Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 6 de 6
Filtrar
1.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 3345, 2020 Feb 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32076109

RESUMEN

An amendment to this paper has been published and can be accessed via a link at the top of the paper.

2.
Sci Rep ; 9(1): 7590, 2019 05 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31110242

RESUMEN

Spiking activity exhibits a large degree of variability across identical trials, which has been shown to be significantly reduced by stimulus onset in a wide range of cortical areas. Whether similar dynamics apply to the thalamus and in particular to the pulvinar is largely unknown. Here, we examined electrophysiological recordings from two adult rhesus macaques performing a perceptual task and comparatively investigated trial-to-trial variability in higher-order thalamus (ventral and dorsal pulvinar), the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) and visual cortex (area V4) prior to and following the presentation of a visual stimulus. We found spiking variability during stable fixation prior to stimulus onset to be considerably lower in both pulvinar and the LGN as compared to area V4. In contrast to the prominent variability reduction in V4 upon stimulus onset, variability in the thalamic nuclei was largely unaffected by visual stimulation. There was a small but significant variability decrease in the dorsal pulvinar, but not in the ventral portion of the pulvinar, which is closely connected to visual cortices and would thus have been expected to reflect cortical response properties. This dissociation did not stem from differences in response strength or mean firing rates and indicates fundamental differences in variability quenching between thalamus and cortex.


Asunto(s)
Tálamo/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Percepción Visual , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Estimulación Luminosa , Vías Visuales/fisiología
3.
PLoS One ; 4(7): e6183, 2009 Jul 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19593384

RESUMEN

Neurons in the visual cortex are responsive to the presentation of oriented and curved line segments, which are thought to act as primitives for the visual processing of shapes and objects. Prolonged adaptation to such stimuli gives rise to two related perceptual effects: a slow change in the appearance of the adapting stimulus (perceptual drift), and the distortion of subsequently presented test stimuli (adaptational aftereffects). Here we used a psychophysical nulling technique to dissociate and quantify these two classical observations in order to examine their underlying mechanisms and their relationship to one another. In agreement with previous work, we found that during adaptation horizontal and vertical straight lines serve as attractors for perceived orientation and curvature. However, the rate of perceptual drift for different stimuli was not predictive of the corresponding aftereffect magnitudes, indicating that the two perceptual effects are governed by distinct neural processes. Finally, the rate of perceptual drift for curved line segments did not depend on the spatial scale of the stimulus, suggesting that its mechanisms lie outside strictly retinotopic processing stages. These findings provide new evidence that the visual system relies on statistically salient intrinsic reference stimuli for the processing of visual patterns, and point to perceptual drift as an experimental window for studying the mechanisms of visual perception.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa
5.
Proc Biol Sci ; 272(1566): 897-904, 2005 May 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16024343

RESUMEN

Several recent demonstrations using visual adaptation have revealed high-level aftereffects for complex patterns including faces. While traditional aftereffects involve perceptual distortion of simple attributes such as orientation or colour that are processed early in the visual cortical hierarchy, face adaptation affects perceived identity and expression, which are thought to be products of higher-order processing. And, unlike most simple aftereffects, those involving faces are robust to changes in scale, position and orientation between the adapting and test stimuli. These differences raise the question of how closely related face aftereffects are to traditional ones. Little is known about the build-up and decay of the face aftereffect, and the similarity of these dynamic processes to traditional aftereffects might provide insight into this relationship. We examined the effect of varying the duration of both the adapting and test stimuli on the magnitude of perceived distortions in face identity. We found that, just as with traditional aftereffects, the identity aftereffect grew logarithmically stronger as a function of adaptation time and exponentially weaker as a function of test duration. Even the subtle aspects of these dynamics, such as the power-law relationship between the adapting and test durations, closely resembled that of other aftereffects. These results were obtained with two different sets of face stimuli that differed greatly in their low-level properties. We postulate that the mechanisms governing these shared dynamics may be dissociable from the responses of feature-selective neurons in the early visual cortex.


Asunto(s)
Cara , Expresión Facial , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Individualidad , Modelos Neurológicos , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa , Factores de Tiempo
6.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 24(3): 727-31, 2005 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15894471

RESUMEN

Though each foot is controlled primarily by the contralateral hemisphere, the event-related brain potentials preceding an overt foot movement are largest over the ipsilateral side of the head. Because such "paradoxical lateralization" results from the spatial organization of the motor homunculus, it can provide a sign of motor-cortex activation. We report paradoxical lateralization in the potentials accompanying imagined foot movements, thereby demonstrating a contribution of cortical areas directly involved in movement execution.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados Motores/fisiología , Pie/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Electromiografía , Electrooculografía , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Retroalimentación Psicológica , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Humanos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA