Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 4.088
Filtrar
1.
Neuropsychologia ; 198: 108882, 2024 Jun 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38599569

RESUMEN

Several studies have analyzed the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on verbal fluency tasks in non-clinical populations. Nevertheless, the reported effects on verbal fluency are inconsistent. In addition, the effect of other techniques such as transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS) on verbal fluency enhancement has yet to be studied in healthy multilingual populations. This study aims to explore the effects of tRNS on verbal fluency in healthy multilingual individuals. Fifty healthy multilingual (Spanish, English and Basque) adults were randomly assigned to a tRNS or sham group. Electrodes were placed on the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and left inferior frontal gyrus. All participants performed phonemic and semantic verbal fluency tasks before, during (online assessment) and immediately after (offline assessment) stimulation in three different languages. The results showed significantly better performance by participants who received tRNS in the phonemic verbal fluency tasks in Spanish (in the online and offline assessment) and English (in the offline assessment). No differences between conditions were found in Basque nor semantic verbal fluency. These findings suggests that tRNS on the left prefrontal cortex could help improve phonemic, yet not semantic, fluency in healthy multilingual adults.


Asunto(s)
Multilingüismo , Estimulación Transcraneal de Corriente Directa , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Fonética , Conducta Verbal/fisiología , Semántica , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Corteza Prefontal Dorsolateral/fisiología
2.
Infant Behav Dev ; 75: 101945, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38579484

RESUMEN

Conversational turn-taking is ubiquitously found in caregiver-infant interactions, and robustly predictive of infant communicative development. Over the first year, infants take quick adult-like vocal turns with caregivers. Many studies have documented the consistency of caregiver responsiveness and its influence on infant rapid language growth. However, few have examined how caregiver responsiveness facilitates extended vocal turn-taking in real-time with infants over the first year. The influence of prelinguistic vocal turn-taking on the emergence of language has also been under-investigated. We analyzed free-play sessions of 30 caregivers and their infants at both 5 and 10 months, and obtained infant language outcomes at 18 months. We examined the developmental consistency (group-level continuity and dyad-order stability) and change of infant volubility, caregiver responses to babbling in vocal, non-vocal and multimodal modalities, and the influence of modality on caregiver-infant vocal turn-taking. Caregiver contingent responsiveness to infant babbling at 5 months predicted vocal turn-taking at 10 months. Developmental increases in prelinguistic vocalizing and vocal turn-taking from 5 to 10 months predicted infant language outcomes at 18 months. At both 5 and 10 months, caregiver vocal responses were more effective in extending turn-taking than non-vocal or multimodal responses. In summary, prelinguistic vocal turn-taking, facilitated by caregiver vocal responsiveness, is positively related to the emergence of early language.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Lactante , Conducta Verbal/fisiología , Cuidadores/psicología , Adulto , Conducta del Lactante/fisiología , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología
3.
Dev Cogn Neurosci ; 67: 101379, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38615557

RESUMEN

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental condition frequently associated with structural cerebellar abnormalities. Whether cerebellar grey matter volumes (GMV) are linked to verbal impairments remains controversial. Here, the association between cerebellar GMV and verbal abilities in ASD was examined across the lifespan. Lobular segmentation of the cerebellum was performed on structural MRI scans from the ABIDE I dataset in male individuals with ASD (N=144, age: 8.5-64.0 years) and neurotypical controls (N=188; age: 8.0-56.2 years). Stepwise linear mixed effects modeling including group (ASD vs. neurotypical controls), lobule-wise GMV, and age was performed to identify cerebellar lobules which best predicted verbal abilities as measured by verbal IQ (VIQ). An age-specific association between VIQ and GMV of bilateral Crus II was found in ASD relative to neurotypical controls. In children with ASD, higher VIQ was associated with larger GMV of left Crus II but smaller GMV of right Crus II. By contrast, in adults with ASD, higher VIQ was associated with smaller GMV of left Crus II and larger GMV of right Crus II. These findings indicate that relative to the contralateral hemisphere, an initial reliance on the language-nonspecific left cerebellar hemisphere is offset by more typical right-lateralization in adulthood.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Cerebelo , Sustancia Gris , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Humanos , Masculino , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/diagnóstico por imagen , Cerebelo/diagnóstico por imagen , Sustancia Gris/patología , Sustancia Gris/diagnóstico por imagen , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Niño , Adulto , Adolescente , Adulto Joven , Persona de Mediana Edad , Conducta Verbal/fisiología
4.
Int J Speech Lang Pathol ; 26(2): 257-266, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37337670

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Semantic fluency is potentially a useful tool for vocabulary assessment in children with vision impairment because it contains no visual test stimuli. It is not known whether in the primary school years children with vision impairment perform more poorly on semantic fluency tasks compared to their sighted peers. METHOD: We compared semantic fluency performance of two groups of 5- to 11-year-old British English speaking children-one group with vision impairment and one without. We also investigated within-group differences in performance, based on severity of vision impairment. We administered one category (animals) to children with vision impairment (n = 45) and sighted children (n = 30). Participants had one minute to respond. Responses were coded for accuracy, error type, clusters, and switches. RESULT: Correct responses increased with age within each group. Groups did not differ significantly on any outcome measure. Severity of vision impairment did not impact task performance. CONCLUSION: Results suggested that semantic fluency performance-at least for the category animals-is not different in children with vision impairment compared to sighted children. Findings also suggest that semantic fluency could be a suitable addition to the tools that speech-language pathologists use to assess language abilities in children with vision impairment.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje , Semántica , Niño , Humanos , Preescolar , Vocabulario , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Instituciones Académicas , Conducta Verbal/fisiología
5.
J Appl Behav Anal ; 57(1): 204-225, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37828795

RESUMEN

Autoclitics are secondary verbal operants that are controlled by a feature of the conditions that occasion or evoke a primary verbal operant such as a tact or mand. Qualifying autoclitics extend, negate, or assert a speaker's primary verbal response and modify the intensity or direction of the listener's behavior. Howard and Rice (1988) established autoclitics that indicated weak stimulus control (e.g., "like a [primary tact]") with four neurotypical preschool children. However, generalization to newly acquired tacts was limited. In Experiment 1, we addressed similar behavior as in Howard and Rice but with autistic children while using simultaneous teaching procedures, and we observed generalization across sets and with newly acquired tacts. In Experiment 2, we evaluated the effects of multiple-exemplar training on generalization of autoclitics across sets of naturalistic stimuli. Across participants, gradual increases in the frequency of autoclitics occurred with untaught stimuli after teaching with one or more sets.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Telurio , Preescolar , Humanos , Conducta Verbal/fisiología , Generalización Psicológica
6.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 66(12): 4967-4983, 2023 12 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37889261

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Verbal fluency evaluation in bilingual speakers should include dual-language assessment to obtain a comprehensive profile of word retrieval abilities. This study is the first to compare classic semantic, action, emotional, and phonemic fluency in terms of the magnitude of their performance gaps between the dominant and nondominant language in unbalanced bilingual speakers. We also examined the quantitative relationship between language dominance and verbal fluency performance. METHOD: Twenty-six bilingual adults completed a comprehensive set of classic semantic ("animals," "vegetables"), action ("do"), emotional ("happy," "sad," "afraid"), and phonemic ("F," "A," "S") fluency tasks in their dominant language (English) and nondominant language (Spanish) in two sessions on separate days. Participants also completed subjective and objective measures of language proficiency. RESULTS: All tasks yielded fewer correct responses in the nondominant language. The between-languages performance gap was the largest for "animals" and the smallest for emotional fluency. "Happy" yielded the most balanced performance among all semantic tasks and a positivity bias that was unaffected by language dominance. Finally, language dominance scores computed by a newly developed formula indicated relationships between self-rated proficiency and fluency performance. CONCLUSIONS: This study provides preliminary, normative data of classic semantic, action, emotional, and phonemic fluency that could be used to gauge unbalanced bilingual speakers' performance. Significant impacts of language dominance on "animals" demand caution in using this widely used classic semantic category in evaluating bilingual speakers' performance. The data also underscore the robustness of positivity biases in emotional fluency and the validity of using subjective measures to supplement neuropsychological assessment of fluency performance.


Asunto(s)
Semántica , Conducta Verbal , Adulto , Humanos , Conducta Verbal/fisiología , Lenguaje , Emociones , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
7.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 120(42): e2312462120, 2023 10 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37824523

RESUMEN

Humans may retrieve words from memory by exploring and exploiting in "semantic space" similar to how nonhuman animals forage for resources in physical space. This has been studied using the verbal fluency test (VFT), in which participants generate words belonging to a semantic or phonetic category in a limited time. People produce bursts of related items during VFT, referred to as "clustering" and "switching." The strategic foraging model posits that cognitive search behavior is guided by a monitoring process which detects relevant declines in performance and then triggers the searcher to seek a new patch or cluster in memory after the current patch has been depleted. An alternative body of research proposes that this behavior can be explained by an undirected rather than strategic search process, such as random walks with or without random jumps to new parts of semantic space. This study contributes to this theoretical debate by testing for neural evidence of strategically timed switches during memory search. Thirty participants performed category and letter VFT during functional MRI. Responses were classified as cluster or switch events based on computational metrics of similarity and participant evaluations. Results showed greater hippocampal and posterior cerebellar activation during switching than clustering, even while controlling for interresponse times and linguistic distance. Furthermore, these regions exhibited ramping activity which increased during within-patch search leading up to switches. Findings support the strategic foraging model, clarifying how neural switch processes may guide memory search in a manner akin to foraging in patchy spatial environments.


Asunto(s)
Fonética , Semántica , Animales , Humanos , Conducta Verbal/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
8.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 45(5): 452-463, 2023 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37656122

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Verbal fluency tests (VFTs) are widely used to assess cognitive-linguistic performance in neurological diseases. However, the influence of dysarthria on performance in tests requiring oral responses is unclear in ataxia and Parkinson's disease. OBJECTIVES: To determine the impact of dysarthria on VFT performance and evaluate the validity and reliability of alternative methods for analyzing VFT data. METHOD: Trained raters evaluated dysarthria using VFT recordings in people with ataxia (N = 61) or Parkinson's disease (PD; N = 69). Total Correct Items scores and qualitative parameters (intrusions, ambiguous verbalizations, perseverations, and interjections) were compared across semantic, phonemic, and alternating fluency tasks. Disease severity was considered as a covariate in the regression model. RESULTS: VFT dysarthria ratings correlated with the benchmark (ground truth) dysarthria scores derived from a monologue. Ambiguous responses resulting from unclear speech impeded the rater's ability to determine if a response was correct. Regression analysis indicated that more severe dysarthria ratings predicted diminished scores in all three tasks (semantic fluency, phonemic fluency and alternating fluency) in the ataxia group. The contribution of disease severity to semantic, phonemic and alternating fluency was reduced substantially in the ataxia group after accounting for dysarthria severity in the model in both groups. CONCLUSIONS: Dysarthria severity can be estimated based on speech samples derived from VFT. Dysarthria can lead to lower total correct items and is associated with more ambiguous verbalizations in VFT. Dysarthria severity should be considered when interpreting VFT performance in common movement disorders.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Parkinson , Degeneraciones Espinocerebelosas , Humanos , Disartria/etiología , Disartria/complicaciones , Enfermedad de Parkinson/complicaciones , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Semántica , Degeneraciones Espinocerebelosas/complicaciones , Ataxia/complicaciones , Conducta Verbal/fisiología
9.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 23(3): 957-972, 2023 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37188856

RESUMEN

Language researchers view utterance planning as implicit decision-making: producers must choose the words, sentence structures, and various other linguistic features to communicate their message. To date, much of the research on utterance planning has focused on situations in which the speaker knows the full message to convey. Less is known about circumstances in which speakers begin utterance planning before they are certain about their message. In three picture-naming experiments, we used a novel paradigm to examine how speakers plan utterances before a full message is known. In Experiments 1 and 2, participants viewed displays showing two pairs of objects, followed by a cue to name one pair. In an Overlap condition, one object appeared in both pairs, providing early information about one of the objects to name. In a Different condition, there was no object overlap. Across both spoken and typed responses, participants tended to name the overlapping target first in the Overlap condition, with shorter initiation latencies compared with other utterances. Experiment 3 used a semantically constraining question to provide early information about the upcoming targets, and participants tended to name the more likely target first in their response. These results suggest that in situations of uncertainty, producers choose word orders that allow them to begin early planning. Producers prioritize message components that are certain to be needed and continue planning the rest when more information becomes available. Given similarities to planning strategies for other goal-directed behaviors, we suggest continuity between decision-making processes in language and other cognitive domains.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones , Incertidumbre , Conducta Verbal , Humanos , Lingüística , Habla/fisiología , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Conducta Verbal/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Estudiantes
10.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 119(3): 513-528, 2023 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36800892

RESUMEN

Intraverbal behavior is a type of verbal behavior in which the response form has no point-to-point correspondence with its verbal stimulus. However, the form and occurrence of most intraverbals is under the control of multiple variables. Establishing this form of multiple control may depend on a variety of preestablished skills. The purpose of Experiment 1 was to evaluate these potential prerequisites with adult participants using a multiple probe design. The results suggest that training was not required for each putative prerequisite. In Experiment 2, probes for all skills were conducted following convergent intraverbal probes. The results showed that convergent intraverbals only emerged when proficiency of each skill was demonstrated. Finally, Experiment 3 evaluated alternating training of multiple tact and intraverbal categorization. The results showed that this procedure was effective for half of the participants.


Asunto(s)
Telurio , Conducta Verbal , Adulto , Humanos , Conducta Verbal/fisiología
11.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 37(7): 1479-1497, 2023 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36550679

RESUMEN

Objective: Parkinson's disease (PD) and essential tremor (ET) involve neuroanatomical circuitry that impact frontal lobe functioning, via the striatum and cerebellum, respectively. The aim of this exploratory study was to investigate quantitative and qualitative performance between and within these groups on measures of verbal fluency. Method: Sixty-three PD and 53 ET patients completed neuropsychological testing. Linear regression models with robust variance estimation compared verbal fluency performance between groups related to correct responses and errors. Paired t-tests investigated within group error rates. Results: PD patients gave more correct responses for phonological (ß̂ =5.3, p=.01) and category fluency (ß̂ =4.1, p=.01) than ET patients; however, when processing speed was added as a covariate, this attenuated performance on both measures and only phonological fluency remained significant (ß̂ =4.0, p=.04). There were no statistical differences in error scores between groups. Error rates within groups suggested that PD patients had higher error rates in total errors and perseveration errors on phonological fluency (M = 2.6, p=.00; M = 1.6, p=.00) and higher total errors and set-loss error rates on category switching (M = 5.1, p<.001; M = 4.1, p<.001). ET patients had higher error rate with relation to total errors and set-loss errors on phonological fluency (M = 2.5, p=.00; M = 1.5, p=.02) and category switching (M = 3.9, p=,00; M = 3.9, p<.001). Conclusions: PD patients performed better than ET patients on phonological fluency. PD patients appear to make more perseveration errors on phonological fluency, while ET patients made more set-loss errors. Implications for frontal lobe dysfunction and clinical impact are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Temblor Esencial , Enfermedad de Parkinson , Humanos , Enfermedad de Parkinson/complicaciones , Enfermedad de Parkinson/psicología , Temblor Esencial/complicaciones , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Velocidad de Procesamiento , Conducta Verbal/fisiología
12.
Distúrb. comun ; 34(4): 57797, dez. 2022. ilus, tab
Artículo en Portugués | LILACS | ID: biblio-1425824

RESUMEN

Introdução: os treinamentos para o desenvolvimento da comunicação oral podem melhorar a auto percepção da fala e da voz, principalmente, em situações de fala em público. Objetivo: descrever a auto percepção dos efeitos de um treinamento para a comunicação oral dos locutores de uma rádio universitária em situações de fala em público. Método: este é um estudo antes e após intervenção. Foi aplicado o Programa de Desenvolvimento da Expressividade para Comunicação Oral em oito locutores durante oito encontros de duas horas de duração. O questionário de Auto avaliação das Habilidades de Voz e Fala em Diversos Contextos Comunicativos foi aplicado no primeiro e no último encontro. Resultados: a amostra constituiu-se, majoritariamente, por mulheres jovens, solteiras e estudantes, que trabalhavam por meio período durante três dias. As situações de fala em público que no início do treinamento ocorriam eventualmente passaram a ser mais frequentes. Houve redução nos sintomas de nervosismo, ansiedade, preocupação e confusão no conteúdo durante o discurso. A percepção de tremor e quebras na voz reduziram, e o sintoma de fala mais rápido aumentou. Houve relato prévio de que os interlocutores avaliavam a sua dicção variável com a situação, e ao final, afirmaram que era igual ao habitual. No término, segundo eles, as pessoas avaliavam a sua comunicação como boa. Conclusão: o treinamento resultou discretamente na auto percepção positiva para organização do discurso e nos sintomas de desvios vocais e alterações na fala dos locutores.


Introduction: training for the development of oral communication can improve self-perception of speech and voice, especially in public speaking situations. Objective: to describe the self-perception of the effects of oral communication training for university radio announcers in public speaking situations. Method: this is a before and after intervention study. The Expressiveness Development Program for Oral Communication was applied to eight speakers during eight two-hour meetings. The Self-Assessment of Voice and Speech Skills in Different Communicative Contexts questionnaire was applied in the first and last meeting. Results: the sample consisted mostly of young single women and students, who worked part-time for three days. The public speaking situations that occurred at the beginning of the training eventually became more frequent. There was a reduction in the symptoms of nervousness, anxiety, worry and confusion in the content during the speech. The perception of tremor and voice breaks reduced, and the symptom of faster speech increased. There was a previous report that the interlocutors evaluated their variable diction with the situation, and in the end, they stated that it was the same as usual. At the end, they said, people rated their communication as good. Conclusion: the training discreetly resulted in positive self-perception for speech organization and in symptoms of vocal deviations and changes in the speakers' speech.


Introducción: el entrenamiento para el desarrollo de la comunicación oral puede mejorar la autopercepción del habla y la voz, especialmente en situaciones de hablar en público. Objetivo: describir la autopercepción de los efectos del entrenamiento en comunicación oral para locutores universitarios de radio en situaciones de hablar en público. Método: este es un estudio de intervención antes y después. El Programa de Desarrollo de la Expresividad para la Comunicación Oral se aplicó a ocho ponentes durante ocho encuentros de dos horas. En la primera y última reunión se aplicó el cuestionario Self-Assessment of Voice and Speech Skills in Different Comunicative Contexts. Resultados: la muestra estuvo compuesta en su mayoría por mujeres jóvenes, solteras y estudiantes, que trabajaron a tiempo parcial durante tres días. Las situaciones de hablar en público que ocurrieron al comienzo de la capacitación eventualmente se hicieron más frecuentes. Hubo una reducción en los síntomas de nerviosismo, ansiedad, preocupación y confusión en el contenido durante el discurso. Se redujo la percepción de temblores y roturas de voz, y aumentó el síntoma de habla más rápida. Hubo un reporte previo de que los interlocutores evaluaron su dicción variable con la situación, y al final afirmaron que era la misma de siempre. Al final, dijeron, las personas calificaron su comunicación como buena. Conclusión: el entrenamiento resultó discretamente en una autopercepción positiva para la organización del habla y en síntomas de desviaciones vocales y alteraciones en el habla de los hablantes.


Asunto(s)
Humanos , Femenino , Adulto , Percepción , Conducta Verbal/fisiología , Entrenamiento de la Voz , Radio , Universidades , Evaluación de Resultados de Intervenciones Terapéuticas , Estudios Controlados Antes y Después
13.
Behav Neurol ; 2022: 6935263, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35502419

RESUMEN

Semantic fluency is the ability to name items from a given category within a limited time, which relies on semantic memory, working memory, and executive function. Semantic disfluency is a common problem in Parkinson's disease (PD) and Alzheimer's disease (AD). We demonstrated a graph theoretical analysis of semantic fluency in patients with PD (N = 86), patients with AD (N = 40), and healthy controls (HC, N = 88). All participants completed a standard animal fluency test. Their verbal responses were recorded, transcripted, and transformed into directed speech graphs. Patients with PD generated fewer correct words than HC and more correct words than patients with AD. Patients with PD showed higher density, shorter diameter, and shorter average shortest path length than HC, but lower density, longer diameter, and longer average shortest path length than patients with AD. It suggests that patients with PD produced relatively smaller and denser speech graphs. Moreover, in PD, the densities of speech graphs correlated with the severity of non-motor symptoms, but not the severity of motor symptoms. The graph theoretical analysis revealed new features of semantic disfluency in patients with PD.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Enfermedad de Parkinson , Humanos , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Semántica , Conducta Verbal/fisiología
14.
Commun Biol ; 5(1): 489, 2022 05 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35606497

RESUMEN

When we retell our past experiences, we aim to reproduce some version of the original events; this reproduced version is often temporally compressed relative to the original. However, it is currently unclear how this compression manifests in brain activity. One possibility is that a compressed retrieved memory manifests as a neural pattern which is more dissimilar to the original, relative to a more detailed or vivid memory. However, we argue that measuring raw dissimilarity alone is insufficient, as it confuses a variety of interesting and uninteresting changes. To address this problem, we examine brain pattern changes that are consistent across people. We show that temporal compression in individuals' retelling of past events predicts systematic encoding-to-recall transformations in several higher associative regions. These findings elucidate how neural representations are not simply reactivated, but can also be transformed due to temporal compression during a universal form of human memory expression: verbal retelling.


Asunto(s)
Recuerdo Mental , Conducta Verbal , Humanos , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Conducta Verbal/fisiología
15.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 119(4)2022 01 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35042815

RESUMEN

Clicking is one of the most robust metaphors for social connection. But how do we know when two people "click"? We asked pairs of friends and strangers to talk with each other and rate their felt connection. For both friends and strangers, speed in response was a robust predictor of feeling connected. Conversations with faster response times felt more connected than conversations with slower response times, and within conversations, connected moments had faster response times than less-connected moments. This effect was determined primarily by partner responsivity: People felt more connected to the degree that their partner responded quickly to them rather than by how quickly they responded to their partner. The temporal scale of these effects (<250 ms) precludes conscious control, thus providing an honest signal of connection. Using a round-robin design in each of six closed networks, we show that faster responders evoked greater feelings of connection across partners. Finally, we demonstrate that this signal is used by third-party listeners as a heuristic of how well people are connected: Conversations with faster response times were perceived as more connected than the same conversations with slower response times. Together, these findings suggest that response times comprise a robust and sufficient signal of whether two minds "click."


Asunto(s)
Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Interacción Social/clasificación , Conducta Verbal/fisiología , Comunicación , Emociones/fisiología , Femenino , Amigos/psicología , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Masculino , New Hampshire , Adulto Joven
16.
Autism Res ; 15(4): 677-686, 2022 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35048566

RESUMEN

Verbal fluency is a cognitive function reflecting executive functions and the ability to retrieve the appropriate information from memory quickly. Previous studies reported conflicting results-impaired and intact verbal fluency-in autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Most studies concentrate on overall word productivity, errors, perseverations, clustering, or switching. We used a comprehensive approach to evaluate the reported discrepancy in the literature and introduced a new angle using the concept of word abstraction and imageability. Moreover, we analyzed the performance in two-time intervals (0-30 s and 31-60 s) to assess the temporal dynamics of verbal fluency and a possible activation or initiation deficit in autism. Sixteen adults with ASD and 16 neurotypical control participants, matched by gender, age, and education level, participated in our study. Contrary to our expectations, we did not find a significant difference between groups in word productivity, the number of errors, clustering, or temporal dynamics, neither in semantic nor in phonemic fluency tasks. Surprisingly, the two study groups' performance did not differ in terms of imageability or concreteness characteristics either. Our results raise the possibility that verbal fluency performance is intact in autism. We also suggest using a comprehensive approach when measuring fluency in autism. LAY SUMMARY: People with autism tend to think and communicate differently. In our study, we tested whether people with autism come up with more concrete or imageable words and whether their performance is better compared with neurotypicals in the beginning or in the later phase of a task measuring how many words they can produce in a minute. We did not detect any difference between the two groups; however, we recommend studying verbal fluency in autism from more and different angles in the future.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Trastorno Autístico , Adulto , Humanos , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Fonética , Semántica , Conducta Verbal/fisiología
17.
Psychiatry Res ; 309: 114404, 2022 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35066310

RESUMEN

Linguistic abnormalities can emerge early in the course of psychotic illness. Computational tools that quantify similarity of responses in standardized language-based tasks such as the verbal fluency test could efficiently characterize the nature and functional correlates of these disturbances. Participants with early-stage psychosis (n=20) and demographically matched controls without a psychiatric diagnosis (n=20) performed category and letter verbal fluency. Semantic similarity was measured via predicted context co-occurrence in a large text corpus using Word2Vec. Phonetic similarity was measured via edit distance using the VFClust tool. Responses were designated as clusters (related items) or switches (transitions to less related items) using similarity-based thresholds. Results revealed that participants with early-stage psychosis compared to controls had lower fluency scores, lower cluster-related semantic similarity, and fewer switches; mean cluster size and phonetic similarity did not differ by group. Lower fluency semantic similarity was correlated with greater speech disorganization (Communication Disturbances Index), although more strongly in controls, and correlated with poorer social functioning (Global Functioning: Social), primarily in the psychosis group. Findings suggest that search for semantically related words may be impaired soon after psychosis onset. Future work is warranted to investigate the impact of language disturbances on social functioning over the course of psychotic illness.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Psicóticos , Semántica , Humanos , Lenguaje , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Fonética , Trastornos Psicóticos/complicaciones , Habla , Conducta Verbal/fisiología
18.
J Appl Behav Anal ; 55(2): 412-429, 2022 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34978335

RESUMEN

Intraverbal tacts are an example of multiply controlled verbal behavior. More specifically, they are verbal responses under control of both a nonverbal (visual) stimulus (e.g., a green ball) and a verbal (auditory) stimulus (e.g., "What color?" vs. "What shape?"). Studies have shown that verbal behavior training can be arranged in a way that would lead to the emergence of other verbal operants, including multiply controlled (convergent) intraverbals. Our study sought to evaluate the relevance of a specific set of component skills on the emergence of intraverbal tacts in children with an autism spectrum disorder. Intraverbal tacts were observed only when all component skills were mastered, suggesting that this set of skills was sufficient to produce emergent verbal performance. Preliminary data were obtained on the necessity of 4 of the 6 component skills and tentatively suggest that they may be necessary to produce emergent intraverbal tacts, at least under some conditions.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Niño , Humanos , Conducta Verbal/fisiología
19.
Clin Linguist Phon ; 36(10): 870-886, 2022 10 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34355620

RESUMEN

Data on clustering and switching during semantic fluency (SF) in patients with first-episode psychosis (PwFEP) are scant. We aimed to investigate (1) clustering and switching on SF in PwFEP using more detailed clustering analyses and (2) the possibility of disproportionate clustering deficits across different SF tasks in PwFEP and healthy subjects (HS), with the latter being suggested by the current literature on patients with schizophrenia. We recruited 22 Croatian-speaking PwFEP with schizophrenia features or symptoms and 22 HS matched in age, sex distribution, and handedness. All patients were medicated and had a mean illness duration of 1 month. The categories animals, trees, vegetables, fruits, and musical instruments were administered for SF. PwFEP produced significantly fewer correct words in the aggregate score, as well as across all categories. The switching rate was significantly higher in PwFEP, but no post hoc comparisons were significant. PwFEP also produced significantly smaller clusters, yet the post hoc comparisons for the tree and fruit task were not significant. A higher switching rate and smaller clusters indicate less efficient functional connectivity within subcategories of the given categories, but not necessarily between the subcategories. Although both less likely to produce a cluster once a switch has been uttered and less likely to produce clusters larger than two words compared to HS, the latter deficit was more pronounced. Our results further suggest that PwFEP might show normal clustering performance on some SF tasks. We discuss the results in the context of the hypothesis of semantic hyperactivation in psychoses.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Psicóticos , Semántica , Animales , Humanos , Pacientes Internos , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Conducta Verbal/fisiología
20.
Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg ; 166(1): 171-178, 2022 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34032520

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To use an automated speech-processing technology to identify patterns in sound environments and language output for deaf or hard-of-hearing infants and toddlers. STUDY DESIGN: Observational study based on a convenience sample. SETTING: Home observation conducted by tertiary children's hospital. METHODS: The system analyzed 115 naturalistic recordings of 28 children <3.5 years old. Hearing ability was stratified into groups by access to sound. Outcomes were compared across hearing groups, and multivariable linear regression was used to test associations. RESULTS: There was a significant difference in age-adjusted child vocalizations (P = .042), conversational turns (P = .022), and language development scores (P = .05) between hearing groups but no significant difference in adult words (P = .11). Conversational turns were positively associated with each language development measure, while adult words were not. For each hour of electronic media, there were significant reductions in child vocalizations (ß = -0.47; 95% CI, -0.71 to -0.19), conversational turns (ß = -0.45; 95% CI, -0.65 to -0.22), and language development (ß = -0.37; 95% CI, -0.61 to -0.15). CONCLUSIONS: Conversational turn scores differ among hearing groups and are positively associated with language development outcomes. Electronic media is associated with reduced discernible adult speech, child vocalizations, conversational turns, and language development scores. This effect was larger in children who are deaf or hard of hearing as compared with other reports in typically hearing populations. These findings underscore the need to optimize early language environments and limit electronic noise exposure in children who are deaf or hard of hearing.


Asunto(s)
Pérdida Auditiva/psicología , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Conducta Verbal/fisiología , Adulto , Preescolar , Femenino , Pérdida Auditiva/fisiopatología , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Grabaciones de Sonido , Medición de la Producción del Habla , Televisión
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA