Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 45
Filter
Add more filters

Country/Region as subject
Affiliation country
Publication year range
1.
J Head Trauma Rehabil ; 35(5): E450-E457, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32472838

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: (1) To explore the construct validity of the Ambiguous Intentions Hostility Questionnaire (AIHQ) in participants with traumatic brain injury (TBI) (ie, confirm negative attributions are associated with anger and aggression); and (2) use the AIHQ to examine negative attribution differences between participants with and without TBI. SETTING: Two rehabilitation hospitals. PARTICIPANTS: Eighty-five adults with TBI and 86 healthy controls (HCs). DESIGN: Cross-sectional survey. MAIN MEASURES: The AIHQ, a measure of negative attributions (intent, hostility, and blame), anger, and aggressive responses to hypothetical scenarios. RESULTS: Attributions were significantly correlated with anticipated anger and aggressive responses to AIHQ scenarios. Compared with HCs, participants with TBI reported stronger negative attributions (P ≤ .001), anger (P = .021), and aggressive responses (P = .002) to the scenarios. CONCLUSION: Negative attributions were associated with anger and aggression responses, demonstrating construct validity of the AIHQ in the TBI population. Participants with TBI judged others' behaviors more severely than HCs, similar to prior research using a different attribution measure. The AIHQ has promise as a practical instrument for assessing negative attributions after TBI.


Subject(s)
Aggression , Brain Injuries , Hostility , Intention , Surveys and Questionnaires , Adult , Brain Injuries/complications , Case-Control Studies , Cross-Sectional Studies , Humans
2.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry ; 20(3): 198-221, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25675960

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Individuals with schizophrenia consistently show impairments in social cognition (SC). SC has become a potential treatment target due to its association with functional outcomes. An alternative method of assessment is to administer an observer-based scale incorporating an informant's "first hand" impressions in ratings. METHODS: The present study used the Observable Social Cognition: A Rating Scale (OSCARS) in 62 outpatients and 50 non-psychiatric controls (NPCs) to assess performance in domains of SC (e.g. emotion perception, theory of mind). RESULTS: The OSCARS demonstrated sufficient internal consistency and test-retest reliability. Construct validity was assessed through an exploratory factor analysis. Patient OSCARS indices were not significantly correlated with measures of SC with the exception of aggressive attributional style. Individuals with less impairment in SC reacted more aggressively to ambiguous situations. NPC OSCARS were significantly correlated with measures of theory of mind and attributional style. In a combined sample of patients and controls, six of eight items were significantly correlated with the SC task assessing the same domain, providing modest evidence of convergent validity. In patients, the OSCARS was significantly correlated with measures of functional outcome and neurocognition. Last, the OSCARS was found to be significantly associated with functional outcome after the influence of objective measures of SC was statistically removed. CONCLUSIONS: The present study provides preliminary evidence that the OSCARS may be useful for clinicians in collecting data about patients' potential real-world SC deficits, in turn increasing the degree to which these impairments may be targeted in treatment.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Schizophrenia/diagnosis , Schizophrenic Psychology , Social Behavior , Social Perception , Surveys and Questionnaires , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Reproducibility of Results , Surveys and Questionnaires/standards
3.
Br J Clin Psychol ; 53(3): 281-98, 2014 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24417608

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: In schizophrenia, the ability to adaptively infer the thoughts and feelings of others (i.e., social cognition) is strongly associated with community functioning. Researchers have designed psychosocial interventions to improve social cognition with the aim of improving downstream social functioning. Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) is one such intervention. Previous research on SCIT has been promising, but has consisted largely of smaller trials with insufficient experimental control. DESIGN: Randomized, controlled trial. METHODS: The current article reports on a controlled trial of 66 adults with schizophrenia randomized to receive either SCIT (n = 33), delivered in weekly group sessions, or treatment as usual (n = 33) for 6 months. Participants completed assessments of social cognition, social functioning, neurocognition and symptoms at baseline, post-treatment, and 3-month follow-up. RESULTS: Primary analyses suggest that SCIT may improve social functioning, negative symptoms, and possibly hostile attributional bias. Post-hoc analyses suggest a dose-response effect. CONCLUSIONS: Findings are discussed in the context of continuing to refine and improve social cognitive interventions for schizophrenia. PRACTITIONER POINTS: Social cognitive intervention is a feasible and promising approach to improving social functioning among individuals with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. Dose-response findings suggest that delivering social cognitive interventions with greater frequency may maximize their benefit to patients. Research on social cognitive interventions is still young and effects from well-controlled trials have been inconsistent. It is not yet clear which components of social cognitive training may be the key active ingredients.


Subject(s)
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy/methods , Outpatients/psychology , Psychotherapy, Group , Schizophrenia/therapy , Adult , Cognition/physiology , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Middle Aged , Outpatients/statistics & numerical data , Social Adjustment , Treatment Outcome
4.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 46(1): 55-66, 2024 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38346160

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Many commonly employed performance validity tests (PVTs) are several decades old and vulnerable to compromise, leading to a need for novel instruments. Because implicit/non-declarative memory may be robust to brain damage, tasks that rely upon such memory may serve as an effective PVT. Using a simulation design, this experiment evaluated whether novel tasks that rely upon perceptual memory hold promise as PVTs. METHOD: Sixty healthy participants were provided instructions to simulate symptoms of mild traumatic brain injury (TBI), and they were compared to a group of 20 honest responding individuals. Simulator groups received varying levels of information concerning TBI symptoms, resulting in naïve, sophisticated, and test-coached groups. The Word Memory Test, Test of Memory Malingering, and California Verbal Learning Test-II Forced Choice Recognition Test were administered. To assess perceptual memory, selected images from the Gollin Incomplete Figures and Mooney Closure Test were presented as visual perception tasks. After brief delays, memory for the images was assessed. RESULTS: No group differences emerged on the perception trials of the Gollin and Mooney figures, but simulators remembered fewer images than the honest responders. Simulator groups differed on the standard PVTs, but they performed equivalently on the Gollin and Mooney figures, implying robustness to coaching. Relying upon a criterion of 90% specificity, the Gollin and Mooney figures achieved at least 90% sensitivity, comparing favorably to the standard PVTs. CONCLUSIONS: The Gollin and Mooney figures hold promise as novel PVTs. As perceptual memory tests, they may be relatively robust to brain damage, but future research involving clinical samples is necessary to substantiate this assertion.


Subject(s)
Brain Concussion , Malingering , Neuropsychological Tests , Humans , Male , Female , Adult , Neuropsychological Tests/standards , Malingering/diagnosis , Young Adult , Brain Concussion/diagnosis , Brain Concussion/physiopathology , Reproducibility of Results , Visual Perception/physiology , Memory/physiology , Middle Aged
5.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry ; 18(6): 531-48, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23445398

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Persons with nonclinical paranoia show many of the same biases as those with clinical paranoia, suggesting that paranoia exists on a continuum. However, little is known about the various social cognitive processes found in paranoia and how these relate to social functioning and social behaviours in general. This study will examine performance on emotion perception and attributional style measures and their relationship to social functioning, social problem solving, and social skill. A key element in this study will be the incorporation of ambiguity in the perception of emotional expressions and the assignment of attributional blame, which appears to be an important, yet neglected, construct in paranoia. METHODS: Twenty-six persons with high levels of nonclinical paranoia and 31 persons with low levels of paranoia completed measures of emotion perception, attributional style, social functioning, and social problem solving. Salient and subtle emotional expressions were used to examine how ambiguity impacts emotion perception in paranoia. RESULTS: The group high in nonclinical paranoia showed reduced accuracy for subtle negative emotional expressions and showed more perceived hostility and blame for ambiguous social situations as compared to the group low in nonclinical paranoia. Also, the high nonclinical paranoia group reported less social engagement, fewer social contacts, and more problems in social perception and social skill than the group low in nonclinical paranoia. CONCLUSION: Social cognitive and social functioning biases are found in persons with high levels of nonclinical paranoia. Possible mechanisms of these biases and relevance for treatment approaches are discussed.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Emotions , Paranoid Disorders/psychology , Social Behavior , Social Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Paranoid Disorders/physiopathology , Social Adjustment , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
6.
Psychiatry Res ; 187(1-2): 106-12, 2011 May 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21126772

ABSTRACT

Incapacity to make decisions about medical treatment is associated with neuropsychological impairment in a variety of illnesses. Although cognitive deficits occur often in people with major depressive illness, little research has studied its association with decisional capacity. The present investigation examined ability to understand treatment disclosures, which is a core component of decisional capacity, in 31 inpatients with depression and 16 normal controls. Depressed inpatients with diminished neuropsychological function showed poor understanding of treatment disclosures compared to the control group. Nonetheless, with sufficient cueing, depressed inpatients with diminished neuropsychological function were able to display understanding that was equivalent to the control group. Exploratory regression analyses revealed that diminished new-learning correlated with poorer understanding. Implications of these results for clinical practice and medical research involving people with major depressive illness are discussed.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/etiology , Cognition Disorders/psychology , Decision Making , Depressive Disorder, Major/complications , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Female , Humans , Informed Consent , Male , Mental Competency , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Predictive Value of Tests , Regression Analysis
7.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 43(7): 655-662, 2021 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34686108

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: The Word Memory Test (WMT) assesses non-credible performance in neuropsychological assessment. To mitigate risk of false positives among patients with severe cognitive dysfunction, the Genuine Memory Impairment Profile was derived. Only a modest number of investigations has evaluated classification accuracy among clinical samples, leaving the GMIP's accuracy largely uncertain. Accordingly, a simulation experiment evaluated the classification accuracy of the GMIP in a group of healthy individuals coached to simulate mild traumatic brain injury (TBI) related memory impairment on the WMT. PARTICIPANTS AND METHODS: Eighty healthy individuals were randomly assigned to one of the four experimental groups. One group was provided superficial information concerning TBI symptoms (naïve simulators), another was provided extensive information concerning TBI symptoms (sophisticated simulators), and a third group was provided extensive TBI symptom information and tactics to evade detection by performance validity tests (PVT) (test-coached). An honest responding control group was directed to give their best performance. All participants were administered the California Verbal Learning Test-2 (CVLT-2) and the WMT. RESULTS: Among the TBI simulators, 90% of the test-coached, 95% of the sophisticated simulators, and 100% of the naïve simulators were correctly classified as exaggerating memory impairment on the primary WMT indices. The simulator groups performed worse than the honest responding group on the CVLT-2. Of those who exceeded the WMT cutoffs, 60%, 27%, and 6% of the naïve-, sophisticated-, and test-coached simulators manifested the GMIP profile, respectively. CONCLUSIONS: The GMIP is apt to misclassify individuals as having genuine memory impairment, especially if a naïve or unsophisticated effort is made to exert non-credible performance. Indeed, individuals who employ the least sophisticated efforts to exaggerate cognitive impairment appear most likely to manifest the GMIP. The GMIP should be used cautiously to discriminate genuine impairment from non-credible performance, especially among people with mild TBI.


Subject(s)
Brain Concussion , Cognitive Dysfunction , Cognitive Dysfunction/diagnosis , Humans , Malingering/diagnosis , Memory , Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Memory Disorders/etiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Reproducibility of Results
8.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 43(2): 117-128, 2021 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33622171

ABSTRACT

Objective: Cognitive impairment affects as many as 65% of people with multiple sclerosis (PWMS), and memory impairment confers greater severity of disability and functional impairment. Depression is also common among PWMS, and lifetime prevalence rates are as high as 50%. Research has yet to clearly define the relationship between memory dysfunction and depression among PWMS, and may reflect incomplete assessment of depressive symptoms. The present study examined different aspects of depressive symptoms including anhedonia (i.e., diminished positive mood) and their relationships with verbal learning and memory among PWMS.Method: Participants were 48 healthy individuals and 96 PWMS. They were primarily Caucasian (90.3%) and female (75.0%). Participants completed the California Verbal Learning Test-2 (CVLT-2) to assess verbal learning and memory and the Chicago Multiscale Depression inventory to assess depressed mood (CMDI-Mood) and diminished positive mood (CMDI-DPM).Results: Linear regression revealed that the main effect of CMDI-DPM and the interaction of CMDI-DPM and CMDI-Mood significantly explained variance across learning, recall, and recognition CVLT-2 indices. Follow-up analyses indicated that CMDI-DPM was only significant in the absence of high CMDI-Mood scores. CMDI-Mood explained variance in only CVLT-2 Trial B.Conclusions: Depressed mood had little direct effect upon memory performance in PWMS. In the absence of severe depressed mood, higher levels of positive mood corresponded to better memory performance. However, the impact of diminished positive mood was rendered null among those endorsing high levels of depressed mood. These data may imply that anhedonia corresponds with poorer memory function among PWMS, and suggests that investigators and clinicians should assess multiple mood dimensions among PWMS.


Subject(s)
Depressive Disorder , Multiple Sclerosis , Affect , Female , Humans , Memory Disorders/etiology , Multiple Sclerosis/complications , Verbal Learning
9.
Neuropsychology ; 35(2): 207-219, 2021 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33764111

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: As many as 65% of people with multiple sclerosis (MS) have clinically significant memory impairment, but the nature of this deficit is controversial. Some investigations suggest that an inability to retrieve newly learned information from memory is prominent, whereas others imply that compromised acquisition accounts for impairment. Prior research has not simultaneously evaluated acquisition and retrieval processes in MS, and fewer have attempted to account for initial acquisition when studying retrieval. The Item Specific Deficit Approach (ISDA) offers a method of quantifying acquisition, retrieval, and retention processes, with the latter two mechanisms being adjusted for initial acquisition. To simultaneously quantify acquisition and retrieval abilities, the ISDA was applied to list learning performance in two independent samples of people with MS and corresponding healthy comparison groups. PARTICIPANTS AND METHODS: Study 1 included 85 people with MS and 47 healthy individuals. Study 2 involved a separate sample of 79 people with MS and 22 healthy people. They were administered neuropsychological batteries, and participants with MS were classified as globally impaired or unimpaired. The California Verbal Learning Test-II was administered to assess new-learning in both studies, and responses were scored using the ISDA. RESULTS: Both studies revealed that cognitively impaired people with MS manifest weaknesses involving acquisition and retrieval. Nearly identical effect sizes emerged across samples, with cognitive impairment achieving a medium effect upon acquisition and a large effect upon retrieval. CONCLUSIONS: These findings accord well with previous research showing diminished acquisition and retrieval among people with MS. The results may also reconcile contradictory findings in the extant literature by showing that memory impairment in MS is not exclusively attributable to either acquisition or retrieval. Rather, both processes may manifest across people with MS. The replication across samples with nearly identical effect sizes implies that these effects are reliable and possess external validity. These data hold implications for memory rehabilitation interventions involving people with MS, and suggest that acquisition and retrieval processes should be addressed in treatment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Memory Disorders/physiopathology , Multiple Sclerosis/psychology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
10.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry ; 14(1): 30-52, 2009 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19214841

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Current models of paranoia propose that ambiguous situations, in which cues regarding the intentions of others are lacking, may be perceived as hostile by persons with persecutory delusions (PD). Thus, a social-cognitive bias for the perception of hostility may be present. In this study, the Ambiguous Intentions Hostility Questionnaire (AIHQ) was used to present situations that are ambiguous regarding the intentions of others. It was predicted that on the AIHQ, persons with PD would show greater levels of perceived hostility, blame, and aggression than both psychiatric and nonpsychiatric controls. METHODS: The sample comprised 32 persons with PD, 28 persons without PD (psychiatric controls), and 50 healthy participants (nonpsychiatric controls). Participants completed the AIHQ along with measures of paranoia, attributional style, depression, anxiety, self-esteem, and public self-consciousness. RESULTS: As predicted, the group with PD showed greater perceptions of hostility, blame, and aggression scores for ambiguous situations on the AIHQ. Also, the AIHQ Hostility bias score was predictive of paranoid ideation. CONCLUSIONS: Persons with PD showed a social-cognitive bias for perceiving hostility in ambiguous situations. The bias appears to be specific as it was not found in the psychiatric or nonpsychiatric control groups. Suggestions for future research are discussed.


Subject(s)
Delusions/psychology , Hostility , Paranoid Disorders/psychology , Social Perception , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Depression/psychology , Education , Fear/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Personality Tests , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Self Concept , Socioeconomic Factors , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
12.
Neuropsychology ; 33(2): 212-221, 2019 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30589284

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Among patients with brain damage, executive function deficits and impulsivity correspond with propensity to engage in risky behaviors. Less research has addressed this issue in healthy adults, and fewer still have simultaneously evaluated the importance of executive function and impulsive personality. Additionally, most research has focused exclusively on substance use while ignoring other domains of risky behavior such as sexual activity and antisocial practices. Toward this end, we examined the association of risky behaviors with executive function and self-reported impulsivity. METHOD: Healthy undergraduates (n = 56) were administered the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), and UPPS-P impulsivity questionnaire. A self-report questionnaire assessed risky sexual activities, drug use, and antisocial behaviors over a 2-month interval. RESULTS: After accounting for social desirability and performance effort, multiple regression analyses revealed a robust relationship between executive function and risk-taking. Specifically, WCST performance correlated with risky sexual and substance use behavior, whereas the IGT was uniquely linked with antisocial acts. Trait impulsivity was positively associated with sexual behavior. CONCLUSIONS: Extending previous research, executive function accounted for more variance in risky behaviors than self-reported impulsivity, but this was mediated by facet of executive function. Decision-making under risk seemed to better account for antisocial acts, whereas perseveration was more strongly linked to sexual activity and substance misuse. These data imply that poor executive function increases the likelihood that healthy young adults will engage in risky and potentially dangerous acts, extending the ecological validity of the WCST and IGT. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Decision Making/physiology , Executive Function/physiology , Impulsive Behavior/physiology , Risk-Taking , Adolescent , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Personality/physiology , Self Report , Students , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
13.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 41(4): 331-340, 2019 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30642223

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: As many as 70% of people with multiple sclerosis (MS) have clinically significant cognitive impairment, and most of these individuals exhibit executive dysfunction. Most research concerning executive dysfunction in MS has focused upon nonverbal measures. The Verbal Concept Attainment Test (VCAT) has demonstrated construct validity as an executive function measure in people infected with HIV and in people with focal brain lesions, but its validity among people with MS is unknown. The current study evaluated the VCAT's criterion, diagnostic, and ecological validity in people with MS. METHOD: A comprehensive neuropsychological battery was administered to 44 healthy individuals and 97 people with MS. Based on existing norms, they were classified as impaired or unimpaired, resulting in 65 people with MS categorized as unimpaired and 32 as impaired. They were administered a battery assessing neuropsychological impairment and disability status. RESULTS: The VCAT correlated with most measures of neuropsychological function, but its largest correlations occurred with measures of executive function, working memory, and verbal memory. Regarding classification accuracy, the VCAT achieved satisfactory sensitivity and specificity in identifying neuropsychological impairment in people with MS. The VCAT achieved moderate correlations with measures of disability status. CONCLUSIONS: The data provide evidence for an optimal VCAT cutoff score for establishing neuropsychological impairment in people with MS, and they demonstrate that the VCAT possesses acceptable criterion, diagnostic, and ecological validity. As such, these data support the inclusion of the VCAT in research and clinical practice involving people with MS.


Subject(s)
Cognitive Dysfunction/diagnosis , Concept Formation/physiology , Executive Function/physiology , Memory/physiology , Multiple Sclerosis/psychology , Adult , Cognitive Dysfunction/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Reproducibility of Results
14.
Schizophr Res ; 105(1-3): 68-77, 2008 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18585899

ABSTRACT

Inability to recognize emotional expressions of others (emotion perception) is one of the most common impairments observed among individuals with schizophrenia. Such deficits presumably contribute much to the social dysfunction characteristic of schizophrenia. This study examined the efficacy of a novel attentional-shaping intervention to improve emotion perception abilities. Sixty participants with schizophrenia were randomly assigned to one of three intervention conditions: 1) attentional-shaping, 2) contingent monetary reinforcement, or 3) repeated practice. Participants completed the Face Emotion Identification Test (FEIT) at pre-test, intervention, post-test, and one week follow-up. Participants also completed the Bell-Lysaker Emotion Recognition Test (BLERT) and the Social Behavior Scale at pre-test and follow-up to measure generalization. The results showed that the attentional-shaping condition had significantly higher scores on the FEIT at intervention, post-test, and follow-up compared to monetary reinforcement and repeated practice. Improvement was also found on the BLERT and a trend was found for improved social behaviors at one-week follow-up. Results will be discussed in terms of face scanning and attentional deficits present in schizophrenia and potential uses of this intervention in the remediation of emotion perception deficits.


Subject(s)
Attention , Cognition Disorders/therapy , Cognitive Behavioral Therapy/methods , Emotions , Expressed Emotion , Facial Expression , Schizophrenia/therapy , Schizophrenic Psychology , Adult , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Cognition Disorders/psychology , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Recognition, Psychology , Reinforcement, Psychology , Schizophrenia/diagnosis , Social Behavior , Social Perception , Treatment Outcome , Visual Perception
15.
Schizophr Res ; 94(1-3): 139-47, 2007 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17561378

ABSTRACT

Persons with schizophrenia exhibit consistent deficits in emotion perception (recognizing the emotional expressions of others), but it is currently unclear if their performance represents a specific deficit in identifying emotions only or is a more generalized deficit across different perception tasks. To address this question, it is important to compare emotion perception with face and general visual perception to assess the type of deficit present. The equivocal nature of previous research may suggest the presence of subtypes with different levels and patterns of performance on social perception measures. In this study, we administered measures of emotion, face, and general perception to a sample of 100 persons with schizophrenia. These scores were then subjected to a cluster analysis to determine if different subtypes were present. Two distinct subtypes were identified, and both subtypes scored lower than normal controls across all three measures of perception, suggesting the presence of a generalized performance deficit. One subtype was characterized by mild to moderate impairment and the other showed more severe impairment. The cluster solution was stable, and the subtypes also differed on other variables not used in the cluster analysis (external validation). More specifically, persons in the mild to moderately impaired subtype reported fewer positive symptoms, and this subtype contained more persons with paranoid schizophrenia as compared to the more severely impaired subtype. The implications of the results for the study of social cognition in schizophrenia are discussed.


Subject(s)
Expressed Emotion , Facial Expression , Schizophrenia/complications , Social Perception , Adult , Antipsychotic Agents/therapeutic use , Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , Discrimination, Psychological , Female , Humans , Male , Psychological Tests , Recognition, Psychology , Schizophrenia/drug therapy , Schizophrenic Psychology , Surveys and Questionnaires
16.
Schizophr Res ; 91(1-3): 112-6, 2007 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17293083

ABSTRACT

Individuals with schizophrenia exhibit consistent deficits in social cognition such as emotion perception, attributional style, and theory of mind, which may be targets of psychosocial treatments. Previous intervention studies have typically focused on only one aspect of social cognition and have not assessed generalization of treatment to improvements in social functioning. This paper describes preliminary data from a new group-based treatment, Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT), aimed at improving social cognition in schizophrenia. Eighteen inpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders completed SCIT and were compared with 10 inpatients who completed a coping skills group. Participants were assessed at pre-test and post-test on measures of emotion and social perception, theory of mind, attributional style (e.g., blame, hostility, and aggression), cognitive flexibility, and social relationships. We also collected data on the frequency of aggressive incidents on the treatment ward. The results showed that compared to the control group, SCIT participants improved on all of the social cognitive measures and showed better self-reported social relationships and fewer aggressive incidents on the treatment unit at post-test. Importantly, this change was independent of changes in clinical symptoms over time and supports the unique role of SCIT in improving social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia.


Subject(s)
Interpersonal Relations , Schizophrenia/rehabilitation , Social Perception , Teaching/methods , Adaptation, Psychological , Adult , Affect , Aggression/psychology , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Cognition Disorders/epidemiology , Facial Expression , Female , Hospitalization , Humans , Length of Stay , Male , Psychology , Schizophrenia/epidemiology , Surveys and Questionnaires , Trail Making Test
17.
Psychiatr Serv ; 58(4): 449-51, 2007 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17412842

ABSTRACT

This column describes the development of a treatment, the Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) program, a group-based intervention delivered weekly over a six-month period, with the purpose of improving both social cognition and social functioning among persons with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. SCIT comprises three phases: emotion training, figuring out situations, and integration. Initial pilot testing of 17 inpatients showed that SCIT was associated with improved emotion perception, improved theory of mind, and a reduced tendency to attribute hostile intent to others, with effect sizes being in the medium-large range. Although research is still in the early phases, SCIT is a potential best practice.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/rehabilitation , Cognitive Behavioral Therapy , Interpersonal Relations , Psychotherapy, Group , Schizophrenia/rehabilitation , Schizophrenic Psychology , Social Behavior Disorders/rehabilitation , Adult , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Cognition Disorders/psychology , Cohort Studies , Emotions , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Internal-External Control , Male , Personal Construct Theory , Pilot Projects , Schizophrenia/diagnosis , Social Adjustment , Social Behavior Disorders/diagnosis , Social Behavior Disorders/psychology , Treatment Outcome , United States
18.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 39(1): 46-57, 2017 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27430343

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Cognitive impairment often occurs in people with multiple sclerosis (MS), and dysfunction involving executive function, new learning, and working memory is especially common. Compromised activities of daily living are linked to this cognitive impairment, and people with MS are apt to be unemployed and struggle to manage domestic responsibilities. Financial decision making is an important activity of daily living, and no study has examined whether it is compromised by neuropsychological dysfunction in people with MS. METHOD: A battery of neuropsychological tests and a measure of financial decision making (Financial Capacity Instrument, FCI: Marson, D. C. 2001. Loss of financial capacity in dementia: Conceptual and empirical approaches. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 8, 164-181) were administered to 50 participants (34 patients with MS and 16 cognitively healthy adults). Based on the neuropsychological test results, 14 patients were classified as having cognitive impairment, and 20 had no significant impairment. RESULTS: The impaired MS patients performed significantly worse than unimpaired patients and the healthy comparison group on most financial tasks. The impaired group retained abilities to count money and display adequate financial judgment. Regression analyses showed that measures of mental flexibility and working memory correlated most strongly with performance on the FCI domains across groups. CONCLUSIONS: Cognitively impaired patients with MS have degraded financial skills, which are linked to executive function and working memory deficits.


Subject(s)
Activities of Daily Living , Cognitive Dysfunction/psychology , Decision Making , Judgment , Multiple Sclerosis/psychology , Adult , Cognitive Dysfunction/complications , Executive Function/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Multiple Sclerosis/complications , Neuropsychological Tests
19.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 31(8): 1412-1431, 2017 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28415909

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Performance validity tests (PVTs) and symptom validity tests (SVTs) are often administered during neuropsychological evaluations. Examinees may be coached to avoid detection by measures of response validity. Relatively little research has evaluated whether graduated levels of coaching has differential effects upon PVT and SVT performance. Accordingly, the present experiment evaluated the effect of graduated levels of coaching upon the classification accuracy of commonly used PVTs and SVTs and the currently accepted criterion of failing two or more PVTs or SVTs. METHOD: Participants simulated symptoms associated with mild traumatic brain injury (TBI). One group was provided superficial information concerning cognitive, emotional, and physical symptoms. Another group was provided detailed information about such symptoms. A third group was provided detailed information about symptoms and guidance how to evade detection by PVTs. These groups were compared to an honest-responding group. Extending prior experiments, stand-alone and embedded PVT measures were administered in addition to SVTs. RESULTS: The three simulator groups were readily identified by PVTs and SVTs, but a meaningful minority of those provided test-taking strategies eluded detection. The Word Memory Test emerged as the most sensitive indicator of simulated mild TBI symptoms. PVTs achieved more sensitive detection of simulated head injury status than SVTs. CONCLUSIONS: Individuals coached to modify test-taking performance were marginally more successful in eluding detection by PVTs and SVTs than those coached with respect to TBI symptoms only. When the criterion of failing two or more PVTs or SVTs was applied, only 5% eluded detection.


Subject(s)
Brain Concussion/complications , Brain Injuries/complications , Adult , Brain Concussion/psychology , Brain Injuries/psychology , Female , Healthy Volunteers , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Young Adult
20.
Schizophr Res ; 86(1-3): 80-8, 2006 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16884894

ABSTRACT

This study reports on the development of a new measure of delusional belief conviction, the Conviction of Delusional Beliefs Scale (CDBS). Most of the current scales in use assess belief conviction with a single item and primarily reflect the cognitive aspects of conviction. The CDBS represents an improvement over existing scales in that it contains a larger number of test items that can be subjected to psychometric examination. In addition, the CDBS also broadens the concept of belief conviction by incorporating cognitive, emotional, and behavioral items. In the present study, fifty participants with delusions completed the CDBS along with measures of delusional ideation, psychiatric symptomatology, insight, and reading ability. The CDBS showed very good levels of internal consistency and test-retest stability over a six-week period. All of the CDBS items loaded highly on a unitary factor of belief conviction. The CDBS positively correlated with four measures of belief conviction thereby reflecting the convergent validity of the scale. The CDBS was unrelated to other dimensions of delusional ideation, psychiatric symptomatology, insight, and reading ability, which supported the discriminant validity of the scale. The CDBS appears to be a reliable and valid measure of delusional belief conviction that could be used in clinical and research settings.


Subject(s)
Culture , Delusions/diagnosis , Delusions/psychology , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales/statistics & numerical data , Psychometrics/statistics & numerical data , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychometrics/methods , Reproducibility of Results
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL