Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 43
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 58(4): 1268-1285, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36788456

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Despite accumulated evidence that language development depends on basic cognitive processes, the balance in contributions of verbal and non-verbal cognitive skills to language abilities is still underexplored. Little is known about which cognitive measures best predict the degree of severity in children with language disorder (LD). AIMS: To examine the association between verbal and non-verbal cognitive abilities with language abilities in typically developing and language impaired 8-year-old children, as well as which cognitive abilities are most effective in distinguishing LD severity levels. METHODS & PROCEDURES: Children (N = 509) from the Language-8 Study, which oversampled probable cases of children with LD from a population-based cohort in Norway, were assessed at 8 years. Language skills were assessed using the Norwegian Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-4 (CELF-4). Children's verbal and non-verbal cognitive abilities were assessed via standardized cognitive measures. An exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was first conducted to uncover the underlying factor structure of the cognitive variables. Using a hierarchical multiple regression analysis, we then examined to what extent the non-verbal cognition factor explained language abilities above and beyond verbal cognition factors. Lastly, multinomial logistic regression was used to examine which cognitive measures best predicted the degree of severity in the children with LD. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: The EFA resulted in three factors (Verbal Cognition, Processing Speed and Memory, and Non-Verbal Cognition). The hierarchical multiple regression analysis revealed that all three cognitive factors contributed significantly to individual variation in language abilities. Non-Verbal Cognition explained 5.4% variance in language abilities above and beyond that accounted for by Verbal Cognition and Processing Speed and Memory. Results from the multinomial logistic regression analysis indicated that cognitive subtests, including Familiar Sequences, WASI Vocabulary and WASI Similarities, not only distinguished LD from typically developing children, but were also efficient in distinguishing severity of LD symptoms. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: This study confirms concurrent links between language and non-verbal cognitive skills above and beyond the contribution of verbal cognitive skills. The results provide further evidence that children with LD experience both language and cognitive problems in mid-childhood. Our findings suggest implications for LD intervention and diagnosis. The findings support the importance of measuring both verbal and non-verbal cognitive skills when making an LD diagnosis, and point to the potential of targeting underlying cognitive skills as one strategy to support language abilities. WHAT THIS PAPER ADDS: What is already known on the subject Language development is dependent on basic cognitive processes. These include both verbal and non-verbal cognitive abilities. Children with LD often experience both language and cognitive problems. There is evidence that performance on cognitive tests may be associated with the degree of severity of LD. What this paper adds to existing knowledge The current results from a large population-based cohort establish that a number of verbal and non-verbal cognitive abilities are tightly linked to variation in language abilities and the degree of severity of LD. Our study confirms concurrent links between language and non-verbal cognitive abilities above and beyond the contribution of verbal cognitive abilities. We also identify specific verbal and non-verbal cognitive tests that distinguish between typical children and children with LD, as well as LD severity. What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work? Our findings support the importance of measuring both verbal and non-verbal cognitive skills when making an LD diagnosis. Our findings also point to the potential of targeting underlying cognitive skills as one strategy to support language abilities. We suggest that future intervention studies focus on the impact of non-verbal cognitive skills on language development in children with LD.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Humanos , Niño , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Lenguaje , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/diagnóstico , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/psicología
2.
Nutr J ; 21(1): 46, 2022 07 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35843945

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: The rapid neurodevelopment that occurs during the first years of life hinges on adequate nutrition throughout fetal life and early childhood. Therefore, adhering to a dietary pattern based on healthy foods during pregnancy and the first years of life may be beneficial for future development. The aim of this paper was to investigate the relationship between adherence to a healthy and potentially sustainable Nordic diet during pregnancy and in early childhood and child development. METHODS: This study is based on the Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study (MoBa) and uses data from the Medical Birth Registry of Norway (MBRN). In 83,800 mother-child pairs, maternal pregnancy diet and child diet at 6 months, 18 months and 3 years were scored according to adherence to the New Nordic Diet (NND). NND scores were calculated both as a total score and categorized into low, medium, or high adherence. Child communication and motor development skills were reported by parents at 6 months, 18 months, 3 and 5 years, using short forms of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire and the Child Development Inventory. Associations of NND adherence with child development were estimated with linear and logistic regression in crude and adjusted models. RESULTS: When examining the NND and child developmental scores as percentages of the total scores, we found positive associations between the NND scores (both maternal pregnancy diet and child diet) and higher scoring on child development (adjusted [Formula: see text] s [95% confidence intervals] ranging from 0.007 [0.004, 0.009] to 0.045 [0.040, 0.050]). We further found that low and medium adherence to NND were associated with higher odds of later emerging developmental skills compared to high NND adherence at nearly all measured timepoints (odds ratios [95% CI] ranging from significant values 1.15 [1.03-1.29] to 1.79 [1.55, 2.06] in adjusted analyses). CONCLUSIONS: Our findings support that adherence to a healthy and potentially sustainable diet early in life is important for child development every step of the way from pregnancy until age 5 years.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil , Madres , Preescolar , Estudios de Cohortes , Dieta , Padre , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Noruega , Embarazo
3.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 62(9): 1070-1078, 2021 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33369747

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Delayed walking is common in intellectual disability (ID) but may be less common when ID occurs with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Previous studies examining this were limited by reliance on clinical samples and exclusion of children with severe motor deficits. OBJECTIVE: To examine in a population-based sample if age of walking is differentially related to intellectual ability in children with ASD versus other neurodevelopmental disorders (NDD). METHODS: Participants were from the nested Autism Birth Cohort Study of the Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study (MoBa). Cox proportional hazards regression assessed if diagnosis (ASD n = 212 vs. NDD n = 354), continuous nonverbal IQ, and their interaction, were associated with continuous age of walking. RESULTS: The relationship between nonverbal IQ and age of walking was stronger for NDD than for ASD (Group × nonverbal IQ interaction, χ2  = 13.93, p = .0002). This interaction was characterized by a 21% decrease in the likelihood of walking onset at any given time during the observation period per 10-point decrease in nonverbal IQ (hazard ratio = 0.79, 95% CI: 0.78-0.85) in the NDD group compared to 8% (hazard ratio = 0.92, 95% CI: 0.86-0.98) in the ASD group. CONCLUSIONS: The finding that age of walking is less strongly related to low intellectual ability in children with ASD than in children without other NDDs supports the hypothesis that ID in ASD may result from heterogeneous developmental pathways. Late walking may be a useful stratification variable in etiological research focused on ASD and other NDDs.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Discapacidad Intelectual , Trastornos del Neurodesarrollo , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/epidemiología , Niño , Estudios de Cohortes , Humanos , Discapacidad Intelectual/epidemiología , Trastornos del Neurodesarrollo/epidemiología , Caminata
4.
Tidsskr Nor Laegeforen ; 139(14)2019 Oct 08.
Artículo en Noruego, Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31592612

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: The percentage share of children who are diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder has increased considerably since the 1990s in Norway as well as in other countries. It has previously been demonstrated that there is considerable variation between counties with respect to diagnostic practice. MATERIAL AND METHOD: We calculated the percentage of children with autism spectrum disorder by using patient data obtained from the Norwegian Patient Registry and population data obtained from the National Registry. The calculations were made for the country as a whole as well as by county. The diagnostic assessments and documentation were mapped by linking the Norwegian Patient Registry with the Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort study. We also reviewed patient records obtained from the specialist health service and considered whether diagnostic practice satisfied the research criteria for autism spectrum disorder. RESULTS: By the age of eight, 1.1 % of boys and 0.3 % of girls had been diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. The overall percentages varied from 0.3 to 1.0 between counties. From 2008 to 2016, these percentages increased in all age groups. Our review of patient records included 503 children. In 95 % of cases the patient records provided a high standard of documentation that the diagnostic research criteria had been satisfied. The assessments were largely conducted in accordance with the guidelines drawn up by the various health trusts. INTERPRETATION: Autism diagnoses are generally well documented within the Norwegian specialist health service and meet the diagnostic criteria. In the counties that demonstrate a low prevalence of autism, it appears the health service fails to recognise autism in many children, particularly girls, or the diagnosis is determined late.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Adolescente , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/diagnóstico , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/epidemiología , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Registros Médicos , Noruega/epidemiología , Derivación y Consulta , Sistema de Registros , Atención Secundaria de Salud , Distribución por Sexo
5.
BMC Public Health ; 17(1): 685, 2017 09 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28870201

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Cell phone use during pregnancy is a public health concern. We investigated the association between maternal cell phone use in pregnancy and child's language, communication and motor skills at 3 and 5 years. METHODS: This prospective study includes 45,389 mother-child pairs, participants of the MoBa, recruited at mid-pregnancy from 1999 to 2008. Maternal frequency of cell phone use in early pregnancy and child language, communication and motor skills at 3 and 5 years, were assessed by questionnaires. Logistic regression was used to estimate the associations. RESULTS: No cell phone use in early pregnancy was reported by 9.8% of women, while 39%, 46.9% and 4.3% of the women were categorized as low, medium and high cell phone users. Children of cell phone user mothers had 17% (OR = 0.83, 95% CI: 0.77, 0.89) lower adjusted risk of having low sentence complexity at 3 years, compared to children of non-users. The risk was 13%, 22% and 29% lower by low, medium and high maternal cell phone use. Additionally, children of cell phone users had lower risk of low motor skills score at 3 years, compared to children of non-users, but this association was not found at 5 years. We found no association between maternal cell phone use and low communication skills. CONCLUSIONS: We reported a decreased risk of low language and motor skills at three years in relation to prenatal cell phone use, which might be explained by enhanced maternal-child interaction among cell phone users. No evidence of adverse neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal cell phone use was reported.


Asunto(s)
Uso del Teléfono Celular/estadística & datos numéricos , Desarrollo Infantil , Comunicación , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Madres/psicología , Destreza Motora , Primer Trimestre del Embarazo , Adulto , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Madres/estadística & datos numéricos , Noruega , Embarazo , Estudios Prospectivos , Medición de Riesgo , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
6.
Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol ; 28(3): 255-62, 2014 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24547686

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Previous research on clinical and high-risk samples suggests that signs of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) can be detected between 1 and 2 years of age. We investigated signs of ASD at 18 months in a population-based sample and the association with later ASD diagnosis. METHODS: The study sample includes 52,026 children born 2003 through 2008 and is a subset of children that participated in the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort (MoBa), a population-based longitudinal study, and the Autism Birth Cohort (ABC), a sub-study on ASD. Parents completed all 23 items from the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT) at 18 months. RESULTS: The M-CHAT 6-critical-item criterion and the 23-item criterion had a specificity of 97.9% and 92.7% and a sensitivity of 20.8% and 34.1%, respectively. In the 173 children diagnosed with ASD to date, 60 children (34.7%) scored above the cut-off on either of the screening criteria. The items with the highest likelihood ratios were 'interest in other children', 'show objects to others' and 'response to name'. CONCLUSION: Even though one-third of the children who later received an ASD diagnosis were identified through M-CHAT items, the majority scored below cut-off on the screening criteria at 18 months. The results imply that it might not be possible to detect all children with ASD at this age.


Asunto(s)
Lista de Verificación , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/diagnóstico , Tamizaje Masivo/métodos , Padres , Adulto , Atención , Escolaridad , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Edad Materna , Noruega , Juego e Implementos de Juego , Sensibilidad y Especificidad , Conducta Social , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
7.
Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry ; 23(11): 1081-91, 2014 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24867341

RESUMEN

The Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT) and the Early Screening of Autistic Traits (ESAT) were designed to screen for autism spectrum disorders in very young children. The aim of this study was to explore proportions of children that screened positive on the ESAT or the M-CHAT and to investigate if screening positive on the ESAT and M-CHAT is associated with clinical referral by 18 months and other aspects of children's development, health, and behavior. In this study, the mothers of 12,948 18-month-old children returned a questionnaire consisting of items from the ESAT and M-CHAT, plus questions about clinical and developmental characteristics. The M-CHAT identified more screen-positive children than the ESAT, but the ESAT was associated with more clinical referrals and tended to identify more children with medical, language, and behavioral problems. A post hoc analysis of combining the two instruments found this to be more effective than the individual instruments alone in identifying children referred to clinical services at 18 months. Further analysis at the level of single items is warranted to improve these screening instruments.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/diagnóstico , Tamizaje Masivo/instrumentación , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Derivación y Consulta/estadística & datos numéricos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
8.
Epidemiology ; 24(6): 906-12, 2013 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24045716

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: A recent California study reported increased risk of autistic disorder in children conceived within a year after the birth of a sibling. METHODS: We assessed the association between interpregnancy interval and risk of autistic disorder using nationwide registry data on pairs of singleton full siblings born in Norway. We defined interpregnancy interval as the time from birth of the first-born child to conception of the second-born child in a sibship. The outcome of interest was autistic disorder in the second-born child. Analyses were restricted to sibships in which the second-born child was born in 1990-2004. Odds ratios (ORs) were estimated by fitting ordinary logistic models and logistic generalized additive models. RESULTS: The study sample included 223,476 singleton full-sibling pairs. In sibships with interpregnancy intervals <9 months, 0.25% of the second-born children had autistic disorder, compared with 0.13% in the reference category (≥ 36 months). For interpregnancy intervals shorter than 9 months, the adjusted OR of autistic disorder in the second-born child was 2.18 (95% confidence interval 1.42-3.26). The risk of autistic disorder in the second-born child was also increased for interpregnancy intervals of 9-11 months in the adjusted analysis (OR = 1.71 [95% CI = 1.07-2.64]). CONCLUSIONS: Consistent with a previous report from California, interpregnancy intervals shorter than 1 year were associated with increased risk of autistic disorder in the second-born child. A possible explanation is depletion of micronutrients in mothers with closely spaced pregnancies.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno Autístico/epidemiología , Intervalo entre Nacimientos/estadística & datos numéricos , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Noruega/epidemiología , Sistema de Registros , Riesgo , Factores de Tiempo
9.
Epidemiology ; 24(5): 660-70, 2013 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23867813

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Case-control studies have found increased head growth during the first year of life in children with autism spectrum disorder. Length and weight have not been as extensively studied, and there are few studies of population-based samples. METHODS: The study was conducted in a sample of 106,082 children from the population-based Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort. The children were born in 1999-2009; by the end of follow-up on 31 December 2012, the age range was 3.6 through 13.1 years (mean 7.4 years). Measures were obtained prospectively until age 12 months for head circumference and 36 months for length and weight. We compared growth trajectories in autism spectrum disorder cases and noncases using Reed first-order models. RESULTS: Subjects included 376 children (310 boys and 66 girls) with specialist-confirmed autism spectrum disorder. In boys with autism spectrum disorder, mean head growth was similar to that of other boys, but variability was greater, and 8.7% had macrocephaly (head circumference > 97th cohort percentile) by 12 months of age. Autism spectrum disorder boys also had slightly increased body growth, with mean length 1.1 cm above and mean weight 300 g above the cohort mean for boys at age 12 months. Throughout the first year, the head circumference of girls with autism spectrum disorder was reduced-by 0.3 cm at birth and 0.5 cm at 12 months. Their mean length was similar to that of other girls, but their mean weight was 150-350 g below at all ages from birth to 3 years. The reductions in mean head circumference and weight in girls with autism spectrum disorder appear to be driven by those with intellectual disability, genetic disorders, and epilepsy. DISCUSSION: Growth trajectories in children with autism spectrum disorder diverge from those of other children and the differences are sex specific. Previous findings of increased mean head growth were not replicated.


Asunto(s)
Estatura/fisiología , Peso Corporal/fisiología , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/fisiopatología , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Cabeza/crecimiento & desarrollo , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Noruega
10.
Epilepsia ; 54(8): 1462-72, 2013 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23865818

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Antiepileptic drugs may cause congenital malformations. Less is known about the effect on development in infancy and childhood. The aim of this study was to examine whether exposure to antiepileptic drugs during pregnancy has an effect on early child development. METHODS: From mid-1999 through December 2008, children of mothers recruited at 13-17 weeks of pregnancy were studied in the ongoing prospective Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study. Information on birth outcomes were obtained from the Medical Birth Registry (108,264 children), and mothers reported on their child's motor development, language, social skills, and autistic traits using items from standardized screening tools at 18 months (61,351 children) and 36 months (44,147 children) of age. The relative risk of adverse outcomes in children according to maternal or paternal epilepsy with and without prenatal exposure to antiepileptic drugs was estimated as odds ratios (ORs), using logistic regression with adjustment for maternal age, parity, education, smoking, depression/anxiety, folate supplementation, and child congenital malformation or low birth weight. KEY FINDINGS: A total of 333 children were exposed to antiepileptic drugs in utero. At 18 months, the exposed children had increased risk of abnormal scores for gross motor skills (7.1% vs. 2.9%; OR 2.0, 95% confidence interval [CI] 1.1-3.7) and autistic traits (3.5% vs. 0.9%; OR 2.7, CI 1.1-6.7) compared to children of parents without epilepsy. At 36 months, the exposed children had increased risk of abnormal score for gross motor skills (7.5% vs. 3.3%; OR 2.2, CI 1.1-4.2), sentence skills (11.2% vs. 4.8%; OR 2.1, CI 1.2-3.6), and autistic traits (6.0% vs. 1.5%; OR 3.4, CI 1.6-7.0). The drug-exposed children also had increased risk of congenital malformations (6.1% vs. 2.9%; OR 2.1, CI 1.4-3.4), but exclusion of congenital malformations did not affect the risk of adverse development. Children born to women with epilepsy who did not use antiepileptic drugs had no increased risks. Children of fathers with epilepsy generally scored within the normal range. SIGNIFICANCE: Exposure to antiepileptic drugs during pregnancy is associated with adverse development at 18 and 36 months of age, measured as low scores within key developmental domains rated by mothers. Exposures to valproate, lamotrigine, carbamazepine, or multiple antiepileptic drugs were associated with adverse outcome within different developmental domains.


Asunto(s)
Anticonvulsivantes/efectos adversos , Discapacidades del Desarrollo/inducido químicamente , Discapacidades del Desarrollo/epidemiología , Efectos Tardíos de la Exposición Prenatal/inducido químicamente , Efectos Tardíos de la Exposición Prenatal/epidemiología , Preescolar , Estudios de Cohortes , Planificación en Salud Comunitaria , Epilepsia/tratamiento farmacológico , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Oportunidad Relativa , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Embarazo , Resultado del Embarazo/epidemiología , Sistema de Registros/estadística & datos numéricos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Estudios Retrospectivos , Autoinforme , Sensibilidad y Especificidad , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
11.
Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol ; 27(6): 553-63, 2013 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23919580

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: This study examined potential self-selection bias in a large pregnancy cohort by comparing exposure-outcome associations from the cohort to similar associations obtained from nationwide registry data. The outcome under study was specialist-confirmed diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs). METHODS: The cohort sample (n = 89 836) was derived from the population-based prospective Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study and its substudy of ASDs, the Autism Birth Cohort (ABC) study. The nationwide registry data were derived from the Medical Birth Registry of Norway (n = 507 856). The children were born in 1999­2007, and seven prenatal and perinatal exposures were selected for analyses. RESULTS: ASDs were reported for 234 (0.26%) children in the cohort and 2072 (0.41%) in the nationwide population. Compared with the nationwide population, the cohort had an under-representation of the youngest women (<25 years), those who had single status, mothers who smoked during pregnancy, and non-users of prenatal folic acid supplements. The ratios of the adjusted odds ratios (ORs) in the cohort over the adjusted ORs in the nationwide population were as follows; primipara pregnancy: 1.39/1.22, prenatal folic acid use: 0.85/0.86, prenatal smoking: 1.20/1.17, preterm birth (<37 weeks): 1.48/1.42, low birthweight (<2500 g): 1.60/1.58, male sex: 4.39/4.59 (unadjusted only); and caesarean section history: 1.03/1.04. CONCLUSIONS: Associations estimated between ASDs and perinatal and prenatal exposures in the cohort are close to those estimated in the nationwide population. Self-selection does not appear to compromise validity of exposure-outcome associations in the ABC study.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/epidemiología , Efectos Tardíos de la Exposición Prenatal/epidemiología , Adulto , Niño , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/etiología , Estudios de Cohortes , Femenino , Humanos , Incidencia , Recién Nacido , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Noruega/epidemiología , Oportunidad Relativa , Embarazo , Estudios Prospectivos , Sistema de Registros , Factores de Riesgo , Sesgo de Selección , Adulto Joven
12.
Child Dev ; 84(2): 560-73, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23033814

RESUMEN

This study examined whether poor pointing gestures and imitative actions at 18 months of age uniquely predicted late language production at 36 months, beyond the role of poor language at 18 months of age. Data from the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study were utilized. Maternal reports of the children's nonverbal skills and language were gathered for 42,517 children aged 18 months and for 28,107 of the same children at 36 months. Panel analysis of latent variables revealed that imitative actions, language comprehension, and language production uniquely contributed to predicting late development of language production, while pointing gestures did not. It is suggested that the results can be explained by underlying symbolic representational skills at 18 months.


Asunto(s)
Gestos , Conducta Imitativa , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/diagnóstico , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Comunicación no Verbal , Preescolar , Femenino , Mano , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Estudios Prospectivos
13.
JAMA ; 309(6): 570-7, 2013 Feb 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23403681

RESUMEN

IMPORTANCE: Prenatal folic acid supplements reduce the risk of neural tube defects in children, but it has not been determined whether they protect against other neurodevelopmental disorders. OBJECTIVE: To examine the association between maternal use of prenatal folic acid supplements and subsequent risk of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) (autistic disorder, Asperger syndrome, pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified [PDD-NOS]) in children. DESIGN, SETTING, AND PATIENTS: The study sample of 85,176 children was derived from the population-based, prospective Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa). The children were born in 2002-2008; by the end of follow-up on March 31, 2012, the age range was 3.3 through 10.2 years (mean, 6.4 years). The exposure of primary interest was use of folic acid from 4 weeks before to 8 weeks after the start of pregnancy, defined as the first day of the last menstrual period before conception. Relative risks of ASDs were estimated by odds ratios (ORs) with 95% CIs in a logistic regression analysis. Analyses were adjusted for maternal education level, year of birth, and parity. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURE: Specialist-confirmed diagnosis of ASDs. RESULTS: At the end of follow-up, 270 children in the study sample had been diagnosed with ASDs: 114 with autistic disorder, 56 with Asperger syndrome, and 100 with PDD-NOS. In children whose mothers took folic acid, 0.10% (64/61,042) had autistic disorder, compared with 0.21% (50/24,134) in those unexposed to folic acid. The adjusted OR for autistic disorder in children of folic acid users was 0.61 (95% CI, 0.41-0.90). No association was found with Asperger syndrome or PDD-NOS, but power was limited. Similar analyses for prenatal fish oil supplements showed no such association with autistic disorder, even though fish oil use was associated with the same maternal characteristics as folic acid use. CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE: Use of prenatal folic acid supplements around the time of conception was associated with a lower risk of autistic disorder in the MoBa cohort. Although these findings cannot establish causality, they do support prenatal folic acid supplementation.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/epidemiología , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/prevención & control , Ácido Fólico/uso terapéutico , Efectos Tardíos de la Exposición Prenatal , Complejo Vitamínico B/uso terapéutico , Adolescente , Niño , Preescolar , Estudios de Cohortes , Suplementos Dietéticos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Noruega/epidemiología , Oportunidad Relativa , Embarazo , Primer Trimestre del Embarazo , Atención Prenatal , Análisis de Regresión , Riesgo , Adulto Joven
14.
Tidsskr Nor Laegeforen ; 133(18): 1929-34, 2013 Oct 01.
Artículo en Inglés, Noruego | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24084968

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: In 2012, we published an overview of the prevalence of developmental disorders and neurological diseases in children in Norway, which was unknown at the time. In this article we will compare diagnostics and treatment across counties and institutions. MATERIAL AND METHOD: The prevalence across counties of autism spectrum disorders, ADHD, epilepsy and cerebral palsy in children aged 0-12 was estimated with the aid of data from the Norwegian Patient Register for the years 2008-11. RESULTS: In the age group 6-12 years, nationwide prevalence amounted to 0.6% for autism spectrum disorders, 2.0% for ADHD, 0.9% for epilepsy and 0.3% for cerebral palsy. In total, 5.0% of all twelve-year-olds were registered with one or more of these diagnoses. The prevalence of autism spectrum disorders and ADHD varied between the counties, from 0.3% to 1.5% for autism spectrum disorders and from 1.1% to 3.5% for ADHD. For epilepsy and cerebral palsy there was little variation between the counties. Diagnostics and treatment of these four conditions are spread over 29 somatic hospitals and 102 units for child and youth psychiatry. INTERPRETATION: The variations across counties in the prevalence of autism spectrum disorders and ADHD are most likely due to variations in diagnostic practices. We ask whether it is appropriate to spread the provision of treatment across such a high number of institutions.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/epidemiología , Parálisis Cerebral/epidemiología , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/epidemiología , Epilepsia/epidemiología , Niño , Preescolar , Humanos , Lactante , Noruega/epidemiología , Prevalencia , Sistema de Registros
15.
Autism ; : 13623613231219306, 2023 Dec 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38159069

RESUMEN

LAY ABSTRACT: Using questionnaires in research relies on the expectation that they measure the same things across different groups of individuals. If this is not true, then interpretations of results can be misleading when researchers compare responses across different groups of individuals or use in it a group that differs from that in which the questionnaire was developed. For the questionnaire we investigated, the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ), we found that parents of boys and girls responded to questionnaire items in largely the same way but that the SCQ measured traits and behaviors slightly differently depending on whether the children had autism. Based on these results, we concluded that researchers using this questionnaire should carefully consider these differences when deciding how to interpret findings. SCQ scores as a reflection of "autism-associated traits" in samples that are mostly or entirely made up of individuals without an autism diagnosis may be misleading and we encourage a more precise interpretation of scores as a broader indication of social-communicative and behavioral traits.

16.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 65(4): 1561-1573, 2022 04 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35290086

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Early language difficulties are associated with later internalizing problems across different ages and for different aspects of language. The mechanisms behind this association are, however, less understood. In the current study, we investigated longitudinal associations between language difficulties at 5 years and internalizing problems at 6 years. We also examined emotion regulation, empathy, assertiveness, and social engagement at 6 years as possible pathways for this association. METHOD: A subsample from the Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study (MoBa) was used (N = 928). Structural equation models were developed to test the longitudinal associations and indirect pathways between language and internalizing problems. RESULTS: The results showed high stability for internalizing problems from 5 to 6 years (ß = .59, p < .001). Furthermore, semantic language difficulties predicted change in internalizing problems (ß = .12, p < .001). Finally, the path between semantic language and internalizing problems was partially mediated by social engagement and emotion regulation, with the indirect pathways accounting for 55% of the initial association. For girls, there was a significantly stronger correlation (p < .05) between semantic language difficulties and internalizing problems at baseline (r = .30, p < .001) than for boys (r = .16, p < .001). Otherwise, there were no sex differences. CONCLUSIONS: Indirect pathways from language difficulties to internalizing problems were identified through social engagement and emotion regulation. The results may guide targets for intervention in groups of children with language difficulties at risk for developing internalizing problems.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje , Instituciones Académicas , Niño , Preescolar , Estudios de Cohortes , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Madres
17.
Autism Res ; 15(2): 296-304, 2022 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34837355

RESUMEN

The present study objectives were to examine the performance of the new M-CHAT-R algorithm to the original M-CHAT algorithm. The main purpose was to examine if the algorithmic changes increase identification of children later diagnosed with ASD, and to examine if there is a trade-off when changing algorithms. We included 54,463 screened cases from the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study. Children were screened using the 23 items of the M-CHAT at 18 months. Further, the performance of the M-CHAT-R algorithm was compared to the M-CHAT algorithm on the 23-items. In total, 337 individuals were later diagnosed with ASD. Using M-CHAT-R algorithm decreased the number of correctly identified ASD children by 12 compared to M-CHAT, with no children with ASD screening negative on the M-CHAT criteria subsequently screening positive utilizing the M-CHAT-R algorithm. A nonparametric McNemar's test determined a statistically significant difference in identifying ASD utilizing the M-CHAT-R algorithm. The present study examined the application of 20-item MCHAT-R scoring criterion to the 23-item MCHAT. We found that this resulted in decreased sensitivity and increased specificity for identifying children with ASD, which is a trade-off that needs further investigation in terms of cost-effectiveness. However, further research is needed to optimize screening for ASD in the early developmental period to increase identification of false negatives.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Trastorno Autístico , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/diagnóstico , Trastorno Autístico/diagnóstico , Lista de Verificación , Niño , Estudios de Cohortes , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Tamizaje Masivo/métodos , Madres
18.
BMC Pediatr ; 11: 59, 2011 Jun 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21714885

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Research has highlighted a series of persistent deficits in cognitive ability in preterm low-birth-weight children. Language and attention problems are among these deficits, although the nature of the relation between attention and language in early development is not well known. This study represents a preliminary attempt to shed light on the relations between attention problems and language development in preterm low-birth-weight children. METHODS: The aim of this study was to analyse reciprocal influences between language and attention problems from 18 to 36 months. We used maternal reports on attention problems and language ability referring to a sample of 1288 premature low-birth-weight infants, collected as part of the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa). A sample of children born full-term was used as the control group (N = 37010). Cross-lagged panel analyses were carried out to study reciprocal influences between attention problems and language. RESULTS: Language ability at 18 months did not significantly predict attention problems at 36 months, adjusting for attention problems at 18 months. Attention problems at 18 months significantly predicted changes in language ability from 18 to 36 months, pointing to a precursor role of attention in relation to language in children born preterm. Gender, age corrected for prematurity, and mother's education emerged as important covariates. CONCLUSIONS: Preliminary evidence was found for a precursor role of early attention problems in relation to language in prematurity. This finding can contribute to a better understanding of the developmental pathways of attention and language and lead to better management of unfavourable outcomes associated with co-morbid attention and language difficulties.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Discapacidades del Desarrollo/fisiopatología , Recién Nacido de Bajo Peso/fisiología , Recien Nacido Prematuro/fisiología , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/fisiopatología , Factores de Edad , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Preescolar , Escolaridad , Análisis Factorial , Humanos , Lactante , Recién Nacido , Madres , Factores Sexuales
19.
JAMA ; 306(14): 1566-73, 2011 Oct 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21990300

RESUMEN

CONTEXT: Prenatal folic acid supplements reduce the risk of neural tube defects and may have beneficial effects on other aspects of neurodevelopment. OBJECTIVE: To examine associations between mothers' use of prenatal folic acid supplements and risk of severe language delay in their children at age 3 years. DESIGN, SETTING, AND PATIENTS: The prospective observational Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study recruited pregnant women between 1999 and December 2008. Data on children born before 2008 whose mothers returned the 3-year follow-up questionnaire by June 16, 2010, were used. Maternal use of folic acid supplements within the interval from 4 weeks before to 8 weeks after conception was the exposure. Relative risks were approximated by estimating odds ratios (ORs) with 95% CIs in a logistic regression analysis. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURE: Children's language competency at age 3 years measured by maternal report on a 6-point ordinal language grammar scale. Children with minimal expressive language (only 1-word or unintelligible utterances) were rated as having severe language delay. RESULTS: Among 38,954 children, 204 (0.5%) had severe language delay. Children whose mothers took no dietary supplements in the specified exposure interval were the reference group (n = 9052 [24.0%], with severe language delay in 81 children [0.9%]). Adjusted ORs for 3 patterns of exposure to maternal dietary supplements were (1) other supplements, but no folic acid (n = 2480 [6.6%], with severe language delay in 22 children [0.9%]; OR, 1.04; 95% CI, 0.62-1.74); (2) folic acid only (n = 7127 [18.9%], with severe language delay in 28 children [0.4%]; OR, 0.55; 95% CI, 0.35-0.86); and (3) folic acid in combination with other supplements (n = 19,005 [50.5%], with severe language delay in 73 children [0.4%]; OR, 0.55; 95% CI, 0.39-0.78). CONCLUSION: Among this Norwegian cohort of mothers and children, maternal use of folic acid supplements in early pregnancy was associated with a reduced risk of severe language delay in children at age 3 years.


Asunto(s)
Ácido Fólico/uso terapéutico , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/prevención & control , Efectos Tardíos de la Exposición Prenatal , Complejo Vitamínico B/uso terapéutico , Adulto , Preescolar , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/clasificación , Defectos del Tubo Neural/prevención & control , Noruega/epidemiología , Oportunidad Relativa , Embarazo , Atención Prenatal , Estudios Prospectivos , Análisis de Regresión , Riesgo , Adulto Joven
20.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 51(3): 922-932, 2021 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32424708

RESUMEN

Early identification of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is regarded as crucial for swift access to early intervention and, subsequently, better outcomes later in life. However, current instruments miss large proportions of children who later go on to be diagnosed with ASD, raising a question of what these instruments measure. The present study utilized data from the Norwegian Mother, Father, and Child Cohort Study and the Autism Birth Cohort study to explore the subsequent developmental and diagnostic characteristics of children raising developmental concern on the six-critical discriminative item criterion of the M-CHAT (DFA6) at 18 months of age (N = 834). The DFA6 identified 28.8% of children diagnosed with ASD (N = 163), but 4.4% with language disorder (N = 188) and 81.3% with intellectual disability (N = 32) without ASD. Scoring in the «at-risk¼ range was associated with lower IQ, impaired functional language, and greater severity of autism symptoms whether children had ASD or not.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/diagnóstico , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Desarrollo Infantil , Padre/psicología , Madres/psicología , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/epidemiología , Niño , Preescolar , Estudios de Cohortes , Diagnóstico Precoz , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Discapacidad Intelectual/diagnóstico , Discapacidad Intelectual/epidemiología , Discapacidad Intelectual/psicología , Trastornos del Lenguaje/diagnóstico , Trastornos del Lenguaje/epidemiología , Masculino , Noruega/epidemiología , Estudios Prospectivos , Factores de Riesgo , Resultado del Tratamiento
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA