Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 61
Filter
Add more filters

Country/Region as subject
Publication year range
1.
Neuroimage ; 281: 120365, 2023 11 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37683809

ABSTRACT

Cognitive Reserve (CR) refers to the preservation of cognitive function in the face of age- or disease-related neuroanatomical decline. While bilingualism has been shown to contribute to CR, the extent to which, and what particular aspect of, second language experience contributes to CR are debated, and the underlying neural mechanism(s) unknown. Intrinsic functional connectivity reflects experience-dependent neuroplasticity that occurs across timescales ranging from minutes to decades, and may be a neural mechanism underlying CR. To test this hypothesis, we used voxel-based morphometry and resting-state functional connectivity analyses of MRI data to compare structural and functional brain integrity between monolingual and bilingual older adults, matched on cognitive performance, and across levels of second language proficiency measured as a continuous variable. Bilingualism, and degree of second language proficiency specifically, were associated with lower gray matter integrity in a hub of the default mode network - a region that is particularly vulnerable to decline in aging and dementia - but preserved intrinsic functional network organization. Bilingualism moderated the association between neuroanatomical differences and cognitive decline, such that lower gray matter integrity was associated with lower executive function in monolinguals, but not bilinguals. Intrinsic functional network integrity predicted executive function when controlling for group differences in gray matter integrity and language status. Our findings confirm that lifelong bilingualism is a CR factor, as bilingual older adults performed just as well as their monolingual peers on tasks of executive function, despite showing signs of more advanced neuroanatomical aging, and that this is a consequence of preserved intrinsic functional network organization.


Subject(s)
Cognitive Reserve , Multilingualism , Humans , Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Language
2.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord ; 37(1): 7-12, 2023.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36821175

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Compared with monolinguals, bilinguals have a later onset of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer disease symptoms and greater neuropathology at similar cognitive and clinical levels. The present study follows a previous report showing the faster conversion from MCI to Alzheimer disease for bilingual patients than comparable monolinguals, as predicted by a cognitive reserve (CR). PURPOSE: Identify whether the increased CR found for bilinguals in the previous study was accompanied by greater gray matter (GM) atrophy than was present for the monolinguals. METHODS: A novel deep-learning technique based on convolutional neural networks was used to enhance clinical scans into 1 mm MPRAGEs and analyze the GM volume at the time of MCI diagnosis in the earlier study. PATIENTS: Twenty-four bilingual and 24 monolingual patients were diagnosed with MCI at a hospital memory clinic. RESULTS: Bilingual patients had more GM loss than monolingual patients in areas related to language processing, attention, decision-making, motor function, and episodic memory retrieval. Bilingualism and age were the strongest predictors of atrophy after other variables such as immigration and education were included in a multivariate model. DISCUSSION: CR from bilingualism is evident in the initial stages of neurodegeneration after MCI has been diagnosed.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease , Cognitive Dysfunction , Cognitive Reserve , Multilingualism , Humans , Gray Matter/pathology , Alzheimer Disease/psychology , Cognitive Dysfunction/psychology , Atrophy/pathology
3.
Neuroimage ; 198: 104-113, 2019 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31112787

ABSTRACT

On a daily basis, we constantly deal with changing environmental cues and perceptual conflicts and as such, our brains must flexibly adapt to current demands in order to act appropriately. Brains become more efficient and are able to switch states more readily by increasing the complexity of their neural networks. However, it is unclear how brain signal complexity relates to behavior in young adults performing cognitively demanding executive function tasks. Here we used multiscale entropy analysis and multivariate statistics on EEG data while participants performed a bivalency effect task-switching paradigm to show that brain signal complexity in young adults increases as task demands increase, that increases in brain signal complexity are associated with both speed gains and losses depending on scalp location, and that more difficult tasks are associated with more circumscribed complexity across the scalp. Overall, these findings highlight a critical role for brain signal complexity in predicting behavior on an executive function task among young adults.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Conflict, Psychological , Executive Function/physiology , Psychomotor Performance , Adult , Female , Humans , Information Theory , Male , Multivariate Analysis , Reaction Time , Young Adult
4.
Neuroimage ; 167: 143-150, 2018 02 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29175203

ABSTRACT

Bilingualism can delay the onset of dementia symptoms and has thus been characterized as a mechanism for cognitive or brain reserve, although the origin of this reserve is unknown. Studies with young adults generally show that bilingualism is associated with a strengthening of white matter, but there is conflicting evidence for how bilingualism affects white matter in older age. Given that bilingualism has been shown to help stave off the symptoms of dementia by up to four years, it is crucial that we clarify the mechanism underlying this reserve. The current study uses diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) to compare monolinguals and bilinguals while carefully controlling for potential confounds (e.g., I.Q., MMSE, and demographic variables). We show that group differences in Fractional Anisotropy (FA) and Radial Diffusivity (RD) arise from multivariable interactions not adequately controlled for by sequential bivariate testing. After matching and statistically controlling for confounds, bilinguals still had greater axial diffusivity (AD) in the left superior longitudinal fasciculus than monolingual peers, supporting a neural reserve account for healthy older bilinguals.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Cognitive Reserve/physiology , Diffusion Tensor Imaging/methods , Multilingualism , Neural Pathways/diagnostic imaging , White Matter/diagnostic imaging , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Male
5.
Hippocampus ; 28(10): 745-764, 2018 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29989271

ABSTRACT

The dynamic process of memory consolidation involves a reorganization of brain regions that support a memory trace over time, but exactly how the network reorganizes as the memory changes remains unclear. We present novel converging evidence from studies of animals (rats) and humans for the time-dependent reorganization and transformation of different types of memory as measured both by behavior and brain activation. We find that context-specific memories in rats, and naturalistic episodic memories in humans, lose precision over time and activity in the hippocampus decreases. If, however, the retrieved memories retain contextual or perceptual detail, the hippocampus is engaged similarly at recent and remote timepoints. As the interval between the timepoint increases, the medial prefrontal cortex is engaged increasingly during memory retrieval, regardless of the context or the amount of retrieved detail. Moreover, these hippocampal-frontal shifts are accompanied by corresponding changes in a network of cortical structures mediating perceptually-detailed as well as less precise, schematic memories. These findings provide cross-species evidence for the crucial interplay between hippocampus and neocortex that reflects changes in memory representation over time and underlies systems consolidation.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Hippocampus/cytology , Memory Consolidation/physiology , Memory, Episodic , Neurons/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Animals , Avoidance Learning/physiology , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Fear/psychology , Female , Functional Laterality , Hippocampus/diagnostic imaging , Hippocampus/physiology , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Oxygen/blood , Proto-Oncogene Proteins c-fos/metabolism , Random Allocation , Rats , Rats, Long-Evans , Time Factors , Young Adult
6.
Memory ; 26(2): 251-259, 2018 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28670964

ABSTRACT

Using implicit tests, older adults have been found to retain conceptual knowledge of previously seen task-irrelevant information. While younger adults typically do not show the same effect, evidence from one study [Gopie, N., Craik, F. I. M., & Hasher, L. (2011). A double dissociation of implicit and explicit memory in younger and older adults. Psychological Science, 22, 634-640. doi: 10.1177/0956797611403321 ] suggests otherwise. In that study, young adults showed greater explicit than implicit memory for previous distractors on a word fragment completion task. This was interpreted as evidence for maintaining access to previous conceptual knowledge of the distractors. Here, we report two failures to replicate that original finding, followed by a third study designed to test directly whether young adults use conceptual-level information that was previously irrelevant. Our findings agree with others that young adults show weak to no evidence of conceptual knowledge of previously irrelevant information.


Subject(s)
Attention , Concept Formation/physiology , Memory , Adolescent , Aging/psychology , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Processes , Stroop Test/statistics & numerical data , Young Adult
7.
Behav Res Methods ; 50(1): 250-263, 2018 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28281208

ABSTRACT

Research examining the cognitive consequences of bilingualism has expanded rapidly in recent years and has revealed effects on aspects of cognition across the lifespan. However, these effects are difficult to find in studies investigating young adults. One problem is that there is no standard definition of bilingualism or means of evaluating degree of bilingualism in individual participants, making it difficult to directly compare the results of different studies. Here, we describe an instrument developed to assess degree of bilingualism for young adults who live in diverse communities in which English is the official language. We demonstrate the reliability and validity of the instrument in analyses based on 408 participants. The relevant factors for describing degree of bilingualism are: (1) the extent of non-English language proficiency and use at home, and (2) non-English language use socially. We then use the bilingualism scores obtained from the instrument to demonstrate their association with: (1) performance on executive function tasks, and (2) previous classifications of participants into categories of monolinguals and bilinguals.


Subject(s)
Cultural Diversity , Executive Function , Interpersonal Relations , Language , Multilingualism , Cognition , Female , Humans , Male , Reproducibility of Results , Surveys and Questionnaires , Task Performance and Analysis , Young Adult
8.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 29(3): 560-572, 2017 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28129055

ABSTRACT

Testing older adults in the morning generally improves behavioral performance relative to afternoon testing. Morning testing is also associated with brain activity similar to that of young adults. Here, we used graph theory to explore how time of day (TOD) affects the organization of brain networks in older adults across rest and task states. We used nodes from the automated anatomical labeling atlas to construct participant-specific correlation matrices of fMRI data obtained during 1-back tasks with interference and rest. We computed pairwise group differences for key graph metrics, including small-worldness and modularity. We found that older adults tested in the morning and young adults did not differ on any graph metric. Both of these groups differed from older adults tested in the afternoon during the tasks-but not rest. Specifically, the latter group had lower modularity and small-worldness (indices of more efficient network organization). Across all groups, higher modularity and small-worldness strongly correlated with reduced distractibility on an implicit priming task. Increasingly, TOD is seen as important for interpreting and reproducing neuroimaging results. Our study emphasizes how TOD affects brain network organization and executive control in older adults.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Aging/psychology , Brain/physiology , Circadian Rhythm/physiology , Aged , Analysis of Variance , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain Mapping , Executive Function/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Neural Pathways/diagnostic imaging , Neural Pathways/physiology , Neuronal Plasticity , Neuropsychological Tests , Photoperiod , Rest , Time Factors , Young Adult
9.
Neuroimage ; 159: 280-288, 2017 10 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28782680

ABSTRACT

Brain signal complexity increases with development and is associated with better cognitive outcomes in older age. Research has also shown that bilinguals are able to stave off cognitive decline for longer periods of time than monolinguals, but no studies to date have examined whether bilinguals have more complex brain signals than monolinguals. Here we explored the hypothesis that bilingualism leads to greater brain signal complexity by examining multiscale entropy (MSE) in monolingual and bilingual young adults while EEG was recorded during a task-switching paradigm. Results revealed that bilinguals had greater brain signal complexity than monolinguals in occipital regions. Furthermore, bilinguals performed better with increasing occipital brain signal complexity, whereas monolinguals relied on coupling with frontal regions to demonstrate gains in performance. These findings are discussed in terms of how a lifetime of experience with a second language leads to more automatic and efficient processing of stimuli and how these adaptations could contribute to the prevention of cognitive decline in older age.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Multilingualism , Occipital Lobe/physiology , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
10.
J Neurolinguistics ; 43(A): 17-27, 2017 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28392625

ABSTRACT

Standardized neuropsychological tests are routinely used as diagnostic criteria in aging populations and are an important piece of evidence for the identification of clinical pathology and neurodegenerative conditions such as Alzheimer's disease. Tests include such measures as the Mini Mental Status Exam, Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System, Montreal Cognitive Assessment, and others. These tests cover a range of functions including working memory, verbal fluency, prospective memory, and task switching. Interpretation of test results is based on comparison of the participant's score to standard scores that have been normed on a population database. However, a growing body of research has shown that the skills underlying these tests may be significantly different in monolingual and bilingual older adults, especially for those experiencing cognitive impairment, yet the standardized test scores do not account for such differences. Therefore, results of neuropsychological tests may be different for bilingual populations than for monolinguals, and those differences may be misinterpreted. The issue is important because the consequences of these interpretative errors may be over- or under- diagnosis of cognitive impairment. The present study examined the neuropsychological test scores of monolingual and bilingual older adults who were experiencing healthy aging or cognitive impairment to establish patterns in these scores that can more accurately guide the interpretation for bilingual older adults by considering group differences in the underlying abilities.

11.
Neuroimage ; 139: 231-239, 2016 Oct 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27338513

ABSTRACT

Older adults show decrements in the ability to ignore or suppress distraction relative to younger adults. However, age differences in the neural correlates of distraction control and the role of large-scale network interaction in regulating distractors are scarcely examined. In the current study, we investigated age differences in how the anticorrelation between an externally oriented dorsal attention network (DAN) and an internally focused default mode network (DMN) is related to inhibiting distractors presented during a 1-back working memory task. For both young and older adults, the extent of DAN-DMN anticorrelation predicted reduced distractibility. Activation in a common set of frontal and insular control regions during the task was, however, associated with opposite patterns of network interaction and distractibility in the age groups. For older adults, recruitment of these regions was associated with greater DAN-DMN anticorrelation and less distractibility (better performance). For younger adults, it was associated with decreased DAN-DMN anticorrelation and more distractibility (worse performance). Our findings demonstrate the age-dependent relationship between DAN-DMN interaction patterns and engagement of control regions during an externally oriented distraction control task. This suggests that engagement of those regions may play a compensatory role for older adults but may be indicative of less efficient neural control mechanisms in younger adults.


Subject(s)
Aging , Attention/physiology , Brain/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Middle Aged , Neural Pathways , Young Adult
12.
J Vis ; 16(7): 6, 2016 05 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27153195

ABSTRACT

We assessed age differences in the ability to resolve competition for figural status in stationary displays using small, enclosed, symmetrical silhouettes that participants classified as depicting "novel" or "familiar" shapes. The silhouettes were biased such that the inside was perceived as the shaped figure, and the outside was perceived as a shapeless ground. The critical manipulation was whether a portion of a meaningful object was suggested on the outside of the border of some of the novel silhouettes but not others (M+Ground and M-Ground novel silhouettes, respectively). This manipulation was intended to induce greater inhibitory competition for figural status from the groundside in M+Ground silhouettes than M-Ground silhouettes. In previous studies, young adults classified M+Ground silhouettes as "novel" faster than M-Ground silhouettes (Trujillo, Allen, Schnyer, & Peterson, 2010), suggesting that young adults may recruit more inhibition to resolve figure-ground when there is more competition. We replicated this effect with young adults in the present study, but older adults showed the opposite pattern and were less accurate in classifying M+Ground than M-Ground silhouettes. These results extend the evidence for inhibitory deficits in older adults to figure assignment in stationary displays. The (M+Ground - M-Ground) RT differences were evident in observers' longest responses, consistent with the hypothesis that inhibitory deficits are evident when the need for inhibition is substantial.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Inhibition, Psychological , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
13.
Ann Rheum Dis ; 74(11): 2076-83, 2015 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25015373

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: The mechanisms linking obesity and osteoarthritis (OA) are not fully understood and have been generally attributed to increased weight, rather than metabolic or inflammatory factors. Here, we examined the influence of fatty acids, adipokines, and body weight on OA following joint injury in an obese mouse model. METHODS: Mice were fed high-fat diets rich in various fatty acids (FA) including saturated FAs (SFAs), ω-6 polyunsaturated FAs (PUFAs), and ω-3 PUFAs. OA was induced by destabilising the medial meniscus. Wound healing was evaluated using an ear punch. OA, synovitis and wound healing were determined histologically, while bone changes were measured using microCT. Activity levels and serum cytokines were measured at various time-points. Multivariate models were performed to elucidate the associations of dietary, metabolic and mechanical factors with OA and wound healing. RESULTS: Using weight-matched mice and multivariate models, we found that OA was significantly associated with dietary fatty acid content and serum adipokine levels, but not with body weight. Furthermore, spontaneous activity of the mice was independent of OA development. Small amounts of ω-3 PUFAs (8% by kcal) in a high-fat diet were sufficient to mitigate injury-induced OA, decreasing leptin and resistin levels. ω-3 PUFAs significantly enhanced wound repair, SFAs or ω-6 PUFAs independently increased OA severity, heterotopic ossification and scar tissue formation. CONCLUSIONS: Our results indicate that with obesity, dietary FA content regulates wound healing and OA severity following joint injury, independent of body weight, supporting the need for further studies of dietary FA supplements as a potential therapeutic approach for OA.


Subject(s)
Bone and Bones/drug effects , Ear Auricle/drug effects , Fatty Acids, Omega-3/pharmacology , Fatty Acids, Omega-6/pharmacology , Leg Injuries/pathology , Osteoarthritis/pathology , Stifle/drug effects , Synovitis/pathology , Wound Healing/drug effects , Animals , Body Weight/drug effects , Bone and Bones/diagnostic imaging , Diet, High-Fat , Dietary Fats/pharmacology , Disease Models, Animal , Ear Auricle/injuries , Ear Auricle/pathology , Femur/diagnostic imaging , Femur/drug effects , Leg Injuries/complications , Leptin/metabolism , Mice , Obesity/complications , Osteoarthritis/diagnostic imaging , Osteoarthritis/etiology , Osteoarthritis, Knee , Resistin/metabolism , Stifle/diagnostic imaging , Stifle/injuries , Stifle/pathology , Synovitis/diagnostic imaging , Synovitis/etiology , Tibia/diagnostic imaging , Tibia/drug effects , Tibial Meniscus Injuries , X-Ray Microtomography
15.
J Athl Train ; 2024 Jun 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38835326

ABSTRACT

CONTEXT: Underreporting of concussion symptoms in college athletics presents a challenge for sports medicine clinicians in evaluating and diagnosing such injuries. Some athletes do not report concussion symptoms because they do not recognize that they have a brain injury, however many athletes intentionally withhold symptoms to avoid removal from sport participation. OBJECTIVE: To examine individual factors that influence college athletes' intentions to report concussion symptoms. DESIGN: Cross-sectional study. SETTING: Collegiate athletics. PARTICIPANTS: 2,649 student-athletes from 23 sports, across 22 colleges/universities. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES: The primary outcome was intention to report concussion symptoms. Predictor variables included demographics (age, race/ethnicity, sex, sport type, number of years in sport, number of previous concussions, and perceived concussion symptom knowledge), athletic identity, attitudes toward symptom reporting, perceived social pressure (injunctive and descriptive norms), and perceived behavioral control (capacity and autonomy). RESULTS: Hierarchical ordinary least squares regression revealed positive effects of attitude (b = .063; P = .005), descriptive norms (b = .131; P < .001), injunctive norms (b = .107; P < .001), and capacity (b = .196; P < .001) on intention to report symptoms. Athletic identity and participation in collision sports had small negative indirect effects on intention, while perceived concussion knowledge had a small positive indirect effect. The full regression model explained 14.24% of the variance in concussion reporting intention. CONCLUSIONS: These findings may help clinicians develop more focused interventions that address key social and individual determinants of underreporting, including attitude, injunctive and descriptive norms, and capacity to report. Athletic identity, sport type, and perceived understanding of concussion symptoms also influence reporting intention to a lesser extent. Previous research in this area has often failed to address a diverse population of college-age athletes from different sports and NCAA divisions.

16.
Commun Biol ; 7(1): 938, 2024 Aug 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39097670

ABSTRACT

Brain and breathing activities are closely related. However, the exact neurophysiological mechanisms that couple the brain and breathing to stimuli in the external environment are not yet agreed upon. Our data support that synchronization and dynamic attunement are two key mechanisms that couple local brain activity and breathing to external periodic stimuli. First, we review the existing literature, which provides strong evidence for the synchronization of brain and breathing in terms of coherence, cross-frequency coupling and phase-based entrainment. Second, using EEG and breathing data, we show that both the lungs and localized brain activity at the Cz channel attune the temporal structure of their power spectra to the periodic structure of external auditory inputs. We highlight the role of dynamic attunement in playing a key role in coordinating the tripartite temporal alignment of localized brain activity, breathing and input dynamics across longer timescales like minutes. Overall, this perspective sheds light on potential mechanisms of brain-breathing coupling and its alignment to stimuli in the external environment.


Subject(s)
Brain , Electroencephalography , Respiration , Brain/physiology , Humans , Male , Environment
17.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 242: 104117, 2024 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38134682

ABSTRACT

The current study utilized the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA) data to investigate the relationship between sexual orientation and cognitive health of the aging population. Cognitive flexibility and verbal fluency were examined as outcome variables in the study. A total of 45,993 respondents were included in the analyses. Each model had social support or social participation as a mediator. A series of mediation analysis, stratified by gender, revealed that aging gay men performed better in cognitive tasks related to cognitive flexibility when compared to their heterosexual counterparts. The results also indicated that social support is a protective factor for cognitive health in aging lesbian women. This study provides an opportunity to consider how clinical and social services can strategize to build inclusive environments for the aging sexual minority population.


Subject(s)
Aging , Sexual Behavior , Humans , Male , Female , Aged , Longitudinal Studies , Canada/epidemiology , Aging/psychology , Cognition
18.
Neuropsychologia ; 195: 108801, 2024 03 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38244768

ABSTRACT

This study aimed to systematically investigate the relationship between bilingualism, age, L2 onset age of acquisition (AoA), and white matter integrity (operationalized as fractional anisotropy, FA), addressing inconsistencies in the literature. We conducted a meta-analysis of 23 studies and used meta-regression models to assess the influence of age and L2AoA on effect sizes in studies comparing monolinguals and bilinguals. Even though the overall between-group effect size across the whole brain was unreliable, bilingualism was associated with increased white matter integrity in specific tracts and in groups with a limited range of age and L2AoA. Age had a small, negative effect on white matter integrity, with differences between monolinguals and bilinguals more pronounced in younger adults, consistent with a view of an initial increase in white matter integrity, followed by remodeling for efficiency over time. In contrast, later L2AoA was associated with greater white matter integrity in bilinguals than monolinguals, again consistent with the remodeling for efficiency model. Our findings highlight the importance of considering age and L2AoA when examining the neural basis of bilingualism on white matter in the brain and how bilingualism contributes to structural changes that stave off cognitive decline in older age.


Subject(s)
Multilingualism , White Matter , Adult , Humans , Anisotropy , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain Mapping , White Matter/diagnostic imaging , Young Adult , Aged
19.
Biol Psychiatry Glob Open Sci ; 4(1): 374-384, 2024 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38298786

ABSTRACT

Background: Major depressive disorder (MDD) in late life is a risk factor for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer's disease. However, studies of gray matter changes have produced varied estimates of which structures are implicated in MDD and dementia. Changes in gray matter volume and cortical thickness are macrostructural measures for the microstructural processes of free water accumulation and dendritic spine loss. Methods: We conducted multishell diffusion imaging to assess gray matter microstructure in 244 older adults with remitted MDD (n = 44), MCI (n = 115), remitted MDD+MCI (n = 61), or without psychiatric disorders or cognitive impairment (healthy control participants; n = 24). We estimated measures related to neurite density, orientation dispersion, and free water (isotropic volume fraction) using a biophysically plausible model (neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging). Results: Results showed that increasing age was correlated with an increase in isotropic volume fraction and a decrease in orientation dispersion index, which is consistent with neuropathology dendritic loss. In addition, this relationship between age and increased isotropic volume fraction was more disrupted in the MCI group than in the remitted MDD or healthy control groups. However, the association between age and orientation dispersion index was similar for all 3 groups. Conclusions: The findings suggest that the neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging measures could be used to identify biological risk factors for Alzheimer's disease, signifying both conventional neurodegeneration observed with MCI and dendritic loss seen in MDD.

20.
Phys Sportsmed ; 41(2): 75-86, 2013 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23703520

ABSTRACT

Articular cartilage injuries of the knee are an increasingly common source of pain and dysfunction, particularly in the athletic population. In the athlete, untreated articular cartilage defects can represent a career threatening injury and create a significant obstacle in returning to full athletic participation. The markedly limited healing potential of articular cartilage often leads to continued deterioration and progressive functional limitations. Numerous studies have shown that full thickness articular cartilage lesions are frequently encountered at the time of arthroscopy, particularly associated with athletic injury. A variety of surgical treatment options exist, including debridement, microfracture, osteochondral autograft, osteochondral allograft, and autologous chondrocyte implantation. Each technique has advantages and limitations for restoring articular cartilage function, and emerging technology continues to improve the results of treatment. Our article provides an evidence-based review on the etiology and prevalence of articular cartilage injuries in athletes, along with the principles and techniques available for restoring articular cartilage function following injury.


Subject(s)
Athletic Injuries/surgery , Cartilage, Articular/injuries , Cartilage, Articular/surgery , Knee Injuries/surgery , Algorithms , Anterior Cruciate Ligament Reconstruction , Arthroplasty, Subchondral/methods , Arthroscopy , Athletic Injuries/diagnosis , Bone Marrow/physiology , Chondrocytes/transplantation , Debridement/methods , Diagnosis, Differential , Diagnostic Imaging , Humans , Knee Injuries/diagnosis
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL