Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 1.064
Filtrar
1.
PLoS One ; 19(6): e0302152, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38848421

RESUMEN

The prevalence of neurodegenerative disorders, particularly dementia, is on the rise across many countries worldwide. This negative trend calls for improving our understanding of cognitive aging. While motor-cognitive dual-task approaches have already been proven valuable for clinical diagnosis, comparatively less research is available on the application of Cognitive-Cognitive Dual-Tasking (CCDT), across several cognitive domains. Moreover, there is limited understanding about how healthy aging affects performance in such dual-tasks in the general population. CCDT entails engaging individuals in multiple cognitive tasks simultaneously and holds promise for remote e-Health interventions. In this cross-sectional study, our objective was to evaluate the suitability of a newly developed, self-administered, online tool for examining age-related differences in memory performance under dual-tasking. 337 healthy adults aged 50-90 underwent a visual memory test (Memo) under both single and dual-task conditions (attend to auditory letters). Additional measures included questionnaires on subjective memory complaints (MAC-Q), on cognitive reserve (CR), and a cognitive screening (auto-GEMS). As expected, the accuracy of visual memory performance exhibited a negative correlation with age and MAC-Q, and a positive correlation with CR and auto-GEMS scores. Dual-tasking significantly impaired performance, and its detrimental effect decreased with increasing age. Furthermore, the protective effect of cognitive reserve diminished with advancing age. These findings suggest that the commonly observed age-related increase in dual-task costs is not universally applicable across all tasks and cognitive domains. With further refinement, a longitudinal implementation of this approach may assist in identifying individuals with a distinct cognitive trajectory and potentially at a higher risk of developing cognitive decline.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Cognición , Humanos , Estudios Transversales , Anciano , Masculino , Femenino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Cognición/fisiología , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Envejecimiento/psicología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Memoria/fisiología , Reserva Cognitiva/fisiología , Envejecimiento Cognitivo/fisiología
2.
BMC Psychol ; 12(1): 334, 2024 Jun 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38849930

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) describes an aging profile characterized by a cognitive decline that is worse than expected in normal aging but less pervasive and critical than full-blown dementia. In the absence of an effective treatment strategy, it is important to identify factors that can protect against progression to dementia. In this field, it is hypothesized that one aspect that may be a protective factor against the neurotypical outcome of dementia is cognitive reserve (CR). Cognitive reserve is the ability to maintain cognitive functionality despite accumulating brain pathology. OBJECTIVES: The present study aimed to identify and analyze the differences in CR between healthy adults and patients with MCI. Specifically, it is hypothesized that (i) healthy older adult people have higher CR than older adult people diagnosed with MCI, and (II) CR could predict the classification of subjects into people with or without MCI. METHODS: Two hundred forty-three adults (mean age = 60.4, SD = 7.4) participated in the present study and were classified into three groups based on Petersen's MCI criteria: healthy controls (HC), amnestic MCI (aMCI), and non-amnestic MCI (naMCI). The Cognitive Reserve Index questionnaire (CRIq) was administered to assess the level of CR, FINDINGS: Results showed that HC had significantly higher CR scores than participants diagnosed with aMCI and naMCI. Moreover, a binomial logistic regression suggested that low CR was a significant risk factor for the MCI diagnosis. CONCLUSIONS: The clinical picture that emerged from the results showed that lower CR could be considered a characteristic of pathological aging, such as MCI.Public significance statement, Since the brain attempts to cope with life-related changes or pathologies, it is fundamental for both clinicians and researchers to investigate further the factors that contribute to brain resilience. As an indirect expression of brain reserve, cognitive reserve may be both a marker and a predictor of adaptive aging.


Asunto(s)
Disfunción Cognitiva , Reserva Cognitiva , Humanos , Reserva Cognitiva/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Anciano , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Envejecimiento/psicología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
3.
J Pak Med Assoc ; 74(5): 1009-1012, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38783461

RESUMEN

The recent advancements in medical sciences has resulted in not only increasing life expectancy of the elderly but has also improved survival rate in elderly with neurological disorders including those with head trauma . This has resulted in an increasing number of persons with cognitive deficits. Cognitive functions such as executive functioning and memory play an important role in success of a rehabilitation programme and therefore can positively contribute to public health goals. Considering cognitive decline at present has no cure and pharmacological therapies have a limited role, efforts are usually made to delay the onset and progression of cognitive decline and improve quality of life. Literature suggests that active life style, regular exercise, actively performing activities of daily living can have a significant impact on cognitive skills. In addition different models of cognitive rehabilitation and approaches can be integrated into practice to improve cognitive reserve and cause neuroplastic changes to facilitate cognitive function by providing cognitive stimulus and training. Moreover with technological advancements, the computerized cognitive intervention field is growing. This usually integrates conventional cognitive intervention with digital smart devices to provide an engaging and cost effective alternate approach. This review aims to highlight the importance of cognitive rehabilitation and suggest a few evidence based approaches that may be considered by rehabilitation professionals to promote and improve cognitive rehabilitation in Pakistan.


Asunto(s)
Disfunción Cognitiva , Humanos , Disfunción Cognitiva/rehabilitación , Disfunción Cognitiva/terapia , Anciano , Actividades Cotidianas , Función Ejecutiva , Reserva Cognitiva , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual/métodos , Remediación Cognitiva/métodos , Cognición , Pakistán , Calidad de Vida , Memoria , Entrenamiento Cognitivo
4.
Neurobiol Aging ; 140: 60-69, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38733869

RESUMEN

We tested if cognitive and brain reserve and maintenance explain individual differences in episodic memory and other cognitive domains from late middle to early older adulthood. We used The Vietnam Era Twin Study of Aging data (n=1604 men) with episodic memory measured at mean ages of 56, 62 and 68 years, and magnetic resonance imaging data for a subsample of participants (n=321). Cognitive reserve -young adult general cognitive ability at a mean age of 20 years and, to a lesser degree, educational attainment- was positively related to episodic memory performance at each assessment, but not to memory change. We found no evidence for the associations of brain reserve or brain maintenance on memory change. Results were highly similar when looking at processing speed, executive function and verbal fluency. In conclusion, higher young adult cognitive reserve was related to better episodic memory in midlife and older adulthood, but it did not confer better cognitive maintenance with respect to memory. This supports the importance of early cognitive development in dementia prevention.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Encéfalo , Cognición , Reserva Cognitiva , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Memoria Episódica , Humanos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Masculino , Reserva Cognitiva/fisiología , Envejecimiento/psicología , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Anciano , Cognición/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Adulto Joven , Envejecimiento Cognitivo/fisiología , Envejecimiento Cognitivo/psicología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Demencia/psicología
5.
Alzheimers Dement ; 20(6): 4020-4031, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38690777

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: The effects of sleep-wake behavior on perceived fatigability and cognitive abilities when performing daily activities have not been investigated across levels of cognitive reserve (CR). METHODS: CR Index Questionnaire (CRIq) data were collected and subjected to moderated mediation analysis. RESULTS: In amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI; n = 41), CR moderated sleep-related impairments (SRIs), and fatigability at low CR (CRIq < 105.8, p = 0.004) and mean CR (CRIq = 126.9, p = 0.03) but not high CR (CRIq > 145.9, p = 0.65) levels. SRI affected cognitive abilities mediated by fatigability at low CR (p < 0.001) and mean CR (p = 0.003) levels. In healthy controls (n = 13), SRI in fatigability did not alter cognitive abilities across CR levels; controls had higher leisure scores than patients with aMCI (p = 0.003, effect size = 0.93). DISCUSSION: SRI can amplify impaired cognitive abilities through exacerbation of fatigability in patients with aMCI with below-mean CR. Therefore, improving sleep-wake regulation and leisure activities may protect against fatigability and cognitive decline. HIGHLIGHTS: Clinical fatigue and fatigability cannot be alleviated by rest. Clinical fatigability disrupts daily activities during preclinical Alzheimer's. High cognitive reserve mitigates sleep-wake disturbance effects. High cognitive reserve attenuates clinical fatigability effects on daily functioning. Untreated obstructive sleep apnea potentiates Alzheimer's pathology in the brain.


Asunto(s)
Disfunción Cognitiva , Reserva Cognitiva , Fatiga , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Reserva Cognitiva/fisiología , Anciano , Fatiga/fisiopatología , Disfunción Cognitiva/fisiopatología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Sueño/fisiología , Trastornos del Sueño-Vigilia/fisiopatología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/estadística & datos numéricos , Actividades Cotidianas , Anciano de 80 o más Años
6.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38721999

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: Older adult executive function varies widely due to brain and cognitive aging. Variance in older adult executive function is linked to increased response conflict from cognitive and brain aging. Cognitive reserve (CR) is a theoretical protective mechanism that lessens brain aging's impact on cognition and is associated with greater educational attainment. Recent work in rest-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) suggests CR proxies moderate the relationship between functional connectivity (FC) and cognitive performance. Brain network FC in "control networks," including the salience (SN), dorsal attention and frontoparietal networks, are associated with cognitive processes in older adults. CR is hypothesized to maintain cognitive processing in part through changes in how brain networks respond to cognitive demands. However, it is unclear how CR proxies like educational attainment are related to control network FC during performance when cognitive demands are increased relative to rest. Because CR is expressed more in those with higher education, we hypothesized stronger control network FC would relate to better performance, where this relationship would be strongest among the most educated. METHODS: We collected flanker task data during fMRI to assess the impact of a CR proxy (i.e., educational attainment) on response conflict among older adult subjects (n = 42, age = 65-80). RESULTS: Linear mixed-effects models showed more educated older adults with greater SN-FC had a smaller flanker effect (i.e., less influence of distractors; p < .001) during task performance. DISCUSSION: For the first time, we show that educational attainment moderates the relationship between task-state SN-FC and executive function among older adults.


Asunto(s)
Reserva Cognitiva , Escolaridad , Función Ejecutiva , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Humanos , Masculino , Anciano , Femenino , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Reserva Cognitiva/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/diagnóstico por imagen , Conflicto Psicológico , Envejecimiento Cognitivo/fisiología , Envejecimiento Cognitivo/psicología , Anciano de 80 o más Años
7.
J Prev Alzheimers Dis ; 11(3): 739-748, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38706290

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Cognitive reserve (CR) contributes to preserving cognition when facing brain aging and damage. CR has been linked to dementia risk in late life. However, the association between CR and cognitive changes and brain imaging measures, especially in midlife, is unclear. OBJECTIVE: We aimed to explore the association of CR with cognitive decline and structural brain differences in middle and older age. DESIGN: This longitudinal study was from the UK Biobank project where participants completed baseline surveys between 2006 to 2010 and were followed (mean follow-up: 9 years). SETTING: A population-based study. PARTICIPANTS: A total of 42,301 dementia-free participants aged 40-70 were followed-up to detect cognitive changes. A subsample (n=34,041) underwent brain magnetic resonance imaging scans. MEASUREMENTS: We used latent class analysis to generate a CR indicator (categorized as high, moderate, and low) based on education, occupation, and multiple cognitively stimulating activities. Cognitive tests for global and domain-specific cognition were administrated at baseline and follow-up. Total brain, white matter, grey matter, hippocampal, and white matter hyperintensity volumes (TBV, WMV, GMV, HV, and WMHV) were assessed at the follow-up examination. Data were analyzed using mixed-effects models and analysis of covariance. RESULTS: At baseline, 16,032 (37.9%), 10,709 (25.3%), and 15,560 (36.8%) participants had low, moderate, and high levels of CR, respectively. Compared with low CR, high CR was associated with slower declines in global cognition (ß [95% confidence interval]: 0.10 [0.08, 0.11]), prospective memory (0.10 [0.06, 0.15]), fluid intelligence (0.07 [0.04, 0.10]), and reaction time (0.04 [0.02, 0.06]). Participants with high CR had lower TBV, WMV, GMV, and WMHV, but higher HV when controlling for global cognition (corrected P <0.01 for all). The significant relationships between CR and cognition and TBV were present among both middle-aged (<60 years) and older (≥60 years) participants. The CR-cognition association remained significant despite reductions in brain structural properties. CONCLUSIONS: Higher CR is associated with slower cognitive decline, higher HV, and lower microvascular burden, especially in middle age. Individuals with high CR could tolerate smaller brain volumes while maintaining cognition. The benefit of CR for cognition is independent of structural brain differences. Our findings highlight the contribution of enhancing CR to helping compensate for neuroimaging alterations and ultimately prevent cognitive decline.


Asunto(s)
Bancos de Muestras Biológicas , Encéfalo , Disfunción Cognitiva , Reserva Cognitiva , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Humanos , Reserva Cognitiva/fisiología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Disfunción Cognitiva/diagnóstico por imagen , Masculino , Reino Unido/epidemiología , Femenino , Anciano , Estudios Longitudinales , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Encéfalo/patología , Adulto , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Biobanco del Reino Unido
8.
Neurology ; 102(9): e209353, 2024 May 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38630959

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: The cognitive reserve hypothesis posits that cognitively stimulating work delays the onset of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia. However, the effect of occupational cognitive demands across midlife on the risk of these conditions is unclear. METHODS: Using a cohort study design, we evaluated the association between registry-based trajectories of occupational cognitive demands from ages 30-65 years and clinically diagnosed MCI and dementia in participants in the HUNT4 70+ Study (2017-19). Group-based trajectory modeling identified trajectories of occupational cognitive demands, measured by the routine task intensity (RTI) index (lower RTI indicates more cognitively demanding occupation) from the Occupational Information Network. Multinomial regression was implemented to estimate the relative risk ratios (RRRs) of MCI and dementia, after adjusting for age, sex, education, income, baseline hypertension, obesity, diabetes, psychiatric impairment, hearing impairment, loneliness, smoking status, and physical inactivity assessed at HUNT1-2 in 1984-1986 and 1995-1997. To handle missing data, we used inverse probability weighting to account for nonparticipation in cognitive testing and multiple imputation. RESULTS: Based on longitudinal RTI scores for 305 unique occupations, 4 RTI trajectory groups were identified (n = 7,003, 49.8% women, age range 69-104 years): low RTI (n = 1,431, 20.4%), intermediate-low RTI (n = 1,578, 22.5%), intermediate-high RTI (n = 2,601, 37.1%), and high RTI (n = 1,393, 19.9%). Participants in the high RTI group had a higher risk of MCI (RRR 1.74, 95% CI 1.41-2.14) and dementia (RRR 1.37, 95% CI 1.01-1.86), after adjusting for age, sex, and education compared with participants in the low RTI group. In a sensitivity analysis, controlling for income and baseline health-related factors, the point estimates were not appreciably changed (RRR 1.66, 95% CI 1.35-2.06 for MCI, and RRR 1.31, 95% CI 0.96-1.78 for dementia). DISCUSSION: People with a history of cognitively stimulating occupations during their 30s, 40s, 50s, and 60s had a lower risk of MCI and dementia older than 70 years, highlighting the importance of occupational cognitive stimulation during midlife for maintaining cognitive function in old age. Further research is required to pinpoint the specific occupational cognitive demands that are most advantageous for maintaining later-life cognitive function.


Asunto(s)
Disfunción Cognitiva , Reserva Cognitiva , Demencia , Humanos , Femenino , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Masculino , Estudios de Cohortes , Disfunción Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Cognición
9.
Alzheimers Res Ther ; 16(1): 87, 2024 Apr 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38654379

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: While evidence supports cognitive reserve (CR) in preserving cognitive function, longitudinal validation of CR proxies, including later-life factors, remains scarce. This study aims to validate CR's stability over time and its relation to cognitive function in rural Chinese older adults. METHODS: Within the project on the health status of rural older adults (HSRO), the survey included baseline assessment (2019) and follow-up assessment (2022). 792 older adults (mean age: 70.23 years) were followed up. The confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was constructed using cognitive reserve proxies that included years of formal education, social support, hobbies, and exercise. We examined the longitudinal validity of the CR factor using confirmatory factor analyses and measurement invariance and explored the association of CR with cognition using Spearman's correlation and Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE). RESULTS: The results showed that CR's CFA structure was stable over time (T0, χ2/df: 3.21/2; RMSEA: 0.02, and T1, χ2/df: 7.47/2; RMSEA: 0.05) and that it accepted both configural and metric invariance (Δχ2/df = 2.28/3, P = 0.52). In addition, it was found that CR had a stable positive relationship with cognitive function across time (T0, r = 0.54; T1, r = 0.49). Furthermore, longitudinal CR were associated with MMSE (ß = 2.25; 95%CI = 2.01 ~ 2.49). CONCLUSIONS: This study provided valuable evidence on the stability and validity of cognitive reserve proxy measures in rural Chinese older adults. Our findings suggested that cognitive reserve is associated with cognitive function over time and highlighted the importance of accumulating cognitive reserve in later life.


Asunto(s)
Reserva Cognitiva , Población Rural , Humanos , Reserva Cognitiva/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Anciano , Estudios Longitudinales , China , Estudios de Cohortes , Persona de Mediana Edad , Disfunción Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Disfunción Cognitiva/epidemiología , Disfunción Cognitiva/psicología , Cognición/fisiología , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Pueblos del Este de Asia
10.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 161: 105649, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38579902

RESUMEN

With dementia incidence projected to escalate significantly within the next 25 years, the United Nations declared 2021-2030 the Decade of Healthy Ageing, emphasising cognition as a crucial element. As a leading discipline in cognition and ageing research, psychology is well-equipped to offer insights for translational research, clinical practice, and policy-making. In this comprehensive review, we discuss the current state of knowledge on age-related changes in cognition and psychological health. We discuss cognitive changes during ageing, including (a) heterogeneity in the rate, trajectory, and characteristics of decline experienced by older adults, (b) the role of cognitive reserve in age-related cognitive decline, and (c) the potential for cognitive training to slow this decline. We also examine ageing and cognition through multiple theoretical perspectives. We highlight critical unresolved issues, such as the disparate implications of subjective versus objective measures of cognitive decline and the insufficient evaluation of cognitive training programs. We suggest future research directions, and emphasise interdisciplinary collaboration to create a more comprehensive understanding of the factors that modulate cognitive ageing.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Envejecimiento Saludable , Humanos , Envejecimiento Saludable/fisiología , Envejecimiento Saludable/psicología , Cognición/fisiología , Disfunción Cognitiva/fisiopatología , Reserva Cognitiva/fisiología , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Envejecimiento Cognitivo/fisiología
11.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 161: 105672, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38608829

RESUMEN

Cognitive reserve has shown promise as a justification for neuropathologically unexplainable clinical outcomes in Alzheimer's disease. Recent evidence suggests this effect may be replicated in conditions like Parkinson's disease, dementia with Lewy bodies, and multiple system atrophy. However, the relationships between cognitive reserve and different cognitive abilities, as well as motor outcomes, are still poorly understood in these conditions. Additionally, it is unclear whether the reported effects are confounded by medication. This review analysed studies investigating the relationship between cognitive reserve and clinical outcomes in these α-synucleinopathy cohorts, identified from MEDLINE, Scopus, psycINFO, CINAHL, and Web of Science. 85 records, containing 176 cognition and 31 motor function effect sizes, were pooled using multilevel meta-analysis. There was a significant, positive association between higher cognitive reserve and both better cognition and motor function. Cognition effect sizes differed by disease subtype, cognitive reserve measure, and outcome type; however, no moderators significantly impacted motor function. Review findings highlight the clinical implications of cognitive reserve and importance of engaging in reserve-building behaviours.


Asunto(s)
Reserva Cognitiva , Humanos , Reserva Cognitiva/fisiología , Sinucleinopatías/fisiopatología , Cognición/fisiología , Enfermedad de Parkinson/fisiopatología , Enfermedad de Parkinson/complicaciones
12.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 45(5): e26658, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38520368

RESUMEN

Cognitive reserve (CR) explains differential susceptibility of cognitive performance to neuropathology. However, as brain pathologies progress, cognitive decline occurs even in individuals with initially high CR. The interplay between the structural brain health (= level of brain reserve) and CR-related brain networks therefore requires further research. Our sample included 142 individuals aged 60-70 years. National Adult Reading Test intelligence quotient (NART-IQ) was our CR proxy. On an in-scanner Letter Sternberg task, we used ordinal trend (OrT) analysis to extract a task-related brain activation pattern (OrT slope) for each participant that captures increased expression with task load (one, three, and six letters). We assessed whether OrT slope represents a neural mechanism underlying CR by associating it with task performance and NART-IQ. Additionally, we investigated how the following brain reserve measures affect the association between NART-IQ and OrT slope: mean cortical thickness, total gray matter volume, and brain volumes proximal to the areas contained in the OrT patterns. We found that higher OrT slope was associated with better task performance and higher NART-IQ. Further, the brain reserve measures were not directly associated with OrT slope, but they affected the relationship between NART-IQ and OrT slope: NART-IQ was associated with OrT slope only in individuals with high brain reserve. The degree of brain reserve has an impact on how (and perhaps whether) CR can be implemented in brain networks in older individuals.


Asunto(s)
Reserva Cognitiva , Adulto , Humanos , Anciano , Reserva Cognitiva/fisiología , Pruebas de Inteligencia , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Escalas de Wechsler , Mapeo Encefálico
13.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 45(5): e26634, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38553856

RESUMEN

Cerebral small vessel disease (SVD) can disrupt the global brain network and lead to cognitive impairment. Conversely, cognitive reserve (CR) can improve one's cognitive ability to handle damaging effects like SVD, partly by optimizing the brain network's organization. Understanding how SVD and CR collectively influence brain networks could be instrumental in preventing cognitive impairment. Recently, brain redundancy has emerged as a critical network protective metric, providing a nuanced perspective of changes in network organization. However, it remains unclear how SVD and CR affect global redundancy and subsequently cognitive function. Here, we included 121 community-dwelling participants who underwent neuropsychological assessments and a multimodal MRI examination. We visually examined common SVD imaging markers and assessed lifespan CR using the Cognitive Reserve Index Questionnaire. We quantified the global redundancy index (RI) based on the dynamic functional connectome. We then conducted multiple linear regressions to explore the specific cognitive domains related to RI and the associations of RI with SVD and CR. We also conducted mediation analyses to explore whether RI mediated the relationships between SVD, CR, and cognition. We found negative correlations of RI with the presence of microbleeds (MBs) and the SVD total score, and a positive correlation of RI with leisure activity-related CR (CRI-leisure). RI was positively correlated with memory and fully mediated the relationships between the MBs, CRI-leisure, and memory. Our study highlights the potential benefits of promoting leisure activities and keeping brain redundancy for memory preservation in older adults, especially those with SVD.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedades de los Pequeños Vasos Cerebrales , Disfunción Cognitiva , Reserva Cognitiva , Humanos , Anciano , Persona de Mediana Edad , Cognición , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Disfunción Cognitiva/psicología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Enfermedades de los Pequeños Vasos Cerebrales/complicaciones
14.
Geriatr Gerontol Int ; 24(4): 398-403, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38475988

RESUMEN

AIM: We investigated the effect of lifespan cognitive reserve and its components on cognitive frailty among older adults. METHODS: A total of 4922 participants aged ≥65 years were recruited in 2008 and were followed up in 2011 from the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey. Cognitive frailty was determined through the simultaneous presence of physical frailty (pre-frailty or frailty) and mild cognitive impairment, excluding concurrent dementia. The assessment of physical frailty and mild cognitive impairment was based on the Fatigue, Resistence, Ambulation, Illness, Loss of weight (FRAIL) (Fatigue, Resistence, Ambulation, Illness, Loss) and Mini-Mental State Examination scale, respectively. The lifespan cognitive reserve consisted of education attainment, occupational complexity and later-life leisure activities. We used logistic regression models to estimate the risk of cognitive frailty associated with the lifespan cognitive reserve and its components. RESULTS: A higher level of lifespan cognitive reserve, higher educational attainment or leisure activities engagement, but not occupational complexity, were associated with lower risk of incident cognitive frailty. Furthermore, cognitive, social and physical activities were associated with lower risk of incident cognitive frailty. CONCLUSION: Cognitive reserve, particularly educational attainment and leisure activities, can protect from cognitive frailty. This implicates that individuals should accumulate cognitive reserve in their lifespan, and older adults should actively participate in leisure activities to prevent cognitive frailty. Geriatr Gerontol Int 2024; 24: 398-403.


Asunto(s)
Disfunción Cognitiva , Reserva Cognitiva , Fragilidad , Humanos , Anciano , Fragilidad/diagnóstico , Estudios Prospectivos , Disfunción Cognitiva/epidemiología , Disfunción Cognitiva/psicología , Estudios Longitudinales , Evaluación Geriátrica , Anciano Frágil/psicología
15.
Percept Mot Skills ; 131(3): 720-736, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38523555

RESUMEN

In this study we investigated the relationship between cognitive reserve (CR) proxies, such as education, physical activity (PA), and cognitive dysfunction (CD) in the presence or absence of frontotemporal disorders (FTD). Previous research has suggested that education and PA may delay the onset of CD and reduce the risk of developing dementia. However, it remains unclear whether these CR proxies can protect against CD when FTD is present. We aimed to explore this relationship and determine whether sustained CR may be evident regardless of FTD. We recruited 149 older adults (aged 65-99 years) from community centers where they were voluntarily participating in leisure activities. We used bioelectrical impedance to measure their body composition, and we administered the International PA Questionnaire and the Mini-Mental State Examination to measure their PA and cognitive function, respectively. We used the Frontal Assessment Battery to screen for frontotemporal dementia. Our results showed that people with FTD were older, had lower education, and engaged in less PA, relative to other participants. Regression models revealed that age, education, and PA were significant predictors of FTD. More specifically, FTD was negatively associated with cognitive functioning, and there were significant interaction effects between FTD and education and PA. PA and education were significant predictors of cognitive functioning, and, when values for PA and education were high, they offset the effects of FTD on cognitive function. These findings support impressions that PA and years of education provide an insulating or compensatory effect on cognitive functioning in older adults with executive dysfunction or frontotemporal dementia, highlighting the importance of encouraging both pursuits.


Asunto(s)
Disfunción Cognitiva , Reserva Cognitiva , Escolaridad , Ejercicio Físico , Demencia Frontotemporal , Humanos , Anciano , Reserva Cognitiva/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Ejercicio Físico/fisiología , Ejercicio Físico/psicología , Demencia Frontotemporal/psicología
16.
Brain Inj ; 38(7): 550-558, 2024 Jun 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38481123

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Older adults (OA) after mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) have a high risk of developing persistent post-injury cognitive impairments. Lower pre-morbid cognitive reserve (CR) is increasingly investigated as a risk factor for cognitive dysfunction in OA. However, how CR protects against effects of mTBI at the brain level remains largely understudied. METHODS: We examined 22 OA who sustained mTBI (mean 67.69 years, SD 5.11) in the sub-acute phase and 15 age- and CR-matched healthy OA (mean 68 years, SD 5.55) performing a three-level visual N-back task using electroencephalography. We calculated inverse efficiency scores of performance from accuracy and reaction times. Event-related potentials served as neurocognitive correlates of attentional (P2) and working memory (P3) processing. RESULTS: Overall, mTBI OA performed worse than healthy OA (p = 0.031). Lower CR generally decreased performance (p < 0.001). Furthermore, with increasing task difficulty, task performance was more affected by CR (p = 0.004). At the brain level, P2 amplitude was lower in mTBI OA than in healthy OA (p = 0.05). There was no clear effect of CR on P2 or P3 measures. CONCLUSION: As mTBI OA with lower CR performed worse on a working-memory task, lower CR may be a risk factor for worse recovery after mTBI in this group.


Asunto(s)
Conmoción Encefálica , Reserva Cognitiva , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Reserva Cognitiva/fisiología , Anciano , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Conmoción Encefálica/fisiopatología , Conmoción Encefálica/psicología , Conmoción Encefálica/complicaciones , Persona de Mediana Edad , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Disfunción Cognitiva/etiología , Disfunción Cognitiva/fisiopatología , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Trastornos de la Memoria/fisiopatología
17.
Alzheimers Dement ; 20(5): 3567-3586, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38477378

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: This review examines the concept of cognitive reserve (CR) in relation to brain aging, particularly in the context of dementia and its early stages. CR refers to an individual's ability to maintain or regain cognitive function despite brain aging, damage, or disease. Various factors, including education, occupation complexity, leisure activities, and genetics are believed to influence CR. METHODS: We revised the literature in the context of CR. A total of 842 articles were identified, then we rigorously assessed the relevance of articles based on titles and abstracts, employing a systematic approach to eliminate studies that did not align with our research objectives. RESULTS: We evaluate-also in a critical way-the methods commonly used to define and measure CR, including sociobehavioral proxies, neuroimaging, and electrophysiological and genetic measures. The challenges and limitations of these measures are discussed, emphasizing the need for more targeted research to improve the understanding, definition, and measurement of CR. CONCLUSIONS: The review underscores the significance of comprehending CR in the context of both normal and pathological brain aging and emphasizes the importance of further research to identify and enhance this protective factor for cognitive preservation in both healthy and neurologically impaired older individuals. HIGHLIGHTS: This review examines the concept of cognitive reserve in brain aging, in the context of dementia and its early stages. We have evaluated the methods commonly used to define and measure cognitive reserve. Sociobehavioral proxies, neuroimaging, and electrophysiological and genetic measures are discussed. The review emphasizes the importance of further research to identify and enhance this protective factor for cognitive preservation.


Asunto(s)
Reserva Cognitiva , Humanos , Reserva Cognitiva/fisiología , Demencia , Encéfalo/fisiología , Neuroimagen , Envejecimiento/fisiología
18.
Psychol Res ; 88(4): 1081-1091, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38315217

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: A common belief among people and some researchers is that keeping yourself mentally active may decrease the risk of dementia. Over the past years, despite widespread efforts to identify proxies for protecting cognitive reserve against age-related changes, it is still not clear what type of intellectual activity would be beneficial for cognitive reserve. To fill this gap, we propose a three-dimensional model of intellectual activity. According to this conceptual model, intellectual activities could be distinguished based on their locations in a three-dimensions space, including; (1) Activation: active vs. passive, (2) Novelty: novel vs. familiar, and (3) Productivity: productive vs. receptive. We assumed that the activities that are categorized as more active, novel, and productive could be considered as a cognitive reserve proxy. METHODS: To test this hypothesis, a sample of 237 participants older than 50 years (Mage = 58.76 ± 6.66; 63.7% women) was recruited to take part in the study. Episodic, semantic and working memory were assessed with computerized battery tests (Sepidar) and a self-report questionnaire was used to assess intellectual activities. Activities were categorized in terms of; (1) passive, familiar, and receptive activities (radio/watching TV), (2) active, familiar, and receptive activities (solving crosswords), (3) active, novel, and receptive activities (reading), and (4) active, novel, and productive activities (writing). RESULTS: The results indicated that writing moderates the effect of age on episodic and semantic memory. Reading only moderates the effect of age on semantic memory, and radio/watching TV and solving crosswords do not play a role in moderation analysis. CONCLUSIONS: Our finding suggests that intellectual activities have different moderating effects on the relationships between age and memory performance. Individuals with high levels of participation in novel and productive activities over the life course are less likely to clinically demonstrate cognitive impairments. Our results support the potential benefit of the three-dimensional model to provide a better insight into the complex role of intellectual activities in cognitive reserve, particularly for older adults. Further research is needed to evaluate the efficacy and the benefits of the model.


Asunto(s)
Reserva Cognitiva , Humanos , Reserva Cognitiva/fisiología , Femenino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Masculino , Anciano , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Memoria Episódica , Modelos Psicológicos
19.
Br J Psychiatry ; 224(6): 213-220, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38328972

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: It remains unclear whether cognitive reserve can attenuate dementia risk among people with different genetic predispositions. AIMS: We aimed to examine the association between cognitive reserve and dementia, and further to explore whether and to what extent cognitive reserve may modify the risk effect of genetic factors on dementia. METHOD: Within the UK Biobank, 210 631 dementia-free participants aged ≥60 years were followed to detect incident dementia. Dementia was ascertained through medical and death records. A composite cognitive reserve indicator encompassing education, occupation and multiple cognitively loaded activities was created using latent class analysis, categorised as low, moderate and high level. Polygenic risk scores for Alzheimer's disease were constructed to evaluate genetic risk for dementia, categorised by tertiles (high, moderate and low). Data were analysed using Cox models and Laplace regression. RESULTS: In multi-adjusted Cox models, the hazard ratio (HR) of dementia was 0.66 (95% confidence interval (CI) 0.61-0.70) for high cognitive reserve compared with low cognitive reserve. In Laplace regression, participants with high cognitive reserve developed dementia 1.62 (95% CI 1.35-1.88) years later than those with low cognitive reserve. In stratified analysis by genetic risk, high cognitive reserve was related to more than 30% lower dementia risk compared with low cognitive reserve in each stratum. There was an additive interaction between low cognitive reserve and high genetic risk on dementia (attributable proportion 0.24, 95% CI 0.17-0.31). CONCLUSIONS: High cognitive reserve is associated with reduced risk of dementia and may delay dementia onset. Genetic risk for dementia may be mitigated by high cognitive reserve. Our findings underscore the importance of enhancing cognitive reserve in dementia prevention.


Asunto(s)
Reserva Cognitiva , Demencia , Herencia Multifactorial , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Demencia/genética , Demencia/epidemiología , Predisposición Genética a la Enfermedad , Modelos de Riesgos Proporcionales , Factores de Riesgo , Biobanco del Reino Unido , Reino Unido/epidemiología
20.
J Appl Res Intellect Disabil ; 37(2): e13204, 2024 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38361365

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Cognitive reserve (CR) has not been studied in people with Intellectual Developmental Disability, a population with a high incidence of dementia. Commonly adopted CR proxies should be adapted to reflect more specifically the experiences of people with Intellectual Developmental Disability. METHOD: This scoping review intended to identify CR proxies relevant to people with this condition. RESULTS: Some of these were the same already detected in a population without intellectual disabilities (education, occupation, physical activity, leisure, community and social activities); others were found to be specifically relevant for this population: type of schooling, parental educational level, environmental stimulation and living place. CONCLUSIONS: These proxies need to be considered in studies on CR and Intellectual Developmental Disability and in clinical practice. Research on the protective effect of CR aims to encourage policies promoting lifestyle-based educational and preventive interventions and overcome participation barriers for people with Intellectual Developmental Disability.


Asunto(s)
Reserva Cognitiva , Discapacidad Intelectual , Humanos , Niño , Reserva Cognitiva/fisiología , Discapacidades del Desarrollo , Actividades Recreativas , Ejercicio Físico
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA