Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 39
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Cell ; 137(3): 472-84, 2009 May 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19410544

RESUMEN

The centromere is responsible for accurate chromosome segregation. Mammalian centromeres are specified epigenetically, with all active centromeres containing centromere-specific chromatin in which CENP-A replaces histone H3 within the nucleosome. The proteins responsible for assembly of human CENP-A into centromeric nucleosomes during the G1 phase of the cell cycle are shown here to be distinct from the chromatin assembly factors previously shown to load other histone H3 variants. Here we demonstrate that prenucleosomal CENP-A is complexed with histone H4, nucleophosmin 1, and HJURP. Recruitment of new CENP-A into nucleosomes at replicated centromeres is dependent on HJURP. Recognition by HJURP is mediated through the centromere targeting domain (CATD) of CENP-A, a region that we demonstrated previously to induce a unique conformational rigidity to both the subnucleosomal CENP-A heterotetramer and the corresponding assembled nucleosome. We propose HJURP to be a cell-cycle-regulated CENP-A-specific histone chaperone required for centromeric chromatin assembly.


Asunto(s)
Autoantígenos/metabolismo , Centrómero/metabolismo , Ensamble y Desensamble de Cromatina , Proteínas Cromosómicas no Histona/metabolismo , Proteínas de Unión al ADN/metabolismo , Nucleosomas/metabolismo , Línea Celular , Centrómero/ultraestructura , Proteína A Centromérica , Fase G1 , Histonas/metabolismo , Humanos , Proteínas Nucleares/metabolismo , Nucleofosmina , Nucleosomas/ultraestructura
2.
J Elder Abuse Negl ; 35(1): 1-33, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37042040

RESUMEN

Using a mix-method design, we examined participants' willingness to respond to mass marketing scams (MMS). In Experiment 1, we examined the effect of age (young versus older) and letter style ("hot" versus "cold") on the intention to respond. The intention of responding was negatively associated with risk (p < .001) and having at least a high school education was positively associated with perception of benefits (b = .684, p < .001). In Experiment 2, we examined reward sensitivity on the intention to respond by manipulating reward amounts (low versus high) and the presence of an activation fee. The presence of an activation fee decreased intent to contact, but percentages remained high (25.75%). Analyses of qualitative data indicated that risk and benefit were both predicted by perceived self-efficacy. The results indicate that consumers' beliefs about their ability to control the outcomes of future interactions affected how they behaved when provided with MMS materials.


Asunto(s)
Abuso de Ancianos , Anciano , Humanos , Mercadotecnía , Intención
3.
J Elder Abuse Negl ; 32(2): 152-172, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32149596

RESUMEN

There have been inconsistent results regarding whether older adults are more vulnerable to fraud than younger adults. The two main goals of this study were to investigate the claim that there is an age-related vulnerability to fraud and to examine whether emotional intelligence (EI) may be associated with fraud susceptibility. Participants (N = 281; 18-82 years; M = 53.4) were recruited via Amazon's Mechanical Turk and completed measures of EI, decision-making, and scam susceptibility. Participants who scored higher on "ability" EI were less susceptible to scams. The "younger" group (M = 2.50, SD = 1.06) was more susceptible to scams than the "older" group, p <.001, d = 0.56, while the "older" group (M = 4.64, SD = 1.52) reported the scams as being more risky than the "younger" group, p =.002, d = 0.37. "Older" participants were more sensitive to risk, less susceptible to persuasion, and had higher than average emotional understanding. Emotional understanding was found to be a partial mediator for age-related differences in scam susceptibility and susceptibility to persuasion.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones , Inteligencia Emocional , Fraude/economía , Inversiones en Salud/economía , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Factores Protectores
4.
Risk Anal ; 37(9): 1632-1643, 2017 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28095602

RESUMEN

In later life, people are faced with a multitude of risky decisions that concern their health, finance, and personal security. Older adults often exercise caution in situations that involve risk. In this research, we asked whether older adults are also more responsive to warnings about potential risk. An answer to this question could reveal a factor underlying increased cautiousness in older age. In Study 1, participants decided whether they would engage in risky activities (e.g., using an ATM machine in the street) in four realistic scenarios about which participants could be expected to have relevant knowledge or experience. They then made posterior decisions after listening to audio extracts of real reports relevant to each activity. In Study 2, we explored the role that emotions play in decision updating. As in Study 1, participants made prior and posterior decisions, with the exception that for each scenario the reports were presented in their original audio format (high emotive) or in a written transcript format (low emotive). Following each posterior decision, participants indicated their emotional valence and arousal responses to the reports. In both studies, older adults engaged in fewer risky activities than younger adults, indicative of increased cautiousness in older age, and exhibited stronger decision updating in response to the reports. Older adults also showed stronger emotional responses to the reports, even though emotional responses did not differ for audio and written transcript formats. Finally, age differences in emotional responses to the reports accounted for age differences in decision updating.

5.
J Perianesth Nurs ; 32(6): 578-588, 2017 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29157764

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Identify the perceptions of perianesthesia nurses regarding behaviors that promote or detract from sustaining a safe, efficient, and satisfying work environment. DESIGN: Two focus groups and seven individual interviews (n=14) were conducted exploring the perceptions regarding team behavior of registered nurses in one pediatric perianesthesia unit. METHODS: Qualitative descriptive data collection, inductive content analysis. FINDINGS: Nurses described a responsive, engaged health care team whose leadership is available and directive when needed, as creating an effective, satisfying work environment. Primary themes that emerged were Leadership Sets the Tone, Playing Fair, No One Gets Hurt, and Why We Stay. This nursing team acknowledged that inattentive, distracted team members cause frustration, work inequities, and care delays, potentially undermining patient safety. CONCLUSIONS: Results demonstrate the need to create and sustain consistently respectful perianesthesia work cultures. Research focusing on unit specific approaches to work distribution, communication, leadership, and technology use is needed.


Asunto(s)
Personal de Enfermería en Hospital/psicología , Enfermería Perioperatoria , Humanos , Satisfacción en el Trabajo , Liderazgo , Grupo de Atención al Paciente , Seguridad del Paciente , Investigación Cualitativa
6.
Clin Gerontol ; 40(1): 3-13, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28452630

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Financial exploitation (FE) of older adults is a social issue that is beginning to receive the attention that it deserves in the mediathanks to some high profile cases, but empirical research and clinical guidelines on the topic are just emerging. OBJECTIVE: Our review seeks to synthesize the current research in the area and develop a concentpual model. METHOD: In this review, we describes the significance of the problem, proposes a theoretical model for conceptualizing FE, and summarizes related areas of research that may be useful to consider in the understanding of FE. RESULTS: We identify key structural issues that have limited interventions in the past and make specific public policy recommendations in lightof the largest intergenerational transfer of wealth in history. CONCLUSIONS: FE is a significant social problem, in this article we discuss implications for clinical practice.


Asunto(s)
Víctimas de Crimen/psicología , Abuso de Ancianos/prevención & control , Abuso de Ancianos/estadística & datos numéricos , Anciano , Disfunción Cognitiva/complicaciones , Víctimas de Crimen/estadística & datos numéricos , Decepción , Toma de Decisiones , Abuso de Ancianos/economía , Abuso de Ancianos/legislación & jurisprudencia , Función Ejecutiva , Fraude , Humanos , Modelos Teóricos , Factores de Riesgo
7.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 108(40): 16588-93, 2011 Oct 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21949362

RESUMEN

Centromere protein A (CENP-A) is a histone H3 variant that marks centromere location on the chromosome. To study the subunit structure and folding of human CENP-A-containing chromatin, we generated a set of nucleosomal arrays with canonical core histones and another set with CENP-A substituted for H3. At the level of quaternary structure and assembly, we find that CENP-A arrays are composed of octameric nucleosomes that assemble in a stepwise mechanism, recapitulating conventional array assembly with canonical histones. At intermediate structural resolution, we find that CENP-A-containing arrays are globally condensed relative to arrays with the canonical histones. At high structural resolution, using hydrogen-deuterium exchange coupled to mass spectrometry (H/DX-MS), we find that the DNA superhelical termini within each nucleosome are loosely connected to CENP-A, and we identify the key amino acid substitution that is largely responsible for this behavior. Also the C terminus of histone H2A undergoes rapid hydrogen exchange relative to canonical arrays and does so in a manner that is independent of nucleosomal array folding. These findings have implications for understanding CENP-A-containing nucleosome structure and higher-order chromatin folding at the centromere.


Asunto(s)
Autoantígenos/genética , Centrómero/genética , Proteínas Cromosómicas no Histona/genética , ADN Superhelicoidal/metabolismo , Conformación de Ácido Nucleico , Nucleosomas/genética , Conformación Proteica , Proteína A Centromérica , Electroforesis en Gel de Poliacrilamida , Epigenómica , Histonas/química , Histonas/genética , Humanos , Espectrometría de Masas , Microscopía Electrónica de Transmisión , Nucleosomas/ultraestructura , Ultracentrifugación
8.
J Elder Abuse Negl ; 26(4): 414-23, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24848863

RESUMEN

The current article examines neuropsychological correlates of financial elder exploitation in a sample of older adults who have been documented victims of financial elder exploitation. The purpose of this exploratory study was twofold. First, a subsample of the referrals at the Los Angeles County Elder Abuse Forensic Center (LACEAFC) was compared to community dwelling adults in terms of the specific cognitive domains linked to financial capacity including memory, calculation, and executive functioning. Next, the correlation between presence of neuropsychological data and the likelihood of filing a case with the LA County's District Attorney office was examined. Twenty-seven LACEAFC cases and 32 controls were assessed. Overall, the forensic center group performed worse than a community-based age-matched control group on the MMSE, calculation, and executive functioning (ps < .01). The presence of neuropsychological data was significantly correlated to an increased likelihood of a case being filed.


Asunto(s)
Víctimas de Crimen/legislación & jurisprudencia , Víctimas de Crimen/psicología , Abuso de Ancianos/legislación & jurisprudencia , Abuso de Ancianos/psicología , Evaluación Geriátrica/métodos , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Derecho Penal , Abuso de Ancianos/economía , Femenino , Medicina Legal/métodos , Fraude/legislación & jurisprudencia , Humanos , Los Angeles , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Medición de Riesgo , Responsabilidad Social
9.
Gerontology ; 59(3): 283-8, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23391745

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Statistical numeracy, necessary for making informed medical decisions, is reduced among older adults who make more decisions about their medical care and treatment than at any other stage of life. Objective numeracy scales are a source of anxiety among patients, heightened among older adults. OBJECTIVE: We investigate the subjective numeracy scale as an alternative tool for measuring statistical numeracy with older adult samples. METHODS: Numeracy was assessed using objective measures for 526 adults ranging in age from 18 to 93 years, and all participants provided subjective numeracy ratings. RESULTS: Subjective numeracy correlated highly with objective measurements among oldest adults (70+ years; r = 0.51, 95% CI 0.32, 0.66), and for younger age groups. Subjective numeracy explained 33.2% of age differences in objective numeracy. CONCLUSION: The subjective numeracy scale provides an effective tool for assessing statistical numeracy for broad age ranges and circumvents problems associated with objective numeracy measures.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Toma de Decisiones , Alfabetización en Salud , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Ansiedad , Bioestadística , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Conceptos Matemáticos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Riesgo , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
10.
Front Psychol ; 12: 789883, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34975685

RESUMEN

As the COVID-19 pandemic was unfolding, a surge in scams was registered across the globe. While COVID-19 poses higher health risks for older adults, it is unknown whether older adults are also facing higher financial risks as a result of COVID-19 scams. Here, we examined age differences in vulnerability to COVID-19 scams and individual difference measures (such as impulsivity, ad skepticism, and past experiences with fraud) that might help explain them. A lifespan sample (M = 48.03, SD = 18.56) of sixty-eight younger (18-40 years, M = 25.67, SD = 5.93), 79 middle-aged (41-64 years, M = 49.86, SD = 7.20), and 63 older adults (65-84 years, M = 69.87, SD = 4.50) recruited through Prolific completed questions and questionnaires online. In a within-subjects design, each participant responded to five COVID-19 solicitations, psychological measures, and demographic questions. Age group comparisons revealed that older adults were marginally less likely to perceive COVID-19 solicitations as genuine than middle-aged adults were. In addition, older adults perceived significantly fewer benefits than both younger and middle-aged adults did and perceived marginally higher risks than younger adults did. Hence, older adults did not exhibit greater vulnerability to COVID-19 scams. Regardless of age, intentions to respond to COVID-19 solicitations were positively predicted by higher levels of educational attainment, being married, past fraud victimization, and higher levels of positive urgency. As expected, stronger genuineness and benefit perceptions positively predicted action intentions, whereas stronger risk perceptions negatively predicted action intentions As such, COVID-19 scam susceptibility appears to be the result of a impulse control issue that is not easily inhibited, not even by past experiences of scam victimization.

11.
Brain Behav ; 11(11): e2391, 2021 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34662495

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Mass marketing scams threaten financial and personal well-being. Grounded in fuzzy-trace theory, we examined whether verbatim and gist-based risk processing predicts susceptibility to scams and whether such processing can be altered. METHODS: Seven hundred and one participants read a solicitation letter online and indicated willingness to call an "activation number" to claim an alleged $500,000 sweepstakes prize. Participants focused on the solicitation's verbatim details (hypothesized to increase risk-taking) or its broad gist (hypothesized to decrease risk-taking). RESULTS: As expected, measures of verbatim-based processing positively predicted contact intentions, whereas measures of gist-based processing negatively predicted contact intentions. Contrary to hypotheses, experimental conditions did not influence intentions (43% across conditions). Contact intentions were associated with perceptions of low risk, high benefit, and the offer's apparent genuineness, as well as self-reported decision regret, subjective vulnerability to scams, and prior experience falling for scams. CONCLUSIONS: Overall, message perceptions and prior susceptibility, rather than experimental manipulations, mattered in predicting scam susceptibility.


Asunto(s)
Intención , Mercadotecnía , Emociones , Humanos , Autoinforme
12.
J Prim Care Community Health ; 12: 21501327211027104, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34477004

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION/OBJECTIVES: The healthcare intake process plays a significant role in informing medical personnel about patients' demographic information, subjective health status, and health complaints. Intake forms can help providers personalize care to assist patients in getting proper referrals and treatment. Previous studies examined factors that could be included in intake forms independently, but this study analyzed loneliness, religiousness, household income, and social integration together to see how the combined effect influences mental and physical health status. This study aims to determine which of those 4 variables better inform patients' mental versus physical health status. METHODS: One hundred and seventy-nine participants completed surveys, including the SF-12® Health Survey, measuring perceived physical and mental health, UCLA 3-item Loneliness Scale, and a demographics questionnaire with questions about household income and time spent dedicated to religious practice, if applicable. Additionally, individuals answered social integration questions about how often they contact close family and friends or volunteer in the community. Using loneliness, household income, religiousness, social integration as independent variables, and controlling for demographic variables such as age, gender, and race, 2 regression models were built with Mental and Physical Health Composite Scores from the the SF-12® Health Survey as dependent variables. RESULTS: Loneliness was associated with mental health measures (b = -2.190, P < .001), while household income was associated with physical health measures (b = 0.604, P = .019) above and beyond other variables in the regression models. CONCLUSIONS: Integrating the 3 loneliness questions into intake forms can help approximate an individual's mental health status. This would allow the provider to be able to assess mental health problems more effectively and provide needed resources.


Asunto(s)
Soledad , Salud Mental , Estado de Salud , Humanos , Atención Primaria de Salud , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
13.
J Elder Abuse Negl ; 22(3-4): 281-90, 2010 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20711915

RESUMEN

This article describes the clinical work that three sets of geriatricians and psychologists provided in three elder abuse forensic centers in California. After a brief history of how the clinical services in each program developed, the contributions of geriatricians and psychologists within these elder abuse teams are detailed through the use of several case anecdotes. Beyond providing physical and psychological evaluations, geriatricians and psychologists provide consultations and education to other professionals and to elder abuse victims and their caregivers. These clinical teams emphasize the importance of conducting home visits and functional assessments, working with interdisciplinary team members, and providing expert testimony.


Asunto(s)
Abuso de Ancianos/diagnóstico , Evaluación Geriátrica , Geriatría , Psicología , Anciano , California , Cuidadores/psicología , Abuso de Ancianos/psicología , Ciencias Forenses , Humanos , Médicos
14.
Pediatr Qual Saf ; 4(3): e180, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31579878

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Current literature demonstrates that standardizing interunit patient handoff improves communication, information transfer, and patient safety. However, few studies have focused on increasing staff compliance with new handoff processes. The purpose of this quality improvement project was to incorporate both user input into process design and on-the-job coaching with a newly introduced nurse handoff process between the postanesthesia care unit and Medical/Surgical units. We hypothesized that staff compliance would be 100% within 90 days. METHODS: The team's intervention consisted of (1) involving representative frontline nursing staff in the standardization and modification of the handoff process and (2) providing on-the-job coaching as the new process was being trialed at the bedside. We designed the handoff process during a 2-day workshop and a 1.5-week pilot. Data included the number of observed noncompliant process elements and handoff duration. Three sequential 30-day plan-do-study-act cycles were followed, during which compliance observations and user feedback were used to refine the design and coaching iteratively. RESULTS: A total of 1,800 process elements were observed and coached throughout a 90-day trial period. The number of observed noncompliant elements decreased from 15% (92) to 4% (22) from the first 30-day interval to the final 30-day interval. There was no undesirable increase in handoff duration (mean, 8.05 ± 4.72 minutes), and several potential errors-related to orders, charting, and patient placement-were prevented by using the new handoff. CONCLUSIONS: User input and on-the-job coaching resulted in iteratively increasing frontline compliance with a new standardized handoff process.

15.
J Exp Psychol Appl ; 24(2): 196-206, 2018 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29683690

RESUMEN

Mass marketing scams extract an enormous toll, yet the literature on scams is just emerging. In Experiment 1, 211 adults reviewed a solicitation and rated their intention of contacting an "activation number" for a prize. Scarcity and authority were manipulated. Many (48.82%) indicated some willingness to contact to "activate" the winnings. Intention of responding was inversely related to the perception of risk (b = -.441, p < .001) and positively associated with perception of benefits (b = .554, p < .001), but not with the experimental condition. In Experiment 2, 291 adults were randomly assigned to one of the three conditions (low, medium, or high activation fee), and were asked to report willingness to contact. Activation fees decreased intent to contact, but percentages remained high (25.70%), with higher perception of risk reducing contact rates (b = -.581, p < .001), and benefit perception increasing intent to contact (b = .381, p < .001). Our studies indicate that consumers are responding to perceived risks and benefits in their decision-making, regardless of persuasion elements used by scammers. In summary, our studies find that consumers with lower levels of education and high perception of benefits are at increased risk for mass marketing scams. (PsycINFO Database Record


Asunto(s)
Distinciones y Premios , Toma de Decisiones , Intención , Mercadotecnía/métodos , Percepción , Medición de Riesgo , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
16.
Gerontologist ; 47(5): 604-12, 2007 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17989402

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: This preliminary study compared clinical evaluations for guardianship in three states with varying levels of statutory reform. DESIGN AND METHODS: Case files for 298 cases of adult guardianship were reviewed in Massachusetts, Pennsylvania, and Colorado, three states with varying degrees of statutory reform. The quality and content of the written clinical evidence for guardianship and the hearing outcome were recorded. RESULTS: The quality of the written clinical evidence for guardianship was best in Colorado, the state with the most progressive statutory reform, earning a grade of B in our ratings, and worst in Massachusetts, a state with minimal reform, earning a grade of D - with nearly two thirds of the written evidence illegible. Information on specific functional deficits was frequently missing and conclusory statements were common. Information about the individual's key values and preferences was almost never provided, and individuals were rarely present at the hearing. Limited orders were used for 34% of the cases in Colorado, associated with more complete clinical testimony, but such orders were used in only 1 case in either Massachusetts or Pennsylvania. IMPLICATIONS: In this study, states with progressive statutes that promote functional assessment are associated with increased quality of clinical testimony and use of limited orders. A continuing dialogue between clinical and legal professionals is needed to advance reform in guardianship, and thereby provide for the needs and protect the rights of adults who face guardianship proceedings.


Asunto(s)
Evaluación de la Discapacidad , Estudios de Evaluación como Asunto , Tutores Legales , Gobierno Estatal , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estados Unidos
17.
Innov Aging ; 1(1): igx016, 2017 Mar 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30480110

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVE: Social support is known to be an important protective factor against elder financial exploitation (FE), yet few empirical studies have examined the relationship between FE and distinct components of social support. Perceived social support, social network size, and interactions with close network members (positive and negative) were measured separately and tested as potential predictors of FE. RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS: Three hundred and ninety-five community-dwelling adults aged 60 and older were recruited to complete a 90-minute survey and interview. We used OLS regression to examine the role of social support in FE. Other risk factors associated with FE including dependency, poor physical health, depression, cognition, and demographic characteristics were included as potential predictors. RESULTS: Negative interactions with close network members predicted FE, and remained predictive when all other variables and social support factors were included in the model. Other social support factors were not unique predictors of FE. DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS: Negative social interactions with close network members are important to assess and consider in FE prevention and intervention programs; relationships between social interactions and other risk factors warrant further attention.

18.
Cortex ; 42(1): 38-47, 2006 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16509107

RESUMEN

In previous demonstrations of differences between left- and right-handers in dual-task performance, participants' hand preference has been confounded with asymmetry of manual skill. The present study was designed to disentangle those two factors as sources of lateralized interference in the concurrent-task paradigm. Forty-eight normal adults (24 females and 24 males) counted backward by ones or by twos while typing an easy or difficult sequence of letters with either hand. When participants were grouped according to self-reported hand preference, both groups showed bilaterally symmetric slowing, relative to single-task conditions. However, when the same participants were grouped according to manual asymmetry in the baseline condition, the cognitive task interfered significantly more with the faster hand than with the slower hand. Baseline typing rate, averaged across hands, did not influence dual-task interference. Both self-reported left-hand preference and left-hand superiority in baseline typing were associated with reduced interference on the cognitive task, and the reduced interference in those groups seemed to reflect relatively loose coupling between manual and cognitive tasks. The results support and extend Caroselli et al.'s (1997) findings regarding the effect of baseline manual asymmetry on the pattern of dual-task interference. Irrespective of the participant's hand preference, the presence or absence of baseline asymmetry may be sufficient to determine whether dual-task interference is lateralized.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Área de Dependencia-Independencia , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Solución de Problemas/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Discriminación en Psicología , Femenino , Dedos , Humanos , Masculino , Matemática , Valores de Referencia
19.
Psychol Aging ; 21(4): 815-20, 2006 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17201501

RESUMEN

Studies of younger adults have found that negative information has a stronger influence than positive information across a wide range of domains. T. A. Ito, J. T. Larsen, N. K. Smith, and J. T. Cacioppo (1998) reported that during evaluative categorization, extreme negative images produced greater brain activity than did equally extreme positive images in younger adults. Older adults have been reported to optimize affect and attend less to negative information. In this article, the negativity bias was examined in 20 older versus 20 younger adults during evaluative categorization, with a focus on brain activity occurring roughly 500 ms after presentation of visual stimuli. Results demonstrated a significant decrease in brain activity to both positive and negative stimuli (p < .05) and an elimination of the negativity bias in older adults.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Envejecimiento/psicología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad
20.
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA