Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 294
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Multisens Res ; 36(6): 557-572, 2023 08 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37582513

RESUMO

Temporal perception in multisensory processing is important for an accurate and efficient understanding of the physical world. In general, it is executed in a dynamic environment in our daily lives. In particular, the motion-binding property is important for correctly identifying moving objects in the external environment. However, how this property affects multisensory temporal perception remains unclear. We investigate whether the motion-binding property influences audiovisual temporal integration. The study subjects performed four types of temporal-order judgment (TOJ) task experiments using three types of perception. In Experiment 1, the subjects conducted audiovisual TOJ tasks in the motion-binding condition, between two flashes, and in the simultaneous condition, in which the two flashes are perceived as simultaneous stimuli without motion. In Experiment 2, subjects conducted audiovisual TOJ tasks in the motion-binding condition and the short and long successive interval condition, in which the two stimuli are perceived as successive with no motion. The results revealed that the point of subjective simultaneity (PSS) and the just-noticeable difference (JND) in the motion-binding condition differed significantly from those in the simultaneous and short and long successive interval conditions. Specifically, the PSS in the motion-binding condition was shifted toward a sound-lead stimulus in which the PSS became closer to zero (i.e., physical simultaneity) and the JND became narrower compared to other conditions. This suggests that the motion-binding property contributes to accurate temporal integration in multisensory processing by precisely encoding the temporal order of the physical stimuli.


Assuntos
Percepção do Tempo , Percepção Visual , Humanos , Percepção Auditiva , Limiar Diferencial , Exame Físico , Julgamento , Estimulação Acústica , Estimulação Luminosa
2.
Annu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc ; 2020: 3877-3880, 2020 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33018847

RESUMO

Ultrasound can be clearly perceived by bone-conduction, and this "bone-conducted ultrasound (BCU)" can transmit speech information by using amplitude modulation (AM). Further, BCU can be perceived not only on the head but also on the distal parts of the body like the neck, trunk and arms. This "distantly-presented BCU" can be applied to the novel interface that can transmit sound information selectively to specific users who touches the vibrator. However, the ability to transmit sound information of distantly-presented BCU is unclear. First, to assess frequency discrimination ability, difference limens for frequency (DLFs) of the distantly-presented AM-BCU were measured with/without a low-pass masking noise that masked the self-demodulated components generated by the nonlinearity of biological tissues. DLFs comparable to that of air-conducted sounds were observed, whereas DLFs significantly increased above 1 kHz under the masking condition. These results suggest that practical frequency discrimination ability can be obtained even when BCUs were presented to distal body parts. Additionally, it is indicated that the demodulated components may contribute to transmitting frequency information above 1 kHz. Second, monosyllable articulation and word intelligibility tests were conducted in Japanese. The intelligibility and articulation at the neck were 55% and 38% respectively, whereas they decreased as the stimulus placement gets farther from the head. The results suggest the distantly-presented BCU device can be applied to transmission of speech information.


Assuntos
Condução Óssea , Fala , Estimulação Acústica , Limiar Diferencial , Ultrassonografia
3.
Int J Audiol ; 59(2): 140-147, 2020 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31584306

RESUMO

Objective: Assessments of the medial olivocochlear reflex (MOCR) may have clinical utility. The MOCR is measured using contralateral inhibition of otoacoustic emissions but concurrent activation of the middle ear muscle reflex (MEMR) confounds test interpretation. MEMR activation can be detected using the change in ear-canal stimulus amplitude without versus with an MOCR elicitor. This study provides a description of how critical differences in ear-canal stimulus amplitude can be established.Design: Clicks were presented in right ears without and with a contralateral MOCR elicitor. Ear-canal stimulus amplitudes were measured. Two measurements without an elicitor were used to develop critical differences. MEMR activation was considered present if the difference in ear-canal stimulus amplitude without versus with an elicitor exceeded the critical difference.Study sample: Forty-six normal-hearing adults (mean age = 23.4 years, 35 females) participated, with data from 44 participants included in the final analysis.Results: Two participants exceeded the 95% critical difference. The 80, 90 and 99% critical differences are also reported for reference.Conclusions: Results suggest that the contralateral elicitor can evoke the MEMR in a small number of participants. The methods described in this paper can be used for developing equipment- and clinic-specific critical differences for detecting MEMR activation.


Assuntos
Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Limiar Auditivo/fisiologia , Orelha Média/fisiologia , Testes Auditivos/estatística & dados numéricos , Reflexo Acústico/fisiologia , Cóclea/fisiologia , Limiar Diferencial , Meato Acústico Externo/fisiologia , Feminino , Voluntários Saudáveis , Humanos , Masculino , Músculo Esquelético/fisiologia , Emissões Otoacústicas Espontâneas/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
4.
Int Tinnitus J ; 22(1): 1-9, 2018 Jun 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29993210

RESUMO

Animal models of tinnitus rely on interpretation of behavioural or reflexive tests to determine the presence of this phantom perception. A commonly used test is the gap prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle (GPIAS), which is often combined with prepulse inhibition (PPI) to ensure that reduced GPIAS suppression is not due to hearing loss caused by the acoustic trauma commonly used to trigger tinnitus development. In our laboratory GPIAS and PPI are routinely used on two colonies of outbred tri-colour guinea pigs. However, our results show that these colonies show divergent results even before any tinnitus-inducing treatment, which impacts their suitability in tinnitus models. Although colony 1 and 2 show similar results in PPI (~95% of animals showing significant suppression), only ~30% of colony 2 also shows significant suppression in GPIAS compared to ~75% of colony 1. Cochlear sensitivity measured using compound action potentials showed no significant differences between colonies. Therefore, peripheral threshold loss was excluded as a possible factor. Our results show that similar strains of laboratory animals can show highly divergent results and GPIAS testing for tinnitus will not work for every animal strain. In addition, our data support the notion that PPI and GPIAS responses may rely on different neural circuitry.


Assuntos
Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Cóclea/fisiopatologia , Inibição Pré-Pulso/fisiologia , Reflexo de Sobressalto/fisiologia , Zumbido/fisiopatologia , Estimulação Acústica , Animais , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Cobaias , Modelos Animais , Zumbido/etiologia
5.
Med Biol Eng Comput ; 56(12): 2213-2219, 2018 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29949020

RESUMO

There is multiple evidence in the literature that a sub-threshold pre-pulse, delivered immediately prior to an electrical stimulation pulse, can alter the activation threshold of nerve fibers and motor unit recruitment characteristics. So far, previously published works combined monophasic stimuli with sub-threshold depolarizing pre-pulses (DPPs) with inconsistent findings-in some studies, the DPPs decreased the activation threshold, while in others it was increased. This work aimed to evaluate the effect of DPPs during biphasic transcutaneous electrical stimulation and to study the possible mechanism underlying those differences. Sub-threshold DPPs between 0.5 and 15 ms immediately followed by biphasic or monophasic pulses were administered to the tibial nerve; the electrophysiological muscular responses (motor-wave, M-wave) were monitored via electromyogram (EMG) recording from the soleus muscle. The data show that, under the specific studied conditions, DPPs tend to lower the threshold for nerve fiber activation rather than elevating it. DPPs with the same polarity as the leading phase of biphasic stimuli are more effective to increase the sensitivity. This work assesses for the first time the effect of DPPs on biphasic pulses, which are required to achieve charge-balanced stimulation, and it provides guidance on the effect of polarity and intensity to take full advantage of this feature. Graphical abstract In this work, the effect of sub-threshold depolarizing pre-pulses (DPP) is investigated in a setup with transcutaneous electrical stimulation. We found that, within the tested 0-15 ms DPP duration range, the DPPs administered immediately before biphasic pulses proportionally increase the nerve excitability as visible in the M-waves recorded from the soleus muscle. Interestingly, these findings oppose published results, where DPPs, administered immediately before monophasic stimuli via implanted electrodes, led to decrease of nerve excitability.


Assuntos
Estimulação Elétrica Nervosa Transcutânea/métodos , Adulto , Limiar Diferencial , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Nervo Tibial/fisiologia
6.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 143(4): 2355, 2018 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29716248

RESUMO

Performance was measured on seven common psychoacoustical tasks for about 75 highly trained subjects. Because some psychoacoustical outcomes varied by race, the subjects were partitioned into White and Non-White categories for analysis. Sex, race, and menstrual-cycle differences in performance are described in a companion paper [McFadden, Pasanen, Maloney, Leshikar, and Pho (2018). J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 143, 2338-2354]. Also measured for all subjects were three types of otoacoustic emissions (OAEs): spontaneous otoacoustic emissions (SOAEs), click-evoked otoacoustic emissions (CEOAEs), and distortion-product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs). The experimental question was whether and how OAEs were correlated with psychoacoustical performance. In accord with past findings, the SOAEs and CEOAEs exhibited substantial sex and race differences, but the DPOAEs did not. Somewhat surprisingly, the correlations between OAEs and psychoacoustical performance were generally weak. No form of OAE was highly correlated with any psychoacoustical task for both sexes within a race category. Thus, there was no compelling evidence that the mechanisms underlying OAEs also contribute systematically to performance in any of the simultaneous or temporal masking tasks studied here. Especially surprising were the weak correlations between OAEs and detection of a tone in the quiet. Apparently individual differences in psychoacoustical performance reside more in post-cochlear (neural) mechanisms than in individual differences in the cochlear ("mechanical") mechanisms underlying the OAEs measured here.


Assuntos
Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Vias Auditivas/fisiologia , Ciclo Menstrual , Emissões Otoacústicas Espontâneas , Psicoacústica , Grupos Raciais/estatística & dados numéricos , Adulto , Audiometria , Limiar Diferencial , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais , Adulto Jovem
7.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 143(4): 2338, 2018 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29716303

RESUMO

The psychoacoustical literature contains multiple reports about small differences in performance depending upon the sex and phase of the menstrual cycle of the subjects. In an attempt to verify these past reports, a large-scale study was implemented. After extensive training, the performance of about 75 listeners was measured on seven common psychoacoustical tasks. For most tasks, the signal was a 3.0-kHz tone. The initial data analyses failed to confirm some past outcomes. Additional analyses, incorporating the limited information available about the racial background of the listeners, did confirm some of the past reports, with the direction and magnitude of the differences often diverging for the White and Non-White listeners. Sex differences and race differences interacted for six of the seven tasks studied. These interactions suggest that racial background needs to be considered when making generalizations about human auditory performance, and when considering failures of reproducibility across studies. Menstrual differences were small, but generally larger for Whites than Non-Whites. Hormonal effects may be responsible for the sex and cycle differences that do exist, and differences in intra-cochlear melanocytes may account for the race differences.


Assuntos
Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Vias Auditivas/fisiologia , Ciclo Menstrual , Psicoacústica , Grupos Raciais/estatística & dados numéricos , Adolescente , Adulto , Limiar Diferencial , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais , Adulto Jovem
8.
J Manipulative Physiol Ther ; 40(7): 511-516, 2017 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29079256

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: The aim of this study was to investigate if there were differences in the two-point discrimination (2-PD) of fingers among students at different stages of a chiropractic program. METHODS: This study measured 2-PD thresholds for the dominant and nondominant index finger and dominant and nondominant forearm in groups of students in a 4-year chiropractic program at the International Medical University in Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia. Measurements were made using digital calipers mounted on a modified weighing scale. Group comparisons were made among students for each year of the program (years 1, 2, 3, and 4). Analysis of the 2-PD threshold for differences among the year groups was performed with analysis of variance. RESULTS: The mean 2-PD threshold of the index finger was higher in the students who were in the higher year groups. Dominant-hand mean values for year 1 were 2.93 ± 0.04 mm and 1.69 ± 0.02 mm in year 4. There were significant differences at finger sites (P < .05) among all year groups compared with year 1. There were no significant differences measured at the dominant forearm between any year groups (P = .08). The nondominant fingers of the year groups 1, 2, and 4 showed better 2-PD compared with the dominant finger. There was a significant difference (P = .005) between the nondominant (1.93 ± 1.15) and dominant (2.27 ± 1.14) fingers when all groups were combined (n = 104). CONCLUSIONS: The results of this study demonstrated that the finger 2-PD of the chiropractic students later in the program was more precise than that of students in the earlier program.


Assuntos
Quiroprática/educação , Competência Clínica , Limiar Sensorial , Tato/fisiologia , Limiar Diferencial , Dedos/fisiologia , Humanos , Malásia , Masculino , Palpação/métodos , Estudantes de Ciências da Saúde , Adulto Jovem
9.
J Pharmacol Sci ; 134(4): 225-233, 2017 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28826625

RESUMO

We sought to determine the preventive effects of curcumin and its highly bioavailable preparation on noise-induced hearing loss in a novel murine model of permanent hearing loss developed by repeated exposure to noise. Upon exposure to noise (8-kHz octave band noise, 90 dB sound pressure level, 1 h), hearing ability was impaired in a temporary and reversible manner. During repeated noise exposure (1-h exposure per day, 5 days), there was a progressive increase in the auditory threshold shift at 12 and 20 kHz. The threshold shift persisted for at least 6 days after noise exposure. Oral administration of curcumin for 3 days before and each day during noise exposure significantly alleviated the hearing loss induced by repeated noise exposure. Curcumin abolished intranuclear translocation of nuclear factor-κB-p65 and generation of 4-hydroxynonenal-adducted proteins found in the cochlea after noise exposure. Theracurmin®, a highly absorbable and bioavailable preparation of curcumin, had strong preventive effects on hearing loss induced by repeated noise exposure. Together, these data suggest that curcumin exerts a preventive effect on noise-induced hearing loss and is therefore a good therapeutic candidate for preventing sensorineural hearing loss.


Assuntos
Curcumina/administração & dosagem , Exposição Ambiental/efeitos adversos , Perda Auditiva Neurossensorial/etiologia , Perda Auditiva Neurossensorial/prevenção & controle , Ruído/efeitos adversos , Fitoterapia , Transporte Ativo do Núcleo Celular/efeitos dos fármacos , Administração Oftálmica , Aldeídos/metabolismo , Animais , Disponibilidade Biológica , Cóclea/metabolismo , Curcumina/farmacologia , Limiar Diferencial , Modelos Animais de Doenças , Formas de Dosagem , Audição/fisiologia , Perda Auditiva Neurossensorial/metabolismo , Perda Auditiva Neurossensorial/fisiopatologia , Camundongos Endogâmicos , Fator de Transcrição RelA/metabolismo
10.
J Neural Eng ; 14(2): 026004, 2017 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28155848

RESUMO

To further improve the quality of visual percepts elicited by microelectronic retinal prosthetics, substantial efforts have been made to understand how retinal neurons respond to electrical stimulation. It is generally assumed that a sufficiently strong stimulus will recruit most retinal neurons. However, recent evidence has shown that the responses of some retinal neurons decrease with excessively strong stimuli (a non-monotonic response function). Therefore, it is necessary to identify stimuli that can be used to activate the majority of retinal neurons even when such non-monotonic cells are part of the neuronal population. Taking these non-monotonic responses into consideration, we establish the optimal voltage stimulation parameters (amplitude, duration, and polarity) for epiretinal stimulation of network-mediated (indirect) ganglion cell responses. We recorded responses from 3958 mouse retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) in both healthy (wild type, WT) and a degenerating (rd10) mouse model of retinitis pigmentosa-using flat-mounted retina on a microelectrode array. Rectangular monophasic voltage-controlled pulses were presented with varying voltage, duration, and polarity. We found that in 4-5 weeks old rd10 mice the RGC thresholds were comparable to those of WT. There was a marked response variability among mouse RGCs. To account for this variability, we interpolated the percentage of RGCs activated at each point in the voltage-polarity-duration stimulus space, thus identifying the optimal voltage-controlled pulse (-2.4 V, 0.88 ms). The identified optimal voltage pulse can activate at least 65% of potentially responsive RGCs in both mouse strains. Furthermore, this pulse is well within the range of stimuli demonstrated to be safe and effective for retinal implant patients. Such optimized stimuli and the underlying method used to identify them support a high yield of responsive RGCs and will serve as an effective guideline for future in vitro investigations of retinal electrostimulation by establishing standard stimuli for each unique experimental condition.


Assuntos
Potenciais de Ação , Estimulação Elétrica/métodos , Potenciais Evocados , Rede Nervosa/fisiopatologia , Células Ganglionares da Retina , Retinose Pigmentar/fisiopatologia , Animais , Limiar Diferencial , Feminino , Masculino , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Retinose Pigmentar/terapia
11.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 140(3): 1783, 2016 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27914394

RESUMO

Just-noticeable differences (JNDs) in interaural time delay (ITD), interaural level difference (ILD), and interaural cross-correlation (ICC) were measured with low- and high-frequency noise bands over multiple sessions for 10 normal-hearing (NH) and 11 hearing-impaired (HI) listeners. Individual subject thresholds tended to improve with training then stabilize. Measured JNDs varied over these experienced listeners, for both subject groups and all tasks. Group JNDs were seldom predictable from hearing level. Individual listeners' JNDs were highly correlated across frequency for each task and group, except for ICC in the HI listeners. Further, ITD JNDs almost always significantly correlated with ILD JNDs within a group. Finally, although the ICC JNDs always significantly correlated with the ITD or ILD JNDs for the NH listeners, they often did not for the HI listeners. These findings suggest that little information about binaural sensitivity is added for NH listeners with multiple ITD, ILD, and ICC measures. For HI listeners, however, while ITD and ILD measures are well correlated, information is added with ICC measures. In general, the results suggest that less information is added with JND measures for NH listeners (15 significant correlations) than for HI listeners (six significant correlations).


Assuntos
Perda Auditiva , Estimulação Acústica , Adulto , Limiar Auditivo , Limiar Diferencial , Feminino , Testes Auditivos , Humanos , Masculino , Ruído , Adulto Jovem
12.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 138(5): 3093-104, 2015 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26627783

RESUMO

The question of how frequency is coded in the peripheral auditory system remains unresolved. Previous research has suggested that slow rates of frequency modulation (FM) of a low carrier frequency may be coded via phase-locked temporal information in the auditory nerve, whereas FM at higher rates and/or high carrier frequencies may be coded via a rate-place (tonotopic) code. This hypothesis was tested in a cohort of 100 young normal-hearing listeners by comparing individual sensitivity to slow-rate (1-Hz) and fast-rate (20-Hz) FM at a carrier frequency of 500 Hz with independent measures of phase-locking (using dynamic interaural time difference, ITD, discrimination), level coding (using amplitude modulation, AM, detection), and frequency selectivity (using forward-masking patterns). All FM and AM thresholds were highly correlated with each other. However, no evidence was obtained for stronger correlations between measures thought to reflect phase-locking (e.g., slow-rate FM and ITD sensitivity), or between measures thought to reflect tonotopic coding (fast-rate FM and forward-masking patterns). The results suggest that either psychoacoustic performance in young normal-hearing listeners is not limited by peripheral coding, or that similar peripheral mechanisms limit both high- and low-rate FM coding.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Cóclea/fisiologia , Nervo Coclear/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Individualidade , Modelos Neurológicos , Estimulação Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Limiar Auditivo , Testes com Listas de Dissílabos , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Mascaramento Perceptivo , Periodicidade , Discriminação da Altura Tonal/fisiologia , Análise de Componente Principal , Psicoacústica , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
13.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 138(5): 3245-61, 2015 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26627798

RESUMO

Previous psychoacoustical and physiological studies indicate that the medial olivocochlear reflex (MOCR), a bilateral, sound-evoked reflex, may lead to improved sound intensity discrimination in background noise. The MOCR can decrease the range of basilar-membrane compression and can counteract effects of neural adaptation from background noise. However, the contribution of these processes to intensity discrimination is not well understood. This study examined the effect of ipsilateral, contralateral, and bilateral noise on the "mid-level hump." The mid-level hump refers to intensity discrimination Weber fractions (WFs) measured for short-duration, high-frequency tones which are poorer at mid levels than at lower or higher levels. The mid-level hump WFs may reflect a limitation due to basilar-membrane compression, and thus may be decreased by the MOCR. The noise was either short (50 ms) or long (150 ms), with the long noise intended to elicit the sluggish MOCR. For a tone in quiet, mid-level hump WFs improved with ipsilateral noise for most listeners, but not with contralateral noise. For a tone in ipsilateral noise, WFs improved with contralateral noise for most listeners, but only when both noises were long. These results are consistent with MOCR-induced WF improvements, possibly via decreases in effects of compression and neural adaptation.


Assuntos
Limiar Auditivo/fisiologia , Membrana Basilar/fisiologia , Cóclea/fisiologia , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Ruído , Núcleo Olivar/fisiologia , Detecção de Sinal Psicológico/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Dominância Cerebral , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicoacústica , Reflexo/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
14.
Int J Audiol ; 54(12): 918-23, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26446950

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To re-evaluate the current BSA recommendation that the test ear should be occluded during the bone-conduction procedure at frequencies above 2 kHz to prevent audible air-borne radiation. DESIGN: Pure-tone audiometry was undertaken during routine hearing tests. The audiograms of fifty-two ears met the criteria for the study and were included. Bone conduction at 4 kHz was tested in three different conditions: test ear open/occluded by earplug and occluded by circumaural earphone. STUDY SAMPLE: Forty-four adults aged 41-77 years with average hearing levels from normal to severe loss. All complied fully with the test procedure. No audiogram had a significant conductive element. RESULTS: There was no significant difference in each of the three test situations. Only two audiograms showed any (5 dB) difference at 4 kHz when bone conduction was retested with the ear occluded. CONCLUSIONS: The errors that result in a false air-bone gap at 4 kHz would not appear to be due to air-borne radiation. Failure to occlude the ear canal at 4 kHz, where air-borne radiation is greatest, makes no significant difference to the audiometric results. It is therefore suggested that it is unnecessary to block the test ear during routine pure-tone bone-conduction testing to prevent audible air-borne radiation, and that this should no longer form part of normal clinical practice.


Assuntos
Ar , Condução Óssea , Dispositivos de Proteção das Orelhas , Perda Auditiva/diagnóstico , Testes Auditivos/métodos , Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Adulto , Idoso , Audiometria de Tons Puros , Limiar Auditivo/fisiologia , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Meato Acústico Externo , Feminino , Testes Auditivos/instrumentação , Testes Auditivos/normas , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Oclusão Terapêutica
15.
Biol Cybern ; 109(4-5): 479-91, 2015 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26228799

RESUMO

Sensitization is an example of malfunctioning of the nociceptive pathway in either the peripheral or central nervous system. Using quantitative sensory testing, one can only infer sensitization, but not determine the defective subsystem. The states of the subsystems may be characterized using computational modeling together with experimental data. Here, we develop a neurophysiologically plausible model replicating experimental observations from a psychophysical human subject study. We study the effects of single temporal stimulus parameters on detection thresholds corresponding to a 0.5 detection probability. To model peripheral activation and central processing, we adapt a stochastic drift-diffusion model and a probabilistic hazard model to our experimental setting without reaction times. We retain six lumped parameters in both models characterizing peripheral and central mechanisms. Both models have similar psychophysical functions, but the hazard model is computationally more efficient. The model-based effects of temporal stimulus parameters on detection thresholds are consistent with those from human subject data.


Assuntos
Simulação por Computador , Modelos Biológicos , Fibras Nervosas/fisiologia , Peptídeos Opioides/fisiologia , Detecção de Sinal Psicológico/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Psicofísica , Tempo de Reação , Estimulação Elétrica Nervosa Transcutânea , Nociceptina
16.
Pacing Clin Electrophysiol ; 38(10): 1173-80, 2015 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26137999

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: There are several methods to induce ventricular fibrillation (VF) during defibrillation threshold (DFT) testing. Delivering a shock at a critical time during the T wave (T-shock) is the conventional approach, while delivering a constant direct current voltage (DC stim) from the implantable cardioverter defibrillator is an alternative method. Only a few reports compare VF induction methods. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the effects and safety of DC stim versus T-shock. METHODS: We retrospectively investigated 414 consecutive patients undergoing DFT testing. We compared the two groups (DC stim and T-shock) with respect to clinical characteristics, electrocardiogram (ECG) changes, and complications. RESULTS: Ventricular arrhythmia, including ventricular tachycardia (VT) and VF, was induced by DC stim in 93 patients or T-shock in 321 patients. No more than three attempts were performed during one procedure. There was no significant difference in the baseline ECG, induced tachycardia cycle length (TCL), or complications between the two groups. However, the induced TCL was significantly shorter than the clinical TCL regardless of induction method (P = 0.001). Five patients suffered major complications (i.e., electromechanical dissociation or incessant VT). A history of atrial fibrillation was significantly greater in patients with major complications than the others (80% vs 24%, P = 0.004), and was an independent predictor on multivariate analysis. CONCLUSIONS: There is no significant difference in induced TCL or complications between the DC stim and T-shock. The induced TCL is significantly shorter than clinical TCL regardless of induction method.


Assuntos
Desfibriladores Implantáveis/estatística & dados numéricos , Estimulação Elétrica/métodos , Técnicas Eletrofisiológicas Cardíacas/estatística & dados numéricos , Disfunção Ventricular Esquerda/epidemiologia , Fibrilação Ventricular/diagnóstico , Fibrilação Ventricular/prevenção & controle , Adulto , Distribuição por Idade , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Limiar Diferencial , Estimulação Elétrica/efeitos adversos , Técnicas Eletrofisiológicas Cardíacas/efeitos adversos , Técnicas Eletrofisiológicas Cardíacas/métodos , Feminino , Humanos , Incidência , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Prevalência , Estudos Retrospectivos , São Francisco/epidemiologia , Distribuição por Sexo , Resultado do Tratamento , Disfunção Ventricular Esquerda/etiologia , Fibrilação Ventricular/epidemiologia
17.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 137(1): 388-96, 2015 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25618068

RESUMO

This study measured listener sensitivity to increments of a target inter-onset interval (IOI) embedded within tone sequences that featured different rhythmic patterns. The sequences consisted of six 50-ms 1000-Hz tone bursts separated by silent intervals that were adjusted to create different timing patterns. Control sequences were isochronous, with all tonal IOIs fixed at either 200 or 400 ms, while other patterns featured combinations of the two IOIs arranged to create different sequential tonal groupings. Duration difference limens in milliseconds for increments of a single sequence IOI were measured adaptively by adjusting the duration of an inter-tone silent interval. Specific target IOIs within sequences differed across discrimination conditions. Listeners included younger normal-hearing adults and groups of older adults with and without hearing loss. Discrimination performance measured for each of the older groups of listeners was observed to be equivalent, with each group exhibiting significantly poorer discrimination performance than the younger listeners in each sequence condition. Additionally, the specific influence of variable rhythmic grouping on temporal sensitivity was found to be greatest among older listeners.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Audiometria , Limiar Auditivo/fisiologia , Cóclea/fisiopatologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Feminino , Perda Auditiva Neurossensorial/fisiopatologia , Perda Auditiva Neurossensorial/psicologia , Humanos , Masculino , Discriminação da Altura Tonal/fisiologia , Presbiacusia/fisiopatologia , Presbiacusia/psicologia , Desempenho Psicomotor , Reflexo Acústico , Adulto Jovem
18.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 136(6): 3172, 2014 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25480064

RESUMO

Real-world auditory stimuli are highly variable across occurrences and sources. The present study examined the sensitivity of human listeners to differences in global stimulus variability. In a two-interval, forced-choice task, variance discrimination was measured using sequences of five 100-ms tone pulses. The frequency of each pulse was sampled randomly from a distribution that was Gaussian in logarithmic frequency. In the non-signal interval, the sampled distribution had a variance of σSTAN (2), while in the signal interval, the variance of the sequence was σSIG (2) (with σSIG (2) > σSTAN (2)). The listener's task was to choose the interval with the larger variance. To constrain possible decision strategies, the mean frequency of the sampling distribution of each interval was randomly chosen for each presentation. Psychometric functions were measured for various values of σSTAN (2). Although the performance was remarkably similar across listeners, overall performance was poorer than that of an ideal observer (IO) which perfectly compares interval variances. However, like the IO, Weber's Law behavior was observed, with a constant ratio of ( σSIG (2)- σSTAN (2)) to σSTAN (2) yielding similar performance. A model which degraded the IO with a frequency-resolution noise and a computational noise provided a reasonable fit to the real data.


Assuntos
Discriminação da Altura Tonal , Espectrografia do Som , Estimulação Acústica , Adulto , Limiar Auditivo , Comportamento de Escolha , Limiar Diferencial , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicoacústica , Psicometria
19.
PLoS One ; 9(2): e87176, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24498299

RESUMO

The human brain is able to predict the sensory effects of its actions. But how precise are these predictions? The present research proposes a tool to measure thresholds between a simple action (keystroke) and a resulting sound. On each trial, participants were required to press a key. Upon each keystroke, a woodblock sound was presented. In some trials, the sound came immediately with the downward keystroke; at other times, it was delayed by a varying amount of time. Participants were asked to verbally report whether the sound came immediately or was delayed. Participants' delay detection thresholds (in msec) were measured with a staircase-like procedure. We hypothesised that musicians would have a lower threshold than non-musicians. Comparing pianists and brass players, we furthermore hypothesised that, as a result of a sharper attack of the timbre of their instrument, pianists might have lower thresholds than brass players. Our results show that non-musicians exhibited higher thresholds for delay detection (180 ± 104 ms) than the two groups of musicians (102 ±65 ms), but there were no differences between pianists and brass players. The variance in delay detection thresholds could be explained by variance i n sensorimotor synchronisation capacities as well as variance in a purely auditory temporal irregularity detection measure. This suggests that the brain's capacity to generate temporal predictions of sensory consequences can be decomposed into general temporal prediction capacities together with auditory-motor coupling. These findings indicate that the brain has a relatively large window of integration within which an action and its resulting effect are judged as simultaneous. Furthermore, musical expertise may narrow this window down, potentially due to a more refined temporal prediction. This novel paradigm provides a simple test to estimate the temporal precision of auditory-motor action-effect coupling, and the paradigm can readily be incorporated in studies investigating both healthy and patient populations.


Assuntos
Limiar Auditivo , Música , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial , Estimulação Acústica , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Limiar Diferencial , Feminino , Mãos/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Som , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Adulto Jovem
20.
Clin Neurophysiol ; 125(8): 1647-52, 2014 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24559662

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: The minimum time interval between two stimuli that can be reliably detected is called the gap detection threshold. The present study examines whether an unconscious state, natural sleep affects the gap detection threshold. METHODS: Event-related potentials were recorded in 10 young adults while awake and during all-night sleep to provide an objective estimate of this threshold. These subjects were presented with 2, 4, 8 or 16ms gaps occurring in 1.5 duration white noise. RESULTS: During wakefulness, a significant N1 was elicited for the 8 and 16ms gaps. N1 was difficult to observe during stage N2 sleep, even for the longest gap. A large P2 was however elicited and was significant for the 8 and 16ms gaps. Also, a later, very large N350 was elicited by the 16ms gap. An N1 and P2 was significant only for the 16ms gap during REM sleep. SIGNIFICANCE: ERPs to gaps occurring in noise segments can therefore be successfully elicited during natural sleep. The gap detection threshold is similar in the waking and sleeping states.


Assuntos
Potenciais Evocados Auditivos/fisiologia , Sono/fisiologia , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Adulto , Limiar Diferencial , Eletroencefalografia , Potenciais Evocados , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Valores de Referência , Fases do Sono/fisiologia , Sono REM/fisiologia , Vigília/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA