Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 5 de 5
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
Tipo de documento
País de afiliação
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Brain Topogr ; 31(2): 153-160, 2018 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29127530

RESUMO

The combination of frequency-tagging with electroencephalography (EEG) has recently proved fruitful for understanding the perception of beat and meter in musical rhythm, a common behavior shared by humans of all cultures. EEG frequency-tagging allows the objective measurement of input-output transforms to investigate beat perception, its modulation by exogenous and endogenous factors, development, and neural basis. Recent doubt has been raised about the validity of comparing frequency-domain representations of auditory rhythmic stimuli and corresponding EEG responses, assuming that it implies a one-to-one mapping between the envelope of the rhythmic input and the neural output, and that it neglects the sensitivity of frequency-domain representations to acoustic features making up the rhythms. Here we argue that these elements actually reinforce the strengths of the approach. The obvious fact that acoustic features influence the frequency spectrum of the sound envelope precisely justifies taking into consideration the sounds used to generate a beat percept for interpreting neural responses to auditory rhythms. Most importantly, the many-to-one relationship between rhythmic input and perceived beat actually validates an approach that objectively measures the input-output transforms underlying the perceptual categorization of rhythmic inputs. Hence, provided that a number of potential pitfalls and fallacies are avoided, EEG frequency-tagging to study input-output relationships appears valuable for understanding rhythm perception.


Assuntos
Córtex Auditivo/fisiologia , Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Eletroencefalografia , Música , Periodicidade , Estimulação Acústica , Humanos , Som
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 114(31): E6437-E6446, 2017 08 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28652333

RESUMO

Brain systems supporting face and voice processing both contribute to the extraction of important information for social interaction (e.g., person identity). How does the brain reorganize when one of these channels is absent? Here, we explore this question by combining behavioral and multimodal neuroimaging measures (magneto-encephalography and functional imaging) in a group of early deaf humans. We show enhanced selective neural response for faces and for individual face coding in a specific region of the auditory cortex that is typically specialized for voice perception in hearing individuals. In this region, selectivity to face signals emerges early in the visual processing hierarchy, shortly after typical face-selective responses in the ventral visual pathway. Functional and effective connectivity analyses suggest reorganization in long-range connections from early visual areas to the face-selective temporal area in individuals with early and profound deafness. Altogether, these observations demonstrate that regions that typically specialize for voice processing in the hearing brain preferentially reorganize for face processing in born-deaf people. Our results support the idea that cross-modal plasticity in the case of early sensory deprivation relates to the original functional specialization of the reorganized brain regions.


Assuntos
Córtex Auditivo/fisiologia , Surdez/fisiopatologia , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Plasticidade Neuronal/fisiologia , Vias Visuais/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Adulto , Mapeamento Encefálico , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Neuroimagem/métodos , Estimulação Luminosa , Privação Sensorial/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
3.
Brain Struct Funct ; 222(5): 2389-2404, 2017 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27990557

RESUMO

Musical entrainment is shared by all human cultures and the perception of a periodic beat is a cornerstone of this entrainment behavior. Here, we investigated whether beat perception might have its roots in the earliest stages of auditory cortical processing. Local field potentials were recorded from 8 patients implanted with depth-electrodes in Heschl's gyrus and the planum temporale (55 recording sites in total), usually considered as human primary and secondary auditory cortices. Using a frequency-tagging approach, we show that both low-frequency (<30 Hz) and high-frequency (>30 Hz) neural activities in these structures faithfully track auditory rhythms through frequency-locking to the rhythm envelope. A selective gain in amplitude of the response frequency-locked to the beat frequency was observed for the low-frequency activities but not for the high-frequency activities, and was sharper in the planum temporale, especially for the more challenging syncopated rhythm. Hence, this gain process is not systematic in all activities produced in these areas and depends on the complexity of the rhythmic input. Moreover, this gain was disrupted when the rhythm was presented at fast speed, revealing low-pass response properties which could account for the propensity to perceive a beat only within the musical tempo range. Together, these observations show that, even though part of these neural transforms of rhythms could already take place in subcortical auditory processes, the earliest auditory cortical processes shape the neural representation of rhythmic inputs in favor of the emergence of a periodic beat.


Assuntos
Córtex Auditivo/fisiologia , Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos/fisiologia , Periodicidade , Estimulação Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Cognição/fisiologia , Eletroencefalografia/métodos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Música , Adulto Jovem
4.
Perception ; 42(10): 1013-26, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24494433

RESUMO

Compelling evidence that faces are perceived holistically or configurally comes from the composite face illusion: identical top halves of a face are perceived as being different if they are aligned with different bottom halves. The visual illusion disappears when the top and bottom face halves are spatially misaligned. Whether this is because the two halves no longer form a whole face (ie they form two segmented parts), or because of an increase in interfeatures distance in the misaligned condition (eg eyes-mouth distance) remains unclear. Here, thirty-four participants performed a delayed matching composite task in which the amount of spatial misalignment between face halves varied parametrically (from 8.33% of face width to 100%). The difference in performance between aligned and misaligned faces (ie the composite face effect) was already of full magnitude at the smallest level of misalignment. These results imply that a small spatial misalignment is sufficient to measure the composite face effect. From a theoretical standpoint, they indicate that it is the breaking of a whole configuration rather than the increase in relative distance between the face parts that explains the presence or absence of the composite face effect, clarifying an outstanding issue concerning the nature of holistic face perception.


Assuntos
Face , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Adulto , Precisão da Medição Dimensional , Feminino , Fixação Ocular , Saúde Holística , Humanos , Masculino , Ilusões Ópticas/fisiologia , Período Refratário Psicológico/fisiologia , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas
5.
Perception ; 33(2): 217-36, 2004.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15109163

RESUMO

Theories of object recognition differ to the extent that they consider object representations as being mediated only by the shape of the object, or shape and surface details, if surface details are part of the representation. In particular, it has been suggested that color information may be helpful at recognizing objects only in very special cases, but not during basic-level object recognition in good viewing conditions. In this study, we collected normative data (naming agreement, familiarity, complexity, and imagery judgments) for Snodgrass and Vanderwart's object database of 260 black-and-white line drawings, and then compared the data to exactly the same shapes but with added gray-level texture and surface details (set 2), and color (set 3). Naming latencies were also recorded. Whereas the addition of texture and shading without color only slightly improved naming agreement scores for the objects, the addition of color information unambiguously improved naming accuracy and speeded correct response times. As shown in previous studies, the advantage provided by color was larger for objects with a diagnostic color, and structurally similar shapes, such as fruits and vegetables, but was also observed for man-made objects with and without a single diagnostic color. These observations show that basic-level 'everyday' object recognition in normal conditions is facilitated by the presence of color information, and support a 'shape + surface' model of object recognition, for which color is an integral part of the object representation. In addition, the new stimuli (sets 2 and 3) and the corresponding normative data provide valuable materials for a wide range of experimental and clinical studies of object recognition.


Assuntos
Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Animais , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Feminino , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Frutas , Humanos , Imaginação , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Psicofísica , Tempo de Reação , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Propriedades de Superfície , Verduras
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA