Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 9 de 9
Filtrar
1.
Ear Hear ; 37(1): e37-51, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26317298

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: While outcomes with cochlear implants (CIs) are generally good, performance can be fragile. The authors examined two factors that are crucial for good CI performance. First, while there is a clear benefit for adding residual acoustic hearing to CI stimulation (typically in low frequencies), it is unclear whether this contributes directly to phonetic categorization. Thus, the authors examined perception of voicing (which uses low-frequency acoustic cues) and fricative place of articulation (s/∫, which does not) in CI users with and without residual acoustic hearing. Second, in speech categorization experiments, CI users typically show shallower identification functions. These are typically interpreted as deriving from noisy encoding of the signal. However, psycholinguistic work suggests shallow slopes may also be a useful way to adapt to uncertainty. The authors thus employed an eye-tracking paradigm to examine this in CI users. DESIGN: Participants were 30 CI users (with a variety of configurations) and 22 age-matched normal hearing (NH) controls. Participants heard tokens from six b/p and six s/∫ continua (eight steps) spanning real words (e.g., beach/peach, sip/ship). Participants selected the picture corresponding to the word they heard from a screen containing four items (a b-, p-, s- and ∫-initial item). Eye movements to each object were monitored as a measure of how strongly they were considering each interpretation in the moments leading up to their final percept. RESULTS: Mouse-click results (analogous to phoneme identification) for voicing showed a shallower slope for CI users than NH listeners, but no differences between CI users with and without residual acoustic hearing. For fricatives, CI users also showed a shallower slope, but unexpectedly, acoustic + electric listeners showed an even shallower slope. Eye movements showed a gradient response to fine-grained acoustic differences for all listeners. Even considering only trials in which a participant clicked "b" (for example), and accounting for variation in the category boundary, participants made more looks to the competitor ("p") as the voice onset time neared the boundary. CI users showed a similar pattern, but looked to the competitor more than NH listeners, and this was not different at different continuum steps. CONCLUSION: Residual acoustic hearing did not improve voicing categorization suggesting it may not help identify these phonetic cues. The fact that acoustic + electric users showed poorer performance on fricatives was unexpected as they usually show a benefit in standardized perception measures, and as sibilants contain little energy in the low-frequency (acoustic) range. The authors hypothesize that these listeners may overweight acoustic input, and have problems when this is not available (in fricatives). Thus, the benefit (or cost) of acoustic hearing for phonetic categorization may be complex. Eye movements suggest that in both CI and NH listeners, phoneme categorization is not a process of mapping continuous cues to discrete categories. Rather listeners preserve gradiency as a way to deal with uncertainty. CI listeners appear to adapt to their implant (in part) by amplifying competitor activation to preserve their flexibility in the face of potential misperceptions.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica , Implantes Cocleares , Sordera/fisiopatología , Percepción del Habla , Incertidumbre , Adulto , Anciano , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Implantación Coclear , Sordera/rehabilitación , Medidas del Movimiento Ocular , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Fonética , Adulto Joven
2.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 54(7): 745-53, 2013 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23574387

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Many children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) struggle to understand familiar words and learn unfamiliar words. We explored the extent to which these problems reflect deficient use of probabilistic gaze in the extra-linguistic context. METHOD: Thirty children with ASD and 43 with typical development (TD) participated in a spoken word recognition and mapping task. They viewed photographs of a woman behind three objects and simultaneously heard a word. For word recognition, the objects and words were familiar and the woman gazed ahead (neutral), toward the named object (facilitative), or toward an un-named object (contradictory). For word mapping, the objects and words were unfamiliar and only the neutral and facilitative conditions were employed. The children clicked on the named object, registering accuracy and reaction time. RESULTS: Speed of word recognition did not differ between groups but varied with gaze such that responses were fastest in the facilitative condition and slowest in the contradictory condition. Only the ASD group responded slower to low frequency than high-frequency words. Accuracy of word mapping did not differ between groups, but accuracy varied with gaze with higher performance in the facilitative than neutral condition. Both groups scored above single-trial chance levels in the neutral condition by tracking cross-situational information. Only in the ASD group did mapping vary with receptive vocabulary. CONCLUSIONS: Under laboratory conditions, children with ASD can monitor gaze and judge its reliability as a cue to word meaning as well as typical peers. The use of cross-situational statistics to support word learning may be problematic for those who have weak language abilities.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/psicología , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/terapia , Señales (Psicología) , Fijación Ocular , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Percepción del Habla , Aprendizaje Verbal , Adolescente , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Masculino , Modelos Estadísticos , Tiempo de Reacción , Vocabulario
3.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 49(7): 1145-1160, 2023 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35617220

RESUMEN

Lexical access is highly contextual. For example, vowel (rime) information is prioritized over tone in the lexical access of isolated words in Mandarin Chinese, but these roles are flipped in constraining contexts. The time course of these contextual effects remains unclear, and so here we tracked the real-time eye gaze of native Mandarin speakers in a visual-world paradigm. While listening to a noun classifier, before the target noun was even uttered, gaze to the target noun was already greater than looking to phonologically unrelated distractors. Critically, there was also more distraction from a cohort competitor (tone information) than a segmental competitor (vowel information) in more semantically constraining contexts. Results confirm that phonological activation in Mandarin lexical access is highly sensitive to context, with tone taking priority over vowel information even before a target word is heard. Results suggest that phonological activation in real-time lexical access may be highly context-specific across languages. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Percepción del Habla , Humanos , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Tecnología de Seguimiento Ocular , Procesamiento de Texto , Lenguaje , Percepción Auditiva , Fonética
4.
J Child Lang ; 38(2): 380-403, 2011 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20513256

RESUMEN

Error patterns in children's phonological development are often described as simplifying processes that can interact with one another with different consequences. Some interactions limit the applicability of an error pattern, and others extend it to more words. Theories predict that error patterns interact to their full potential. While specific interactions have been documented for certain pairs of processes, no developmental study has shown that the range of typologically predicted interactions occurs for those processes. To determine whether this anomaly is an accidental gap or a systematic peculiarity of particular error patterns, two commonly occurring processes were considered, namely Deaffrication and Consonant Harmony. Results are reported from a cross-sectional and longitudinal study of twelve children (age 3 ; 0-5 ; 0) with functional phonological delays. Three interaction types were attested to varying degrees. The longitudinal results further instantiated the typology and revealed a characteristic trajectory of change. Implications of these findings are explored.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje Infantil , Fonética , Niño , Preescolar , Estudios Transversales , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales
5.
Cognition ; 169: 147-164, 2017 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28917133

RESUMEN

Spoken language unfolds over time. Consequently, there are brief periods of ambiguity, when incomplete input can match many possible words. Typical listeners solve this problem by immediately activating multiple candidates which compete for recognition. In two experiments using the visual world paradigm, we examined real-time lexical competition in prelingually deaf cochlear implant (CI) users, and normal hearing (NH) adults listening to severely degraded speech. In Experiment 1, adolescent CI users and NH controls matched spoken words to arrays of pictures including pictures of the target word and phonological competitors. Eye-movements to each referent were monitored asa measure of how strongly that candidate was considered over time. Relative to NH controls, CI users showed a large delay in fixating any object, less competition from onset competitors (e.g., sandwich after hearing sandal), and increased competition from rhyme competitors (e.g., candle after hearing sandal). Experiment 2 observed the same pattern with NH listeners hearing highly degraded speech. These studies suggests that in contrast to all prior studies of word recognition in typical listeners, listeners recognizing words in severely degraded conditions can exhibit a substantively different pattern of dynamics, waiting to begin lexical access until substantial information has accumulated.


Asunto(s)
Implantes Cocleares , Sordera/fisiopatología , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Habla/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Personas con Deficiencia Auditiva , Adulto Joven
6.
Dev Psychol ; 51(12): 1690-703, 2015 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26479544

RESUMEN

This study investigated the developmental time course of spoken word recognition in older children using eye tracking to assess how the real-time processing dynamics of word recognition change over development. We found that 9-year-olds were slower to activate the target words and showed more early competition from competitor words than 16-year-olds; however, both age groups ultimately fixated targets to the same degree. This contrasts with a prior study of adolescents with language impairment (McMurray, Samelson, Lee, & Tomblin, 2010) that showed a different pattern of real-time processes. These findings suggest that the dynamics of word recognition are still developing even at these late ages, and developmental changes may derive from different sources than individual differences in relative language ability.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo del Adolescente , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Adolescente , Niño , Medidas del Movimiento Ocular/instrumentación , Femenino , Humanos , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Masculino , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología
7.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 40(1): 308-27, 2014 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24041330

RESUMEN

Though much is known about how words are recognized, little research has focused on how a degraded signal affects the fine-grained temporal aspects of real-time word recognition. The perception of degraded speech was examined in two populations with the goal of describing the time course of word recognition and lexical competition. Thirty-three postlingually deafened cochlear implant (CI) users and 57 normal hearing (NH) adults (16 in a CI-simulation condition) participated in a visual world paradigm eye-tracking task in which their fixations to a set of phonologically related items were monitored as they heard one item being named. Each degraded-speech group was compared with a set of age-matched NH participants listening to unfiltered speech. CI users and the simulation group showed a delay in activation relative to the NH listeners, and there is weak evidence that the CI users showed differences in the degree of peak and late competitor activation. In general, though, the degraded-speech groups behaved statistically similarly with respect to activation levels.


Asunto(s)
Implantes Cocleares , Sordera/fisiopatología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Medidas del Movimiento Ocular/instrumentación , Humanos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Fonética , Inteligibilidad del Habla/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo
8.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 56(4): 1328-45, 2013 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23926331

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Researchers have begun to use eye tracking in the visual world paradigm (VWP) to study clinical differences in language processing, but the reliability of such laboratory tests has rarely been assessed. In this article, the authors assess test-retest reliability of the VWP for spoken word recognition. Methods Participants performed an auditory VWP task in repeated sessions and a visual-only VWP task in a third session. The authors performed correlation and regression analyses on several parameters to determine which reflect reliable behavior and which are predictive of behavior in later sessions. RESULTS: Results showed that the fixation parameters most closely related to timing and degree of fixations were moderately-to-strongly correlated across days, whereas the parameters related to rate of increase or decrease of fixations to particular items were less strongly correlated. Moreover, when including factors derived from the visual-only task, the performance of the regression model was at least moderately correlated with Day 2 performance on all parameters ( R > .30). CONCLUSION: The VWP is stable enough (with some caveats) to serve as an individual measure. These findings suggest guidelines for future use of the paradigm and for areas of improvement in both methodology and analysis.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Pruebas del Lenguaje/normas , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Percepción del Habla , Habla , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Adulto , Femenino , Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Masculino , Fonética , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción , Análisis de Regresión , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
9.
Lang Res ; 46(1): 1-38, 2010.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21666872

RESUMEN

Results are reported from a descriptive and experimental study that was intended to evaluate comparative markedness (McCarthy 2002, 2003) as an amendment to optimality theory. Two children (aged 4;3 and 4;11) with strikingly similar, delayed phonologies presented with two independent, interacting error patterns of special interest, i.e., Deaffrication ([tɪn] 'chin') and Consonant Harmony ([ɡɔɡ] 'dog') in a feeding interaction ([kik] 'cheek'). Both children were enrolled in a counterbalanced treatment study employing a multiple base-line single-subject experimental design, which was intended to induce a grandfather effect in one case ([dɔɡ] 'dog' and [kik] 'cheek') and a counterfeeding interaction in the other ([ɡɔɡ] 'dog' and [tik] 'cheek'). The results were largely supportive of comparative markedness, although some anomalies were observed. The clinical implications of these results are also explored.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA