other subjects' true beliefs for the same events. Another novel finding was that the manipulation was sufficiently powerful to affect actual food choices.


Assuntos
Cultura , Repressão Psicológica , Retenção Psicológica , Sugestão , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Inquéritos e Questionários
2.
Memory ; 16(5): 500-16, 2008.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18569679

RESUMO

Many people believe that emotional memories (including those that arise in therapy) are particularly likely to represent true events because of their emotional content. But is emotional content a reliable indicator of memory accuracy? The current research assessed the emotional content of participants' pre-existing (true) and manipulated (false) memories for childhood events. False memories for one of three emotional childhood events were planted using a suggestive manipulation and then compared, along several subjective dimensions, with other participants' true memories. On most emotional dimensions (e.g., how emotional was this event for you?), true and false memories were indistinguishable. On a few measures (e.g., intensity of feelings at the time of the event), true memories were more emotional than false memories in the aggregate, yet true and false memories were equally likely to be rated as uniformly emotional. These results suggest that even substantial emotional content may not reliably indicate memory accuracy.


Assuntos
Emoções/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Repressão Psicológica , Sugestão , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Imaginação , Lactente , Acontecimentos que Mudam a Vida , Masculino , Inquéritos e Questionários
3.
Dev Psychol ; 44(3): 681-94, 2008 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18473636

RESUMO

Two experiments investigated the effects of sadness, anger, and happiness on 4- to 6-year-old children's memory and suggestibility concerning story events. In Experiment 1, children were presented with 3 interactive stories on a video monitor. The stories included protagonists who wanted to give the child a prize. After each story, the child completed a task to try to win the prize. The outcome of the child's effort was manipulated in order to elicit sadness, anger, or happiness. Children's emotions did not affect story recall, but children were more vulnerable to misleading questions about the stories when sad than when angry or happy. In Experiment 2, a story was presented and emotions were elicited using an autobiographical recall task. Children responded to misleading questions and then recalled the story for a different interviewer. Again, children's emotions did not affect the amount of story information recalled correctly, but sad children incorporated more information from misleading questions during recall than did angry or happy children. Sad children's greater suggestibility is discussed in terms of the differing problem-solving strategies associated with discrete emotions.


Assuntos
Emoções , Rememoração Mental , Sugestão , Ira , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Felicidade , Humanos , Masculino
4.
Psychol Res ; 72(4): 362-75, 2008 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17676336

RESUMO

In past research, we planted false memories for food related childhood events using a simple false feedback procedure. Some critics have worried that our findings may be due to demand characteristics. In the present studies, we developed a novel procedure designed to reduce the influence of demand characteristics by providing an alternate magnet for subjects' natural suspicions. We used two separate levels of deception. In addition to giving subjects a typical untrue rationale for the study (i.e., normal deceptive cover story), we built in strong indicators (the "Red Herring") that the study actually had another purpose. Later, we told subjects that we had deceived them, and asked what they believed the "real purpose" of the study was. We also interviewed a subset of subjects in depth in order to analyze their subjective experiences of the procedure and any relevant demand. Our Red Herring successfully tricked subjects, and left little worry that our false memory results were due to demand. This "double cross" technique may have widespread uses in psychological research that hopes to conceal its real hypotheses from experimental subjects.


Assuntos
Atenção , Cultura , Enganação , Repressão Psicológica , Sugestão , Criança , Retroalimentação Psicológica , Comportamento Alimentar , Transtornos da Alimentação e da Ingestão de Alimentos/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Rememoração Mental , Obesidade/psicologia , Projetos de Pesquisa , Estudantes/psicologia , Adulto Jovem
5.
Am J Psychol ; 119(2): 255-74, 2006.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16841781

RESUMO

When given suggestive information, some people can be led to believe that they had experiences that they did not actually have. For example, they may come to believe falsely that they got sick eating particular foods as children, and as a result of that belief they may avoid the foods. But how do we know that someone has developed a false belief or memory in this research? The criteria we choose when classifying whether someone has fallen for the suggestion are somewhat arbitrary. We reanalyze our prior data, using different criteria for deciding that a person fell for the suggestion (called a "believer") or did not (called a "non-believer"). Changing criteria obviously affects the percentage of people who are called susceptible and could conceivably affect the conclusions reached about that group. Comparisons between false memories and true memories could differ, too, depending on how memories are defined.


Assuntos
Cultura , Enganação , Memória , Humanos , Sugestão
6.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 102(39): 13724-31, 2005 Sep 27.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16079200

RESUMO

We suggested to 228 subjects in two experiments that, as children, they had had negative experiences with a fattening food. An additional 107 subjects received no such suggestion and served as controls. In Experiment 1, a minority of subjects came to believe that they had felt ill after eating strawberry ice cream as children, and these subjects were more likely to indicate not wanting to eat strawberry ice cream now. In contrast, we were unable to obtain these effects when the critical item was a more commonly eaten treat (chocolate chip cookie). In Experiment 2, we replicated and extended the strawberry ice cream results. Two different ways of processing the false suggestion succeeded in planting the false belief and producing avoidance of the food. These findings show that it is possible to convince people that, as children, they experienced a negative event involving a fattening food and that this false belief results in avoidance of that food in adulthood. More broadly, these results indicate that we can, through suggestion, manipulate nutritional selection and possibly even improve health.


Assuntos
Gorduras na Dieta/administração & dosagem , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Memória , Sugestão , Cultura , Humanos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA