Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 321
Filtrar
1.
Cognition ; 254: 105971, 2024 Oct 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39369595

RESUMEN

Mathematics is an underexplored domain of human cognition. While many studies have focused on subsets of math concepts such as numbers, fractions, or geometric shapes, few have ventured beyond these elementary domains. Here, we attempted to map out the full space of math concepts and to answer two specific questions: can distributed semantic models, such a GloVe, provide a satisfactory fit to human semantic judgements in mathematics? And how does this fit vary with education? We first analyzed all of the French and English Wikipedia pages with math contents, and used a semi-automatic procedure to extract the 1000 most frequent math terms in both languages. In a second step, we collected extensive behavioral judgements of familiarity and semantic similarity between them. About half of the variance in human similarity judgements was explained by vector embeddings that attempt to capture latent semantic structures based on cooccurence statistics. Participants' self-reported level of education modulated familiarity and similarity, allowing us to create a partial hierarchy among high-level math concepts. Our results converge onto the proposal of a map of math space, organized as a database of math terms with information about their frequency, familiarity, grade of acquisition, and entanglement with other concepts.

2.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 20(9): e1012430, 2024 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39241019

RESUMEN

Learning to read places a strong challenge on the visual system. Years of expertise lead to a remarkable capacity to separate similar letters and encode their relative positions, thus distinguishing words such as FORM and FROM, invariantly over a large range of positions, sizes and fonts. How neural circuits achieve invariant word recognition remains unknown. Here, we address this issue by recycling deep neural network models initially trained for image recognition. We retrain them to recognize written words and then analyze how reading-specialized units emerge and operate across the successive layers. With literacy, a small subset of units becomes specialized for word recognition in the learned script, similar to the visual word form area (VWFA) in the human brain. We show that these units are sensitive to specific letter identities and their ordinal position from the left or the right of a word. The transition from retinotopic to ordinal position coding is achieved by a hierarchy of "space bigram" unit that detect the position of a letter relative to a blank space and that pool across low- and high-frequency-sensitive units from early layers of the network. The proposed scheme provides a plausible neural code for written words in the VWFA, and leads to predictions for reading behavior, error patterns, and the neurophysiology of reading.


Asunto(s)
Redes Neurales de la Computación , Lectura , Humanos , Biología Computacional , Modelos Neurológicos , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Aprendizaje Profundo , Encéfalo/fisiología
3.
N Engl J Med ; 391(7): 598-608, 2024 Aug 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39141852

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Patients with brain injury who are unresponsive to commands may perform cognitive tasks that are detected on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electroencephalography (EEG). This phenomenon, known as cognitive motor dissociation, has not been systematically studied in a large cohort of persons with disorders of consciousness. METHODS: In this prospective cohort study conducted at six international centers, we collected clinical, behavioral, and task-based fMRI and EEG data from a convenience sample of 353 adults with disorders of consciousness. We assessed the response to commands on task-based fMRI or EEG in participants without an observable response to verbal commands (i.e., those with a behavioral diagnosis of coma, vegetative state, or minimally conscious state-minus) and in participants with an observable response to verbal commands. The presence or absence of an observable response to commands was assessed with the use of the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R). RESULTS: Data from fMRI only or EEG only were available for 65% of the participants, and data from both fMRI and EEG were available for 35%. The median age of the participants was 37.9 years, the median time between brain injury and assessment with the CRS-R was 7.9 months (25% of the participants were assessed with the CRS-R within 28 days after injury), and brain trauma was an etiologic factor in 50%. We detected cognitive motor dissociation in 60 of the 241 participants (25%) without an observable response to commands, of whom 11 had been assessed with the use of fMRI only, 13 with the use of EEG only, and 36 with the use of both techniques. Cognitive motor dissociation was associated with younger age, longer time since injury, and brain trauma as an etiologic factor. In contrast, responses on task-based fMRI or EEG occurred in 43 of 112 participants (38%) with an observable response to verbal commands. CONCLUSIONS: Approximately one in four participants without an observable response to commands performed a cognitive task on fMRI or EEG as compared with one in three participants with an observable response to commands. (Funded by the James S. McDonnell Foundation and others.).


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Encefálicas , Trastornos de la Conciencia , Trastornos Disociativos , Estado Vegetativo Persistente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Lesiones Encefálicas/fisiopatología , Lesiones Encefálicas/complicaciones , Lesiones Encefálicas/diagnóstico por imagen , Cognición/fisiología , Trastornos de la Conciencia/diagnóstico por imagen , Trastornos de la Conciencia/etiología , Trastornos de la Conciencia/fisiopatología , Electroencefalografía , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Estado Vegetativo Persistente/diagnóstico por imagen , Estado Vegetativo Persistente/etiología , Estado Vegetativo Persistente/fisiopatología , Estudios Prospectivos , Trastornos Disociativos/diagnóstico por imagen , Trastornos Disociativos/etiología , Trastornos Disociativos/fisiopatología
4.
Commun Biol ; 7(1): 716, 2024 Jun 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38858589

RESUMEN

The awake mammalian brain is functionally organized in terms of large-scale distributed networks that are constantly interacting. Loss of consciousness might disrupt this temporal organization leaving patients unresponsive. We hypothesize that characterizing brain activity in terms of transient events may provide a signature of consciousness. For this, we analyze temporal dynamics of spatiotemporally overlapping functional networks obtained from fMRI transient activity across different anesthetics and levels of anesthesia. We first show a striking homology in spatial organization of networks between monkeys and humans, indicating cross-species similarities in resting-state fMRI structure. We then track how network organization shifts under different anesthesia conditions in macaque monkeys. While the spatial aspect of the networks is preserved, their temporal dynamics are highly affected by anesthesia. Networks express for longer durations and co-activate in an anesthetic-specific configuration. Additionally, hierarchical brain organization is disrupted with a consciousness-level-signature role of the default mode network. In conclusion, large-scale brain network temporal dynamics capture differences in anesthetic-specific consciousness-level, paving the way towards a clinical translation of these cortical signature.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Estado de Conciencia , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Estado de Conciencia/efectos de los fármacos , Estado de Conciencia/fisiología , Animales , Encéfalo/fisiología , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Humanos , Anestesia , Masculino , Macaca mulatta , Adulto , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/diagnóstico por imagen , Red Nerviosa/efectos de los fármacos , Femenino , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos
5.
Sci Data ; 11(1): 590, 2024 Jun 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38839770

RESUMEN

The Individual Brain Charting (IBC) is a multi-task functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging dataset acquired at high spatial-resolution and dedicated to the cognitive mapping of the human brain. It consists in the deep phenotyping of twelve individuals, covering a broad range of psychological domains suitable for functional-atlasing applications. Here, we present the inclusion of task data from both naturalistic stimuli and trial-based designs, to uncover structures of brain activation. We rely on the Fast Shared Response Model (FastSRM) to provide a data-driven solution for modelling naturalistic stimuli, typically containing many features. We show that data from left-out runs can be reconstructed using FastSRM, enabling the extraction of networks from the visual, auditory and language systems. We also present the topographic organization of the visual system through retinotopy. In total, six new tasks were added to IBC, wherein four trial-based retinotopic tasks contributed with a mapping of the visual field to the cortex. IBC is open access: source plus derivatives imaging data and meta-data are available in public repositories.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Encéfalo , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Humanos , Encéfalo/fisiología , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Películas Cinematográficas , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Corteza Visual/diagnóstico por imagen
6.
Open Mind (Camb) ; 8: 535-557, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38746855

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: How does lexical decision behavior vary in students with the same grade level (all students were in their first year of middle-school), but different levels of reading fluency? Here, we tested a prediction of the dual-route model: as fluency increases, variations in the results may reflect a decreasing reliance on decoding and an increasing reliance on the lexical route. METHOD: 1,501 French 6th graders passed a one-minute speeded reading-aloud task evaluating fluency, and a ten-minute computerized lexical decision task evaluating the impact of lexicality, length, word frequency and pseudoword type. RESULTS: As predicted, the word length effect varied dramatically with reading fluency, with the least fluent students showing a length effect even for frequent words. The frequency effect also varied, but solely in proportion to overall reading speed, suggesting that frequency affects the decision stage similarly in all readers, while length disproportionately impacts poor readers. Response times and errors were also affected by pseudoword type (e.g., letter substitutions or transpositions), but these effects showed minimal variation with fluency. Overall, lexical decision variables were excellent predictors of reading fluency (r = 0.62). CONCLUSION: Our results highlight the variability in middle-school reading ability and describe how a simple lexical decision task can be used to assess students' mental lexicon (vocabulary) and the automatization of reading skills.

7.
Neuron ; 112(10): 1527-1530, 2024 May 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38754371

RESUMEN

Stanislas Dehaene is a cognitive neuroscientist elucidating the biological mechanisms that give rise to human perception and cognition. In a conversation with Neuron, he talks about his ongoing interest in consciousness research, the role of theory in neuroscience, and his current work on education and the science of learning.


Asunto(s)
Estado de Conciencia , Humanos , Historia del Siglo XXI , Estado de Conciencia/fisiología , Historia del Siglo XX , Neurociencias/historia , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Neurociencia Cognitiva/historia
8.
Cell Rep ; 43(3): 113952, 2024 Mar 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38483904

RESUMEN

When exposed to sensory sequences, do macaque monkeys spontaneously form abstract internal models that generalize to novel experiences? Here, we show that neuronal populations in macaque ventrolateral prefrontal cortex jointly encode visual sequences by separate codes for the specific pictures presented and for their abstract sequential structure. We recorded prefrontal neurons while macaque monkeys passively viewed visual sequences and sequence mismatches in the local-global paradigm. Even without any overt task or response requirements, prefrontal populations spontaneously form representations of sequence structure, serial order, and image identity within distinct but superimposed neuronal subspaces. Representations of sequence structure rapidly update following single exposure to a mismatch sequence, while distinct populations represent mismatches for sequences of different complexity. Finally, those representations generalize across sequences following the same repetition structure but comprising different images. These results suggest that prefrontal populations spontaneously encode rich internal models of visual sequences reflecting both content-specific and abstract information.


Asunto(s)
Macaca , Corteza Prefrontal , Animales , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral
9.
Cell Rep ; 43(3): 113847, 2024 Mar 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38412098

RESUMEN

The ability to compose successive words into a meaningful phrase is a characteristic feature of human cognition, yet its neural mechanisms remain incompletely understood. Here, we analyze the cortical mechanisms of semantic composition using magnetoencephalography (MEG) while participants read one-word, two-word, and five-word noun phrases and compared them with a subsequent image. Decoding of MEG signals revealed three processing stages. During phrase comprehension, the representation of individual words was sustained for a variable duration depending on phrasal context. During the delay period, the word code was replaced by a working-memory code whose activation increased with semantic complexity. Finally, the speed and accuracy of retrieval depended on semantic complexity and was faster for surface than for deep semantic properties. In conclusion, we propose that the brain initially encodes phrases using factorized dimensions for successive words but later compresses them in working memory and requires a period of decompression to access them.


Asunto(s)
Memoria a Corto Plazo , Semántica , Humanos , Comprensión/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Encéfalo/fisiología
10.
Elife ; 122023 11 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37910588

RESUMEN

According to the language-of-thought hypothesis, regular sequences are compressed in human memory using recursive loops akin to a mental program that predicts future items. We tested this theory by probing memory for 16-item sequences made of two sounds. We recorded brain activity with functional MRI and magneto-encephalography (MEG) while participants listened to a hierarchy of sequences of variable complexity, whose minimal description required transition probabilities, chunking, or nested structures. Occasional deviant sounds probed the participants' knowledge of the sequence. We predicted that task difficulty and brain activity would be proportional to the complexity derived from the minimal description length in our formal language. Furthermore, activity should increase with complexity for learned sequences, and decrease with complexity for deviants. These predictions were upheld in both fMRI and MEG, indicating that sequence predictions are highly dependent on sequence structure and become weaker and delayed as complexity increases. The proposed language recruited bilateral superior temporal, precentral, anterior intraparietal, and cerebellar cortices. These regions overlapped extensively with a localizer for mathematical calculation, and much less with spoken or written language processing. We propose that these areas collectively encode regular sequences as repetitions with variations and their recursive composition into nested structures.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Encéfalo , Humanos , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Lenguaje , Aprendizaje , Memoria
11.
Proc Biol Sci ; 290(2008): 20231665, 2023 10 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37788702

RESUMEN

Does word flickering facilitate reading? Despite a lack of scientific evidence, flickering glasses and lamps for dyslexia are being marketed in various countries. We conducted four experiments to assess their efficacy. Two experiments involved a computerized lexical decision task with constant display or low-frequency flickering (10 or 15 Hz). Among 375 regular adult readers, flicker noticeably slowed down word recognition, while slightly biasing the decision towards pseudowords. No significant effect was observed in 20 dyslexic children. In 22 dyslexic children, we also evaluated the impact of the Lexilight lamp and Lexilens glasses, which operate at higher frequencies, on reading fluency, letter identification and mirror letter processing. No detectable impact was observed. Lastly, in two participants who claimed to benefit from flickering glasses, we orthogonally manipulated whether the glasses were actually on, and whether the participant thought they were on. Only a small placebo effect was noted in one participant. Our findings starkly contrast with marketing claims that these tools can help 90% of dyslexics, and emphasize the role of rigorous scientific research in empowering dyslexic individuals to make informed decisions.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia , Lectura , Adulto , Niño , Humanos
13.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 10266, 2023 06 24.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37355745

RESUMEN

Data plots are widely used in science, journalism and politics, since they efficiently allow to depict a large amount of information. Graphicacy, the ability to understand graphs, has thus become a fundamental cultural skill comparable to literacy or numeracy. Here, we introduce a measure of intuitive graphicacy that assesses the perceptual ability to detect a trend in noisy scatterplots ("does this graph go up or down?"). In 3943 educated participants, responses vary as a sigmoid function of the t-value that a statistician would compute to detect a significant trend. We find a minimum level of core intuitive graphicacy even in unschooled participants living in remote Namibian villages (N = 87) and 6-year-old 1st-graders who never read a graph (N = 27). The sigmoid slope that we propose as a proxy of intuitive graphicacy increases with education and tightly correlates with statistical and mathematical knowledge, showing that experience contributes to refining graphical intuitions. Our tool, publicly available online, allows to quickly evaluate and formally quantify a perceptual building block of graphicacy.


Asunto(s)
Comprensión , Juicio , Humanos , Matemática , Alfabetización , Intuición
14.
J Neurosci ; 43(29): 5350-5364, 2023 07 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37217308

RESUMEN

A sentence is more than the sum of its words: its meaning depends on how they combine with one another. The brain mechanisms underlying such semantic composition remain poorly understood. To shed light on the neural vector code underlying semantic composition, we introduce two hypotheses: (1) the intrinsic dimensionality of the space of neural representations should increase as a sentence unfolds, paralleling the growing complexity of its semantic representation; and (2) this progressive integration should be reflected in ramping and sentence-final signals. To test these predictions, we designed a dataset of closely matched normal and jabberwocky sentences (composed of meaningless pseudo words) and displayed them to deep language models and to 11 human participants (5 men and 6 women) monitored with simultaneous MEG and intracranial EEG. In both deep language models and electrophysiological data, we found that representational dimensionality was higher for meaningful sentences than jabberwocky. Furthermore, multivariate decoding of normal versus jabberwocky confirmed three dynamic patterns: (1) a phasic pattern following each word, peaking in temporal and parietal areas; (2) a ramping pattern, characteristic of bilateral inferior and middle frontal gyri; and (3) a sentence-final pattern in left superior frontal gyrus and right orbitofrontal cortex. These results provide a first glimpse into the neural geometry of semantic integration and constrain the search for a neural code of linguistic composition.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Starting from general linguistic concepts, we make two sets of predictions in neural signals evoked by reading multiword sentences. First, the intrinsic dimensionality of the representation should grow with additional meaningful words. Second, the neural dynamics should exhibit signatures of encoding, maintaining, and resolving semantic composition. We successfully validated these hypotheses in deep neural language models, artificial neural networks trained on text and performing very well on many natural language processing tasks. Then, using a unique combination of MEG and intracranial electrodes, we recorded high-resolution brain data from human participants while they read a controlled set of sentences. Time-resolved dimensionality analysis showed increasing dimensionality with meaning, and multivariate decoding allowed us to isolate the three dynamical patterns we had hypothesized.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Lenguaje , Masculino , Humanos , Femenino , Encéfalo/fisiología , Semántica , Lingüística , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Lectura , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos
15.
Curr Biol ; 33(10): 1906-1915.e6, 2023 05 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37071994

RESUMEN

The core knowledge hypothesis postulates that infants automatically analyze their environment along abstract dimensions, including numbers. According to this view, approximate numbers should be encoded quickly, pre-attentively, and in a supra-modal manner by the infant brain. Here, we directly tested this idea by submitting the neural responses of sleeping 3-month-old infants, measured with high-density electroencephalography (EEG), to decoders designed to disentangle numerical and non-numerical information. The results show the emergence, in approximately 400 ms, of a decodable number representation, independent of physical parameters, that separates auditory sequences of 4 vs. 12 tones and generalizes to visual arrays of 4 vs. 12 objects. Thus, the infant brain contains a number code that transcends sensory modality, sequential or simultaneous presentation, and arousal state.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Electroencefalografía , Humanos , Lactante , Encéfalo/fisiología , Nivel de Alerta
16.
Sci Adv ; 9(14): eadf6140, 2023 04 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37018408

RESUMEN

In expert readers, a brain region known as the visual word form area (VWFA) is highly sensitive to written words, exhibiting a posterior-to-anterior gradient of increasing sensitivity to orthographic stimuli whose statistics match those of real words. Using high-resolution 7-tesla functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we ask whether, in bilingual readers, distinct cortical patches specialize for different languages. In 21 English-French bilinguals, unsmoothed 1.2-millimeters fMRI revealed that the VWFA is actually composed of several small cortical patches highly selective for reading, with a posterior-to-anterior word-similarity gradient, but with near-complete overlap between the two languages. In 10 English-Chinese bilinguals, however, while most word-specific patches exhibited similar reading specificity and word-similarity gradients for reading in Chinese and English, additional patches responded specifically to Chinese writing and, unexpectedly, to faces. Our results show that the acquisition of multiple writing systems can indeed tune the visual cortex differently in bilinguals, sometimes leading to the emergence of cortical patches specialized for a single language.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Humanos , Encéfalo , Mapeo Encefálico , Lectura
17.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 120(17): e2300252120, 2023 04 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37068244

RESUMEN

Reading a sentence entails integrating the meanings of individual words to infer more complex, higher-order meaning. This highly rapid and complex human behavior is known to engage the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and middle temporal gyrus (MTG) in the language-dominant hemisphere, yet whether there are distinct contributions of these regions to sentence reading is still unclear. To probe these neural spatiotemporal dynamics, we used direct intracranial recordings to measure neural activity while reading sentences, meaning-deficient Jabberwocky sentences, and lists of words or pseudowords. We isolated two functionally and spatiotemporally distinct frontotemporal networks, each sensitive to distinct aspects of word and sentence composition. The first distributed network engages the IFG and MTG, with IFG activity preceding MTG. Activity in this network ramps up over the duration of a sentence and is reduced or absent during Jabberwocky and word lists, implying its role in the derivation of sentence-level meaning. The second network engages the superior temporal gyrus and the IFG, with temporal responses leading those in frontal lobe, and shows greater activation for each word in a list than those in sentences, suggesting that sentential context enables greater efficiency in the lexical and/or phonological processing of individual words. These adjacent, yet spatiotemporally dissociable neural mechanisms for word- and sentence-level processes shed light on the richly layered semantic networks that enable us to fluently read. These results imply distributed, dynamic computation across the frontotemporal language network rather than a clear dichotomy between the contributions of frontal and temporal structures.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Lenguaje , Lingüística , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Semántica
18.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 120(6): e2213430120, 2023 02 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36730198

RESUMEN

Many teaching websites, such as the Khan Academy, propose vivid videos illustrating a mathematical concept. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we asked whether watching such a video suffices to rapidly change the brain networks for mathematical knowledge. We capitalized on the finding that, when judging the truth of short spoken statements, distinct semantic regions activate depending on whether the statements bear on mathematical knowledge or on other domains of semantic knowledge. Here, participants answered such questions before and after watching a lively 5-min video, which taught them the rudiments of a new domain. During the video, a distinct math-responsive network, comprising anterior intraparietal and inferior temporal nodes, showed intersubject synchrony when viewing mathematics course rather than control courses in biology or law. However, this experience led to minimal subsequent changes in the activity of those domain-specific areas when answering questions on the same topics a few minutes later. All taught facts, whether mathematical or not, led to domain-general repetition enhancement, particularly prominent in the cuneus, posterior cingulate, and posterior parietal cortices. We conclude that short videos do not suffice to induce a meaningful lasting change in the brain's math-responsive network, but merely engage domain-general regions possibly involved in episodic short-term memory.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Semántica , Humanos , Encéfalo/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Matemática
19.
PLoS One ; 18(2): e0268577, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36763595

RESUMEN

The relationship between conscious experience and brain activity has intrigued scientists and philosophers for centuries. In the last decades, several theories have suggested different accounts for these relationships. These theories have developed in parallel, with little to no cross-talk among them. To advance research on consciousness, we established an adversarial collaboration between proponents of two of the major theories in the field, Global Neuronal Workspace and Integrated Information Theory. Together, we devised and preregistered two experiments that test contrasting predictions of these theories concerning the location and timing of correlates of visual consciousness, which have been endorsed by the theories' proponents. Predicted outcomes should either support, refute, or challenge these theories. Six theory-impartial laboratories will follow the study protocol specified here, using three complementary methods: Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI), Magneto-Electroencephalography (M-EEG), and intracranial electroencephalography (iEEG). The study protocol will include built-in replications, both between labs and within datasets. Through this ambitious undertaking, we hope to provide decisive evidence in favor or against the two theories and clarify the footprints of conscious visual perception in the human brain, while also providing an innovative model of large-scale, collaborative, and open science practice.


Asunto(s)
Estado de Conciencia , Teoría de la Información , Humanos , Estado de Conciencia/fisiología , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Encéfalo/fisiología , Percepción Visual , Electroencefalografía
20.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 49(1): 129-144, 2023 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36395054

RESUMEN

According to a growing body of research, human adults are remarkably accurate at extracting intuitive statistics from graphs, such as finding the best-fitting regression line through a scatterplot. Here, we ask whether humans can also perform outlier rejection, a nontrivial statistical problem. In three experiments, we investigated human adults' capacity to evaluate the linear trend of a flashed scatterplot comprising 0-4 outlier datapoints. Experiment 1 showed that participants did not spontaneously reject outliers: when outliers were not mentioned, their presence biased the participants' trend judgments and regression line estimates. In Experiment 2, where participants were explicitly asked to exclude outliers, the outlier-induced bias was reduced but remained significant. In Experiment 3, where participants were asked to explicitly detect any outlier before adjusting their regression line, outlier detection was satisfactory, but the detected outliers continued to bias the regression responses, unless they were quite distant from the main regression line. We propose a simple model for outlier detection, based on the computation of a z-score that estimates how far a given datapoint is from the distribution of distances to the regression line, and we show that this model closely approximates human performance. Detection is not rejection, however, and our results suggest that humans can remain biased by outliers that they have detected. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Estadística como Asunto , Humanos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA