Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 28
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Optom Vis Sci ; 99(1): 24-30, 2022 01 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34882612

RESUMEN

SIGNIFICANCE: The development of visual acuity has often been looked upon as a function of age. This study considers whether cognition might also be a predictor of acuity in children. The results indicate that cognition is a predictor of acuity and therefore should play a role in vision evaluations and developmental research. PURPOSE: Prior studies have shown that changes in visual acuity in typically developing children occur beyond primary school age. However, these studies almost exclusively use chronological age as the sole predictor for visual development. Because many of the tasks used to measure acuity have a cognitive demand, it is possible that age is not the best predictor for changes in this function. The aims of this study were to explore the effect of cognition on the development of visual acuity and to compare this predictor with age. METHODS: The predictive ability of chronological age and cognition on acuity was assessed in a group of 81 typical children between 5 and 11 years old. RESULTS: Analysis of resulting trajectories showed that, although age indeed was a good predictor, development of visual acuity was equally well predicted by cognition. Moreover, partial correlations showed a strong correlation between cognition and acuity when controlling for age but no significant correlation between age and acuity when controlling for cognition. CONCLUSIONS: These results suggest that age alone is not the optimal determinant for the development of visual acuity in typical school-aged children, as cognition was also found to be an important predictor.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Niño , Humanos , Agudeza Visual
2.
Eur J Nutr ; 58(7): 2911-2920, 2019 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30327868

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Previous evidence suggests consumption of flavonoids, a sub-class of polyphenols, is associated with improved cognitive function across the lifespan. In particular, acute intervention of a flavonoid-rich wild blueberry (WBB) drink has been shown to boost executive function (EF), short-term memory and mood 2-6 h post-consumption in 7-10-year-old children. However, confirmation of the aspects of EF and memory susceptible to WBB ingestion is required, particularly during childhood, a critical period of neurological development. In addition, the child literature on berry flavonoid supplementation and cognition highlights the potential for such interventions to elicit positive benefits to real-world educational scenarios, such as reading, a complex ability which relies upon aspects of cognition already known to improve following WBB. METHODS: Here we examined which aspects of EF and memory are susceptible to acute WBB, as well as investigating whether acute WBB could further benefit reading ability. Fifty-four healthy children, aged 7-10 years, consumed a 200 ml WBB drink (253 mg anthocyanins) or a matched placebo according to a randomised, single-blind, parallel-groups design. Verbal memory (Auditory Verbal Learning Task; AVLT), EF (Modified Attention Network Task; MANT), and reading efficiency (Test of Word Reading Efficiency-2; TOWRE-2) were assessed at baseline and 2 h post-consumption. RESULTS: For the MANT, significantly quicker RTs were observed for WBB participants when compared to placebo participants on 120 ms trials, without cost to accuracy. Furthermore, WBB participants showed enhanced verbal memory performance on the AVLT, recalling more words than placebo participants on short delay and memory acquisition measures post-consumption. Despite these significant improvements in cognitive performance, no significant effects were observed for reading measures. CONCLUSION: Consumption of WBB was found to significantly improve memory and attentional aspects of EF. This indicates that a flavonoid-rich blueberry product, equivalent to 240 g or 1½ cups of fresh blueberries can provide acute cognitive benefits in children. These findings support accumulating evidence that flavonoid-rich products are beneficial for healthy brain function, particularly during critical developmental periods. However, the lack of findings relating to reading ability suggested acute WBB may not be sufficient to elicit benefits to reading. Chronic supplementation and other more sensitive reading measures should be considered for examining the effects of WBB on such a complex skill in the future.


Asunto(s)
Arándanos Azules (Planta) , Cognición/efectos de los fármacos , Suplementos Dietéticos , Jugos de Frutas y Vegetales , Memoria a Corto Plazo/efectos de los fármacos , Lectura , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción , Método Simple Ciego
3.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 30(2): 152-9, 2010 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20444119

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Vergence and accommodation studies often use adult participants with experience of vision science. Reports of infant and clinical responses are generally more variable and of lower gain, with the implication that differences lie in immaturity or sub-optimal clinical characteristics but expert/naïve differences are rarely considered or quantified. METHODS: Sixteen undergraduates, naïve to vision science, were individually matched by age, visual acuity, refractive error, heterophoria, stereoacuity and near point of accommodation to second- and third-year orthoptics and optometry undergraduates ('experts'). Accommodation and vergence responses were assessed to targets moving between 33 cm, 50 cm, 1 m and 2 m using a haploscopic device incorporating a PlusoptiX SO4 autorefractor. Disparity, blur and looming cues were separately available or minimised in all combinations. Instruction set was minimal. RESULTS: In all cases, vergence and accommodation response slopes (gain) were steeper and closer to 1.0 in the expert group (p = 0.001), with the largest expert/naïve differences for both vergence and accommodation being for near targets (p = 0.012). For vergence, the differences between expert and naïve response slopes increased with increasingly open-loop targets (linear trend p = 0.025). Although we predicted that proximal cues would drive additional response in the experts, the proximity-only cue was the only condition that showed no statistical effect of experience. CONCLUSIONS: Expert observers provide more accurate responses to near target demand than closely matched naïve observers. We suggest that attention, practice, voluntary and proprioceptive effects may enhance responses in experienced participants when compared to a more typical general population. Differences between adult reports and the developmental and clinical literature may partially reflect expert/naïve effects, as well as developmental change. If developmental and clinical studies are to be compared to adult normative data, uninstructed naïve adult data should be used.


Asunto(s)
Acomodación Ocular/fisiología , Convergencia Ocular/fisiología , Variaciones Dependientes del Observador , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa/instrumentación , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Estudiantes de Medicina , Disparidad Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
4.
Behav Res Methods ; 42(2): 517-24, 2010 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20479183

RESUMEN

Laboratory animals should be provided with enrichment objects in their cages; however, it is first necessary to test whether the proposed enrichment objects provide benefits that increase the animals' welfare. The two main paradigms currently used to assess proposed enrichment objects are the choice test, which is limited to determining relative frequency of choice, and consumer demand studies, which can indicate the strength of a preference but are complex to design. Here, we propose a third methodology: a runway paradigm, which can be used to assess the strength of an animal's motivation for enrichment objects, is simpler to use than consumer demand studies, and is faster to complete than typical choice tests. Time spent with objects in a standard choice test was used to rank several enrichment objects in order to compare with the ranking found in our runway paradigm. The rats ran significantly more times, ran faster, and interacted longer with objects with which they had previously spent the most time. It was concluded that this simple methodology is suitable for measuring rats' motivation to reach enrichment objects. This can be used to assess the preference for different types of enrichment objects or to measure reward system processes.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Motivación , Psicología Experimental/métodos , Carrera , Animales , Masculino , Ratas , Ratas Endogámicas , Factores de Tiempo
5.
Optom Vis Sci ; 86(11): 1276-86, 2009 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19770814

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Accommodation can mask hyperopia and reduce the accuracy of non-cycloplegic refraction. It is, therefore, important to minimize accommodation to obtain a measure of hyperopia as accurate as possible. To characterize the parameters required to measure the maximally hyperopic error using photorefraction, we used different target types and distances to determine which target was most likely to maximally relax accommodation and thus more accurately detect hyperopia in an individual. METHODS: A PlusoptiX SO4 infra-red photorefractor was mounted in a remote haploscope which presented the targets. All participants were tested with targets at four fixation distances between 0.3 and 2 m containing all combinations of blur, disparity, and proximity/looming cues. Thirty-eight infants (6 to 44 weeks) were studied longitudinally, and 104 children [4 to 15 years (mean 6.4)] and 85 adults, with a range of refractive errors and binocular vision status, were tested once. Cycloplegic refraction data were available for a sub-set of 59 participants spread across the age range. RESULTS: The maximally hyperopic refraction (MHR) found at any time in the session was most frequently found when fixating the most distant targets and those containing disparity and dynamic proximity/looming cues. Presence or absence of blur was less significant, and targets in which only single cues to depth were present were also less likely to produce MHR. MHR correlated closely with cycloplegic refraction (r = 0.93, mean difference 0.07 D, p = n.s., 95% confidence interval +/-<0.25 D) after correction by a calibration factor. CONCLUSIONS: Maximum relaxation of accommodation occurred for binocular targets receding into the distance. Proximal and disparity cues aid relaxation of accommodation to a greater extent than blur, and thus non-cycloplegic refraction targets should incorporate these cues. This is especially important in screening contexts with a brief opportunity to test for significant hyperopia. MHR in our laboratory was found to be a reliable estimation of cycloplegic refraction.


Asunto(s)
Acomodación Ocular , Señales (Psicología) , Hiperopía/diagnóstico , Disparidad Visual , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Preescolar , Humanos , Hiperopía/fisiopatología , Lactante , Estudios Longitudinales , Midriáticos , Refracción Ocular , Adulto Joven
6.
Vision Res ; 48(15): 1613-24, 2008 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18538815

RESUMEN

A remote haploscopic video refractor was used to assess vergence and accommodation responses in a group of 32 emmetropic, orthophoric, symptom free, young adults naïve to vision experiments in a minimally instructed setting. Picture targets were presented at four positions between 2 m and 33 cm. Blur, disparity and looming cues were presented in combination or separately to asses their contributions to the total near response in a within-subjects design. Response gain for both vergence and accommodation reduced markedly whenever disparity was excluded, with much smaller effects when blur and proximity were excluded. Despite the clinical homogeneity of the participant group there were also some individual differences.


Asunto(s)
Acomodación Ocular/fisiología , Convergencia Ocular/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Adulto , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Disparidad Visual/fisiología
7.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 56(9): 5370-80, 2015 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26275135

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: This study investigated whether vergence and accommodation development in preterm infants is preprogrammed or is driven by experience. METHODS: Thirty-two healthy infants, born at mean 34 weeks gestation (range, 31.2-36 weeks), were compared with 45 healthy full-term infants (mean 40.0 weeks) over a 6-month period, starting at 4 to 6 weeks postnatally. Simultaneous accommodation and convergence to a detailed target were measured using a Plusoptix PowerRefII infrared photorefractor as a target moved between 0.33 and 2 m. Stimulus/response gains and responses at 0.33 and 2 m were compared by both corrected (gestational) age and chronological (postnatal) age. RESULTS: When compared by their corrected age, preterm and full-term infants showed few significant differences in vergence and accommodation responses after 6 to 7 weeks of age. However, when compared by chronological age, preterm infants' responses were more variable, with significantly reduced vergence gains, reduced vergence response at 0.33 m, reduced accommodation gain, and increased accommodation at 2 m compared to full-term infants between 8 and 13 weeks after birth. CONCLUSIONS: When matched by corrected age, vergence and accommodation in preterm infants show few differences from full-term infants' responses. Maturation appears preprogrammed and is not advanced by visual experience. Longer periods of immature visual responses might leave preterm infants more at risk of development of oculomotor deficits such as strabismus.


Asunto(s)
Acomodación Ocular/fisiología , Enfermedades del Prematuro/fisiopatología , Recien Nacido Prematuro/crecimiento & desarrollo , Estrabismo/fisiopatología , Disparidad Visual/fisiología , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Preescolar , Convergencia Ocular , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Edad Gestacional , Humanos , Lactante , Recién Nacido , Enfermedades del Prematuro/diagnóstico , Masculino , Estrabismo/congénito , Estrabismo/diagnóstico
8.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 45(2): 714-20, 2004 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14744919

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To investigate the nature of early ocular misalignments in human infants to determine whether they can provide insight into the etiology of esotropia and, in particular, to examine the correlates of misalignments. METHODS: A remote haploscopic photorefraction system was used to measure accommodation and vergence in 146 infants between 0 and 12 months of age. Infants underwent photorefraction immediately after watching a target moving between two of five viewing distances (25, 33, 50, 100, and 200 cm). In some instances, infants were tested in two conditions: both eyes open and one eye occluded. The resultant data were screened for instances of large misalignments. Data were assessed to determine whether accommodative, retinal disparity, or other cues were associated with the occurrence of misalignments. RESULTS: The results showed that there was no correlation between accommodative behavior and misalignments. Infants were more likely to show misalignments when retinal disparity cues were removed through occlusion. They were also more likely to show misalignments immediately after the target moved from a near to a far position in comparison to far-to-near target movement. DISCUSSION: The data suggest that the prevalence of misalignments in infants of 2 to 3 months of age is decreased by the addition of retinal disparity cues to the stimulus. In addition, target movement away from the infant increases the prevalence of misalignments. These data are compatible with the notion that misalignment are caused by poor sensitivity to targets moving away from the infant and support the theory that some forms of strabismus could be related to failure in a system that is sensitive to the direction of motion.


Asunto(s)
Acomodación Ocular , Convergencia Ocular , Esotropía/diagnóstico , Percepción de Movimiento , Trastornos de la Percepción/diagnóstico , Percepción de Profundidad , Humanos , Lactante , Recién Nacido , Modelos Biológicos , Disparidad Visual
9.
Hum Mov Sci ; 23(6): 771-84, 2004 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15664672

RESUMEN

The present study examined the effects of a pre-movement delay on the kinematics of prehension in middle childhood. Twenty-five children between the ages of 5 and 11 years made visually open-loop reaches to two different sized objects at two different distances along the midline. Reaches took place either (i) immediately, or (ii) 2 s after the occlusion of the stimulus. In all age groups, reaches following the pre-movement delay were characterised by longer movement durations, lower peak velocities, larger peak grip apertures and longer time spent in the final slow phase of the movement. This pattern of results suggests that the representations that control the transport and grasp component are affected similarly by delay, and is consistent with the results previously reported for adults. Such representations therefore appear to develop before the age of 5.


Asunto(s)
Fuerza de la Mano/fisiología , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Factores de Tiempo
10.
J AAPOS ; 18(6): 576-83, 2014 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25498466

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To propose an alternative and practical model to conceptualize clinical patterns of concomitant intermittent strabismus, heterophoria, and convergence and accommodation anomalies. METHODS: Despite identical ratios, there can be a disparity- or blur-biased "style" in three hypothetical scenarios: normal; high ratio of accommodative convergence to accommodation (AC/A) and low ratio of convergence accommodation to convergence (CA/C); low AC/A and high CA/C. We calculated disparity bias indices (DBI) to reflect these biases and provide early objective data from small illustrative clinical groups that fit these styles. RESULTS: Normal adults (n = 56) and children (n = 24) showed disparity bias (adult DBI 0.43 [95% CI, 0.50-0.36], child DBI 0.20 [95% CI, 0.31-0.07]; P = 0.001). Accommodative esotropia (n = 3) showed less disparity-bias (DBI 0.03). In the high AC/A-low CA/C scenario, early presbyopia (n = 22) showed mean DBI of 0.17 (95% CI, 0.28-0.06), compared to DBI of -0.31 in convergence excess esotropia (n=8). In the low AC/A-high CA/C scenario near exotropia (n = 17) showed mean DBI of 0.27. DBI ranged between 1.25 and -1.67. CONCLUSIONS: Establishing disparity or blur bias adds to AC/A and CA/C ratios to explain clinical patterns. Excessive bias or inflexibility in near-cue use increases risk of clinical problems.


Asunto(s)
Acomodación Ocular/fisiología , Convergencia Ocular/fisiología , Modelos Teóricos , Errores de Refracción/fisiopatología , Estrabismo/fisiopatología , Disparidad Visual/fisiología , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Preescolar , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
11.
PLoS One ; 9(3): e91988, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24642662

RESUMEN

ERPs were elicited to (1) words, (2) pseudowords derived from these words, and (3) nonwords with no lexical neighbors, in a task involving listening to immediately repeated auditory stimuli. There was a significant early (P200) effect of phonotactic probability in the first auditory presentation, which discriminated words and pseudowords from nonwords; and a significant somewhat later (N400) effect of lexicality, which discriminated words from pseudowords and nonwords. There was no reliable effect of lexicality in the ERPs to the second auditory presentation. We conclude that early sublexical phonological processing differed according to phonotactic probability of the stimuli, and that lexically-based redintegration occurred for words but did not occur for pseudowords or nonwords. Thus, in online word recognition and immediate retrieval, phonological and/or sublexical processing plays a more important role than lexical level redintegration.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Psicolingüística , Adolescente , Adulto , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Mapeo Encefálico , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Fonética , Tiempo de Reacción , Semántica
12.
Br J Ophthalmol ; 98(5): 679-83, 2014 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24532798

RESUMEN

AIM: This paper presents Convergence Insufficiency Symptom Survey (CISS) and orthoptic findings in a sample of typical young adults who considered themselves to have normal eyesight apart from weak spectacles. METHODS: The CISS questionnaire was administered, followed by a full orthoptic evaluation, to 167 university undergraduate and postgraduate students during the recruitment phase of another study. The primary criterion for recruitment to this study was that participants 'felt they had normal eyesight'. A CISS score of ≥21 was used to define 'significant' symptoms, and convergence insufficiency (CI) was defined as convergence ≥8 cm from the nose with a fusion range <15Δ base-out with small or no exophoria. RESULTS: The group mean CISS score was 15.4. In all, 17 (10%) of the participants were diagnosed with CI, but 11 (65%) of these did not have significant symptoms. 41 (25%) participants returned a 'high' CISS score of ≥21 but only 6 (15%) of these had genuine CI. Sensitivity of the CISS to detect CI in this asymptomatic sample was 38%; specificity 77%; positive predictive value 15%; and negative predictive value 92%. The area under a receiver operating characteristic curve was 0.596 (95% CI 0.46 to 0.73). CONCLUSIONS: 'Visual symptoms' are common in young adults, but often not related to any clinical defect, while true CI may be asymptomatic. This study suggests that screening for CI is not indicated.


Asunto(s)
Convergencia Ocular , Tamizaje Masivo/estadística & datos numéricos , Trastornos de la Motilidad Ocular/diagnóstico , Trastornos de la Motilidad Ocular/epidemiología , Acomodación Ocular , Adolescente , Adulto , Enfermedades Asintomáticas , Anteojos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tamizaje Masivo/normas , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Prevalencia , Curva ROC , Sensibilidad y Especificidad , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Agudeza Visual , Adulto Joven
13.
J AAPOS ; 18(2): 162-8, 2014 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24582466

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Although eye exercises appear to help heterophoria, convergence insufficiency, and intermittent strabismus, results can be confounded by placebo, practice, and encouragement effects. This study assessed objective changes in vergence and accommodation responses in naive young adults after a 2-week period of eye exercises under controlled conditions to determine the extent to which treatment effects occur over other factors. METHODS: Asymptomatic young adults were randomly assigned to one of two no-treatment (control) groups or to one of six eye exercise groups: accommodation, vergence, both, convergence in excess of accommodation, accommodation in excess of convergence, and placebo. Subjects were tested and retested under identical conditions, except for the second control group, who were additionally encouraged. Objective accommodation and vergence were assessed to a range of targets moving in depth containing combinations of blur, disparity, and proximity/looming cues. RESULTS: A total of 156 subjects were included. Response gain improved more for less naturalistic targets where more improvement was possible. Convergence exercises improved vergence for near across all targets (P = 0.035). Mean accommodation changed similarly but nonsignificantly. No other treatment group differed significantly from the nonencouraged control group, whereas encouraging effort produced significantly increased vergence (P = 0.004) and accommodation (P = 0.005) gains in the second control group. CONCLUSIONS: True treatment effects were small, significantly better only after vergence exercises to a nonaccommodative target, and rarely related to the response they were designed to improve. Exercising accommodation without convergence made no difference to accommodation to cues containing detail. Additional effort improved objective responses the most.


Asunto(s)
Acomodación Ocular/fisiología , Convergencia Ocular/fisiología , Ejercicio Físico/fisiología , Fenómenos Fisiológicos Oculares , Ortóptica/métodos , Estrabismo/terapia , Adolescente , Adulto , Técnicas de Diagnóstico Oftalmológico/instrumentación , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estrabismo/diagnóstico , Estrabismo/fisiopatología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
14.
Strabismus ; 21(3): 155-64, 2013 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23978142

RESUMEN

AIM: To describe preliminary findings of how the profile of the use of blur, disparity, and proximal cues varies between non-strabismic groups and those with different types of esotropia. DESIGN: This was a case control study. METHODOLOGY: A remote haploscopic photorefractor measured simultaneous convergence and accommodation to a range of targets containing all combinations of binocular disparity, blur, and proximal (looming) cues. Thirteen constant esotropes, 16 fully accommodative esotropes, and 8 convergence excess esotropes were compared with age- and refractive error-matched controls and 27 young adult emmetropic controls. All wore full refractive correction if not emmetropic. Response AC/A and CA/C ratios were also assessed. RESULTS: Cue use differed between the groups. Even esotropes with constant suppression and no binocular vision (BV) responded to disparity in cues. The constant esotropes with weak BV showed trends for more stable responses and better vergence and accommodation than those without any BV. The accommodative esotropes made less use of disparity cues to drive accommodation (p = 0.04) and more use of blur to drive vergence (p = 0.008) than controls. All esotropic groups failed to show the strong bias for better responses to disparity cues found in the controls, with convergence excess esotropes favoring blur cues. AC/A and CA/C ratios existed in an inverse relationship in the different groups. Accommodative lag of > 1.0 D at 33 cm was common (46%) in the pooled esotropia groups compared with 11% in typical children (p = 0.05). CONCLUSION: Esotropic children use near cues differently from matched non-esotropic children in ways characteristic to their deviations. Relatively higher weighting for blur cues was found in accommodative esotropia compared to matched controls.


Asunto(s)
Acomodación Ocular/fisiología , Convergencia Ocular/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Esotropía/fisiopatología , Disparidad Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Adulto Joven
15.
Perception ; 42(7): 693-715, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24344547

RESUMEN

Accurate coordination of accommodation and convergence is necessary to view near objects and develop fine motor coordination. We used a remote haploscopic videorefraction paradigm to measure longitudinal changes in simultaneous ocular accommodation and vergence to targets at different depths, and to all combinations of blur, binocular disparity, and change-in-size ('proximity') cues. Infants were followed longitudinally and compared with older children and young adults, with the prediction that sensitivity to different cues would change during development. Mean infant responses to the most naturalistic condition were similar to those of adults from 6-7 weeks (accommodation) and 8-9 weeks (vergence). Proximity cues influenced responses most in infants of less than 14 weeks of age, but sensitivity declined thereafter. Between 12 and 28 weeks of age infants were equally responsive to all three cues, while in older children and adults manipulation of disparity resulted in the greatest changes in response. Despite rapid development of visual acuity (thus increasing availability of blur cues), responses to blur were stable throughout development. Our results suggest that, during much of infancy, vergence and accommodation responses are not dependent on the development of specific depth cues, but make use of any cues available to drive appropriate changes in response.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Convergencia Ocular/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Disparidad Visual/fisiología , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Adulto Joven
16.
Strabismus ; 21(2): 140-4, 2013 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23713939

RESUMEN

AIM: To provide evidence that a near clinical gradient AC/A ratio could instead reflect the CA/C relationship (the accommodation driven by response to disparity). DESIGN: Case control study. METHODOLOGY: 27 emmetropic participants with heterophoria <4 PD, 19 with intermittent distance exotropia, and 17 with near exophoria >6 PD were tested. A remote haploscopic photorefractor, which can measure simultaneous convergence and accommodation to a range of targets containing all combinations of presence or absence of binocular disparity, blur, and proximal (looming) cues, was used to assess response AC/A and CA/C relationships. These were compared with clinical gradient AC/A ratios at near and distance fixation using alternate prism cover test and plus or minus lenses. RESULTS: Although the near and distance clinical AC/A ratios correlated weakly with each other (p = 0.03), neither clinical method correlated with the more accurate response AC/A ratio from the laboratory method (p = 0.88 and p = 0.93, respectively). The laboratory CA/C ratio correlated strongly with the near clinical AC/A ratio (p = 0.004) but only very weakly with the distance ratio (p = 0.16). CONCLUSIONS: The "near gradient AC/A ratio" may actually reflect the CA/C linkage as the dissociation of the prism cover test disrupts vergence accommodation. If the near deviation diverges more with plus lenses, it may be because the lenses allow clear near vision without needing to recruit convergence accommodation to achieve it.


Asunto(s)
Acomodación Ocular/fisiología , Convergencia Ocular/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Exotropía/fisiopatología , Disparidad Visual/fisiología , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Lentes , Masculino
17.
Clin Exp Optom ; 95(2): 153-9, 2012 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22283788

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: In a previous study, we demonstrated that children with early onset myopia had greater instability of accommodation than a group of emmetropic children. Since that study was correlational, we were unable to determine the causal relationship between this and myopic progression. To address this, we examined the children two years later. We predicted that if accommodative instability was causing the myopic progression, instability at Visit 1 should predict the refractive error at Visit 2. Additionally, instability at Visit 1 should predict myopic progression. METHODS: Thirteen myopic and 16 emmetropic children were included in the analysis. Dynamic measures of accommodation were made using eccentric photorefraction (PowerRefractor) while children viewed targets set at three distances (accommodative demands), namely, 0.25 metres (4.00 D demand), 0.5 metres (2.00 D demand) and 4.00 metres (0.25 D demand). RESULTS: Both refractive error and accommodative instability at Visit 1 were highly correlated with the same measures at Visit 2. Children with myopia showed greater instability of accommodation (0.38 D) than children with emmetropia (0.26 D) at the 4.00 D target on Visit 1 and this instability of accommodation weakly predicted myopic progression. CONCLUSIONS: The results presented in the present study suggest that instability of accommodation accompanies myopic progression, although a casual relationship cannot be established.


Asunto(s)
Acomodación Ocular/fisiología , Miopía/etiología , Miopía/fisiopatología , Adolescente , Edad de Inicio , Niño , Progresión de la Enfermedad , Anteojos , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Miopía/terapia , Optometría/métodos , Optometría/normas , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
18.
Acta Ophthalmol ; 90(2): e109-17, 2012 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22280437

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: This study considered whether vergence drives accommodation or accommodation drives vergence during the control of distance exotropia for near fixation. High accommodative convergence to accommodation (AC/A) ratios are often used to explain this control, but the role of convergence to drive accommodation (the CA/C relationship) is rarely considered. Atypical CA/C characteristics could equally, or better, explain common clinical findings. METHODS: Nineteen distance exotropes, aged 4-11 years, were compared while controlling their deviation with 27 non-exotropic controls aged 5-9 years. Simultaneous vergence and accommodation responses were measured to a range of targets incorporating different combinations of blur, disparity and looming cues at four fixation distances between 2 m and 33 cm. Stimulus and response AC/A and CA/C ratios were calculated. RESULTS: Accommodation responses for near targets (p = 0.017) and response gains (p = 0.026) were greater in the exotropes than in the controls. Despite higher clinical stimulus AC/A ratios, the distance exotropes showed lower laboratory response AC/A ratios (p = 0.02), but significantly higher CA/C ratios (p = 0.02). All the exotropes, whether the angle changed most with lenses ('controlled by accommodation') or on occlusion ('controlled by fusion'), used binocular disparity not blur as their main cue to target distance. CONCLUSIONS: Increased vergence demand to control intermittent distance exotropia for near also drives significantly more accommodation. Minus lens therapy is more likely to act by correcting overaccommodation driven by controlling convergence, rather than by inducing blur-driven vergence. The use of convergence as a major drive to accommodation explains many clinical characteristics of distance exotropia, including apparently high near stimulus AC/A ratios.


Asunto(s)
Acomodación Ocular/fisiología , Convergencia Ocular/fisiología , Exotropía/fisiopatología , Niño , Preescolar , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Errores de Refracción/fisiopatología , Disparidad Visual/fisiología , Visión Binocular/fisiología
19.
Br J Ophthalmol ; 96(4): 508-13, 2012 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21873311

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Disparity cues can be a major drive to accommodation via the convergence accommodation to convergence (CA/C) linkage, but, on decompensation of exotropia, disparity cues are extinguished by suppression so this drive is lost. This study investigated accommodation and vergence responses to disparity, blur and proximal cues in a group of distance exotropes aged between 4 and 11 years both during decompensation and when exotropic. METHODS: 19 participants with distance exotropia were tested using a PlusoptiXSO4 photo refractor set in a remote haploscopic device that assessed simultaneous vergence and accommodation to a range of targets incorporating different combinations of blur, disparity and proximal cues at four fixation distances between 2 m and 33 cm. Responses on decompensation were compared with those from the same children when their deviation was controlled. RESULTS: Manifest exotropia was more common in the more impoverished cue conditions. When decompensated for near, mean accommodation gain for the all-cue (naturalistic) target was significantly reduced (p<0.0001), with resultant mean under-accommodation of 2.33 D at 33 cm. The profile of near cues usage changed after decompensation, with blur and proximity driving residual responses, but these remaining cues did not compensate for loss of accommodation caused by the removal of disparity. CONCLUSIONS: Accommodation often reduces on decompensation of distance exotropia as the drive from convergence is extinguished, providing a further reason to try to prevent decompensation for near.


Asunto(s)
Acomodación Ocular/fisiología , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Exotropía/fisiopatología , Disparidad Visual , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Niño , Preescolar , Convergencia Ocular , Señales (Psicología) , Exotropía/diagnóstico , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa
20.
PLoS One ; 7(10): e48357, 2012.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23118992

RESUMEN

The goal of this research was to investigate the changes in neural processing in mild cognitive impairment. We measured phase synchrony, amplitudes, and event-related potentials in veridical and false memory to determine whether these differed in participants with mild cognitive impairment compared with typical, age-matched controls. Empirical mode decomposition phase locking analysis was used to assess synchrony, which is the first time this analysis technique has been applied in a complex cognitive task such as memory processing. The technique allowed assessment of changes in frontal and parietal cortex connectivity over time during a memory task, without a priori selection of frequency ranges, which has been shown previously to influence synchrony detection. Phase synchrony differed significantly in its timing and degree between participant groups in the theta and alpha frequency ranges. Timing differences suggested greater dependence on gist memory in the presence of mild cognitive impairment. The group with mild cognitive impairment had significantly more frontal theta phase locking than the controls in the absence of a significant behavioural difference in the task, providing new evidence for compensatory processing in the former group. Both groups showed greater frontal phase locking during false than true memory, suggesting increased searching when no actual memory trace was found. Significant inter-group differences in frontal alpha phase locking provided support for a role for lower and upper alpha oscillations in memory processing. Finally, fronto-parietal interaction was significantly reduced in the group with mild cognitive impairment, supporting the notion that mild cognitive impairment could represent an early stage in Alzheimer's disease, which has been described as a 'disconnection syndrome'.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Disfunción Cognitiva/fisiopatología , Memoria/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Conducta/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados , Neuroimagen Funcional , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Análisis de Ondículas
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA