Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 16 de 16
Filter
1.
Psychol Aging ; 13(4): 584-96, 1998 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9883459

ABSTRACT

Differences in duration judgments made by younger and older adults were reviewed. Previous research is unclear about whether such differences exist and, if so, how they may be explained. The meta-analyses revealed substantial age-related differences. Older adults gave larger verbal estimates and made shorter productions of duration than did younger adults. There were no age-related differences in reproduction of duration or in psychophysical slope relating judged and target duration. Older adults' duration estimates were more variable than were those of younger ones. Findings are discussed in terms of pacemaker rate and attentional resources. An explanation regarding divided attention between nontemporal and temporal information processing best explains the findings.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Attention/physiology , Mental Processes/physiology , Time Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aging/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
2.
J Bone Joint Surg Am ; 61(7): 1071-2, 1979 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-489650

ABSTRACT

Seven of eight patients with suspected sarco-iliac joint infection had the diagnosis confirmed by closed aspiration of the joint. A reproducible method of obtaining specimens for culture was developed. Failures of aspiration may occur in patients in whom anatomical variations cause segmentation of the joint, therefore preventing the septic process from involving the most posterior and caudal part.


Subject(s)
Arthritis, Infectious/diagnosis , Biopsy, Needle/methods , Pseudomonas Infections/diagnosis , Sacroiliac Joint , Humans , Male
3.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 8(6): 530-44, 1982 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6218220

ABSTRACT

Three experiments investigated effects of environmental context on temporal memory judgments. An equal number of items occurred within each of two equal durations (D1 and D2). Subjects subsequently were asked to judge the length of a given duration in comparison with the other, then to discriminate the correct list and serial position for each recognized item on a test. If environmental context was unchanged, D1 was remembered as being longer than D2; if the context was disrupted during the interval separating D1 and D2, this effect was reduced; and if the context prevailing during D2 also was changed, the effect was eliminated. List discrimination was improved only if the context was changed. Serial-position judgment was not affected by either manipulation. Changes in process context--the internal context produced by the performance of specific cognitive processes--lengthened remembered duration, but the effect did not simply add on to the effects of environmental context. Results are discussed in terms of a contextual-change hypothesis.


Subject(s)
Discrimination Learning , Social Environment , Time Perception , Attention , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall , Serial Learning
4.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 4(2): 184-97, 1997 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21331825

ABSTRACT

A meta-analytic review compared prospective and retrospective judgments of duration, or duration judgment paradigm. Some theorists have concluded that the two paradigms involve similar cognitive processes, whereas others have found that they involve different processes. A review of 20 experiments revealed that prospective judgments are longer and less variable than are retrospective judgments. Several theoretically important variables moderate these effects, especially those concerned with information processing activities. Therefore, somewhat different cognitive processes subserve experienced and remembered duration. Attentional models are needed to explain prospective judgments, and memory-based models are needed to explain retrospective judgments. These findings clarify models of human duration judgment and suggest directions for future research. Evidence on duration judgments may also influence models of attention and memory.

5.
J Psychol ; 134(4): 443-61, 2000 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10908076

ABSTRACT

The authors investigated the extent to which racial factors, cultural factors, or both influence a person's beliefs about physical time, personal time, and experienced and remembered duration. A total of 750 Black American, Black African, and White American students responded to a questionnaire on these beliefs about time. Factor analysis was used to compare belief structures. Pairwise comparisons, performed separately for each statement, tested the direction and strength of the reported beliefs. The groups showed many similarities, but they also showed some differences. All 3 groups differed in beliefs about physical and personal time, but they did not differ in beliefs about duration experiences. This evidence does not support simplistic views of racial or cultural influences. Culture may differentially influence beliefs about physical time and personal time. Beliefs about duration experiences may represent an ethnic factor that transcends cultures.


Subject(s)
Black or African American/psychology , Cultural Characteristics , Time Perception , White People/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Black People , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Culture , Factor Analysis, Statistical , Female , Humans , Malawi/ethnology , Male , Surveys and Questionnaires , United States
7.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 62(2): 103-22, 1986 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3766191
11.
Mem Cognit ; 2(1): 153-60, 1974 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24214715

ABSTRACT

Two experiments investigated the relationship between long-term memory for events occurring during an interval and the experience of duration of the interval in retrospect. In both, Ss attended to a sequence consisting of a standard, an experimental, and a second standard interval. Then unexpected comparative duration and memory judgments were requested. In Experiment I, either 30 or 60 unrelated words occurred during the 180-see experimental interval. When more words had occurred, judgments of duration of the experimental interval, judgments of number of words presented, and number of words recognized all increased, but free recall of words was unaffected. In Experiment II, 80 categorized words occurred during the 160-see experimental interval, with category instances in either blocked or random order. When words were blocked by category, judgments of duration of the experimental interval, free recall, and recognition all increased, but judgments of number of words were unaffected. Results were discussed in terms of Ornstein's (1969) "storage size" hypothesis.

12.
Mem Cognit ; 28(8): 1333-46, 2000 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11219961

ABSTRACT

We quantitatively reviewed human sex differences in the magnitude and variability of duration judgments. Data from 4,794 females and 4,688 males yielded 87 effect size estimates of magnitude and 28 of variability. The overall sex difference in duration judgment magnitude was small but statistically significant. It was moderated by whether study participants knew in advance (prospective paradigm) or only later (retrospective paradigm) that they would be required to judge duration. Although prospective judgments showed no overall sex effect, some levels of moderator variables showed a small but statistically significant effect. Retrospective judgments showed a larger subjective-to-objective duration ratio for females than for males, and several variables moderated this effect. Females' judgments also showed more intersubject variability than did males' judgments. Relative to males, females sustain attention to time more in the prospective paradigm and have better episodic memory in the retrospective paradigm.


Subject(s)
Attention , Judgment , Memory, Short-Term , Time Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Child , Female , Humans , Knowledge of Results, Psychological , Male , Middle Aged , Sex Factors
13.
Mem Cognit ; 3(3): 287-94, 1975 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21287075

ABSTRACT

Three experiments examined effects of the spacing of repetitions on memory for pictures. In Experiment I, the duration of the first presentation (P(1)) was manipulated, as was P(1)-P(2) spacing. The effect of spacing on judged frequency was independent of P(1) duration. In Experiment II, pictures were given M massed presentations just prior to the P(M)-P(M+1) spacing interval. The form of the spacing curve was independent of M. Neither experiment confirmed the prediction of "overhabituation," derived from the habituation-recovery explanation of the spacing effect. In Experiment III, subjects made both duration and frequency judgments. The duration judgment results were not consistent with the notion that subjects remember multiple massed presentations as single occurrences of especially long duration. Some evidence from Experiments I and III suggests that an interrupted stimulus is recognized better than one that is not interrupted-a finding that, if replicable, would support the habituation-recovery account of the spacing effect.

14.
Brain Cogn ; 9(2): 181-90, 1989 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2923709

ABSTRACT

Relative nostril efficiency (nasal cycle) is related to hemispheric EEG differences and performance on cognitive tasks. We investigated how unilateral forced nostril breathing influences spatial and verbal performance. Right-handed males and females performed both tasks under either left-nostril, right-nostril, or free-breathing conditions. Unilateral breathing affects performance differently in males and females. It influences male performance ipsilaterally on both tasks: Their spatial performance is better during right-nostril breathing, and their verbal performance is better during left-nostril breathing. Unilateral breathing influences female performance contralaterally, but only on the spatial task: Their spatial performance is better during left-nostril breathing. These differences within and between sexes may exist because unilateral nostril breathing differentially activates the two hemispheres and thereby facilitates performance, or because attempts of the brain to control the nasal cycle unilaterally interfere with performance.


Subject(s)
Arousal/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Pulmonary Ventilation , Adult , Evoked Potentials , Female , Humans , Male , Orientation/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Semantics , Verbal Learning/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL