Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Chimpanzee ethnography reveals unexpected cultural diversity.
Boesch, Christophe; Kalan, Ammie K; Mundry, Roger; Arandjelovic, Mimi; Pika, Simone; Dieguez, Paula; Ayimisin, Emmanuel Ayuk; Barciela, Amanda; Coupland, Charlotte; Egbe, Villard Ebot; Eno-Nku, Manasseh; Michael Fay, J; Fine, David; Adriana Hernandez-Aguilar, R; Hermans, Veerle; Kadam, Parag; Kambi, Mohamed; Llana, Manuel; Maretti, Giovanna; Morgan, David; Murai, Mizuki; Neil, Emily; Nicholl, Sonia; Ormsby, Lucy Jayne; Orume, Robinson; Pacheco, Liliana; Piel, Alex; Sanz, Crickette; Sciaky, Lilah; Stewart, Fiona A; Tagg, Nikki; Wessling, Erin G; Willie, Jacob; Kühl, Hjalmar S.
Afiliación
  • Boesch C; Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany. boesch@eva.mpg.de.
  • Kalan AK; Wild Chimpanzee Foundation, Leipzig, Germany. boesch@eva.mpg.de.
  • Mundry R; Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
  • Arandjelovic M; Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
  • Pika S; Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
  • Dieguez P; Institute for Cognitive Science, Comparative Biocognition, University of Osnabrück, Osnabrück, Germany.
  • Ayimisin EA; Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
  • Barciela A; Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
  • Coupland C; Station Biologique Fouta Djallon, Instituto Jane Goodall España, Dindéfélo, Senegal.
  • Egbe VE; Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
  • Eno-Nku M; Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
  • Michael Fay J; WWF Kudu Zombo Programme, Yaoundé, Cameroon.
  • Fine D; Agence Nationale des Parcs Nationaux, Libreville, Gabon.
  • Adriana Hernandez-Aguilar R; WWF Kudu Zombo Programme, Yaoundé, Cameroon.
  • Hermans V; Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary Synthesis, Department of Biosciences, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway.
  • Kadam P; Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium.
  • Kambi M; Department of Archaeology and Anthropology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.
  • Llana M; Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
  • Maretti G; Station Biologique Fouta Djallon, Instituto Jane Goodall España, Dindéfélo, Senegal.
  • Morgan D; Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
  • Murai M; Lester E. Fisher Center for the Study and Conservation of Apes, Chicago, IL, USA.
  • Neil E; Congo Program, Wildlife Conservation Society, Brazzaville, Republic of Congo.
  • Nicholl S; Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
  • Ormsby LJ; Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
  • Orume R; Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
  • Pacheco L; Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
  • Piel A; Korup Rainforest Conservation Society, Mundemba, Cameroon.
  • Sanz C; Station Biologique Fouta Djallon, Instituto Jane Goodall España, Dindéfélo, Senegal.
  • Sciaky L; School of Natural Sciences and Psychology, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK.
  • Stewart FA; Congo Program, Wildlife Conservation Society, Brazzaville, Republic of Congo.
  • Tagg N; Department of Anthropology, Washington University, St. Louis, MO, USA.
  • Wessling EG; Kyoto University Institute for Advanced Study, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan.
  • Willie J; Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
  • Kühl HS; School of Natural Sciences and Psychology, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK.
Nat Hum Behav ; 4(9): 910-916, 2020 09.
Article en En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32451479
Human ethnographic knowledge covers hundreds of societies, whereas chimpanzee ethnography encompasses at most 15 communities. Using termite fishing as a window into the richness of chimpanzee cultural diversity, we address a potential sampling bias with 39 additional communities across Africa. Previously, termite fishing was known from eight locations with two distinguishable techniques observed in only two communities. Here, we add nine termite-fishing communities not studied before, revealing 38 different technical elements, as well as community-specific combinations of three to seven elements. Thirty of those were not ecologically constrained, permitting the investigation of chimpanzee termite-fishing culture. The number and combination of elements shared among individuals were more similar within communities than between them, thus supporting community-majority conformity via social imitation. The variation in community-specific combinations of elements parallels cultural diversity in human greeting norms or chopstick etiquette. We suggest that termite fishing in wild chimpanzees shows some elements of cumulative cultural diversity.
Asunto(s)

Texto completo: 1 Base de datos: MEDLINE Asunto principal: Conducta Social / Diversidad Cultural Tipo de estudio: Qualitative_research Idioma: En Revista: Nat Hum Behav Año: 2020 Tipo del documento: Article

Texto completo: 1 Base de datos: MEDLINE Asunto principal: Conducta Social / Diversidad Cultural Tipo de estudio: Qualitative_research Idioma: En Revista: Nat Hum Behav Año: 2020 Tipo del documento: Article