Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 68
Filter
Add more filters

Country/Region as subject
Publication year range
1.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(4)2024 Apr 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38566509

ABSTRACT

Mixed feelings, the simultaneous presence of feelings with positive and negative valence, remain an understudied topic. They pose a specific set of challenges due to individual variation, and their investigation requires analtyic approaches focusing on individually self-reported states. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to scan 27 subjects watching an animated short film chosen to induce bittersweet mixed feelings. The same subjects labeled when they had experienced positive, negative, and mixed feelings. Using hidden-Markov models, we found that various brain regions could predict the onsets of new feeling states as determined by self-report. The ability of the models to identify these transitions suggests that these states may exhibit unique and consistent neural signatures. We next used the subjects' self-reports to evaluate the spatiotemporal consistency of neural patterns for positive, negative, and mixed states. The insula had unique and consistent neural signatures for univalent states, but not for mixed valence states. The anterior cingulate and ventral medial prefrontal cortex had consistent neural signatures for both univalent and mixed states. This study is the first to demonstrate that subjectively reported changes in feelings induced by naturalistic stimuli can be predicted from fMRI and the first to show direct evidence for a neurally consistent representation of mixed feelings.


Subject(s)
Affect , Brain , Humans , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Emotions , Brain Mapping/methods , Prefrontal Cortex , Magnetic Resonance Imaging
2.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 36(8): 1653-1659, 2024 Jul 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38319678

ABSTRACT

In this article, we summarize our views on the problem of consciousness and outline the current version of a novel hypothesis for how conscious minds can be generated in mammalian organisms. We propose that a mind can be considered conscious when three processes are in place: the first is a continuous generation of interoceptive feelings, which results in experiencing of the organism's internal operations; the second is the equally continuous production of images, generated according to the organism's sensory perspective relative to its surround; the third combines feeling/experience and perspective resulting in a process of subjectivity relative to the image contents. We also propose a biological basis for these three components: the peripheral and central physiology of interoception and exteroception help explain the implementation of the first two components, whereas the third depends on central nervous system integration, at multiple levels, from spinal cord, brainstem, and diencephalic nuclei, to selected regions of the mesial cerebral cortices.


Subject(s)
Consciousness , Emotions , Homeostasis , Consciousness/physiology , Humans , Emotions/physiology , Homeostasis/physiology , Animals , Interoception/physiology , Brain/physiology
3.
Neural Comput ; 35(3): 277-286, 2023 02 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35896152

ABSTRACT

In this view, we address the problem of consciousness, and although we focus on its human presentation, we note that the phenomenon is present in numerous nonhuman species and use findings from a variety of animal studies to explain our hypothesis for how consciousness is made. Consciousness occurs when mind contents, such as perceptions and thoughts, are spontaneously identified as belonging to a specific organism/owner. Conscious minds are said to have a self that experiences mental events. We hypothesize that the automatic identification that associates minds and organisms is provided by a continuous flow of homeostatic feelings. Those feelings arise from the uninterrupted process of life regulation and correspond to both salient physiological fluctuations such as hunger, pain, well-being, or malaise, as well as to states closer to metabolic equilibrium and best described as feelings of life/existence, such as breathing or body temperature. We also hypothesize that homeostatic feelings were the inaugural phenomena of consciousness in biological evolution and venture that they were selected because the information they provided regarding the current state of life regulation conferred extraordinary advantages to the organisms so endowed. The "knowledge" carried by conscious homeostatic feelings provided "overt" guidance for life regulation, an advance over the covert regulation present in nonconscious organisms. Finally, we outline a mechanism for the generation of feelings based on a two-way interaction between interoceptive components of the nervous system and a particular set of nonneural components of the organism's interior, namely, viscera and circulating chemical molecules involved in their operations. Feelings emerge from this interaction as continuous and hybrid phenomena, related simultaneously to two series of events. The first is best described by the terms neural/representational/and mental and the second by the terms nonneural/visceral/and chemical. We note that this account offers a solution for the mind-body problem: homeostatic feelings constitute the "mental" version of bodily processes.


Subject(s)
Biological Evolution , Consciousness , Animals , Humans , Consciousness/physiology
4.
Bioessays ; 43(6): e2000261, 2021 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33763881

ABSTRACT

Feelings are conscious mental events that represent body states as they undergo homeostatic regulation. Feelings depend on the interoceptive nervous system (INS), a collection of peripheral and central pathways, nuclei and cortical regions which continuously sense chemical and anatomical changes in the organism. How such humoral and neural signals come to generate conscious mental states has been a major scientific question. The answer proposed here invokes (1) several distinctive and poorly known physiological features of the INS; and (2) a unique interaction between the body (the 'object' of interoception) and the central nervous system (which generates the 'subject' of interoception). The atypical traits of the INS and the direct interactions between neural and non-neural physiological compartments of the organism, neither of which is present in exteroceptive systems, plausibly explain the qualitative and subjective aspects of feelings, thus accounting for their conscious nature.


Subject(s)
Interoception , Emotions , Homeostasis
5.
Neuroimage ; 218: 116512, 2020 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31901418

ABSTRACT

Psychological theories of emotion often highlight the dynamic quality of the affective experience, yet neuroimaging studies of affect have traditionally relied on static stimuli that lack ecological validity. Consequently, the brain regions that represent emotions and feelings as they unfold remain unclear. Recently, dynamic, model-free analytical techniques have been employed with naturalistic stimuli to better capture time-varying patterns of activity in the brain; yet, few studies have focused on relating these patterns to changes in subjective feelings. Here, we address this gap, using intersubject correlation and phase synchronization to assess how stimulus-driven changes in brain activity and connectivity are related to two aspects of emotional experience: emotional intensity and enjoyment. During fMRI scanning, healthy volunteers listened to a full-length piece of music selected to induce sadness. After scanning, participants listened to the piece twice while simultaneously rating the intensity of felt sadness or felt enjoyment. Activity in the auditory cortex, insula, and inferior frontal gyrus was significantly synchronized across participants. Synchronization in auditory, visual, and prefrontal regions was significantly greater in participants with higher measures of a subscale of trait empathy related to feeling emotions in response to music. When assessed dynamically, continuous enjoyment ratings positively predicted a moment-to-moment measure of intersubject synchronization in auditory, default mode, and striatal networks, as well as the orbitofrontal cortex, whereas sadness predicted intersubject synchronization in limbic and striatal networks. The results suggest that stimulus-driven patterns of neural communication in emotional processing and high-level cortical regions carry meaningful information with regards to our feeling in response to a naturalistic stimulus.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Music/psychology , Adult , Auditory Perception/physiology , Brain Mapping/methods , Empathy/physiology , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Young Adult
6.
Cereb Cortex ; 28(12): 4336-4347, 2018 12 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29126181

ABSTRACT

Several studies comparing adult musicians and nonmusicians have shown that music training is associated with structural brain differences. It is not been established, however, whether such differences result from pre-existing biological traits, lengthy musical training, or an interaction of the two factors, or if comparable changes can be found in children undergoing music training. As part of an ongoing longitudinal study, we investigated the effects of music training on the developmental trajectory of children's brain structure, over two years, beginning at age 6. We compared these children with children of the same socio-economic background but either involved in sports training or not involved in any systematic after school training. We established at the onset that there were no pre-existing structural differences among the groups. Two years later we observed that children in the music group showed (1) a different rate of cortical thickness maturation between the right and left posterior superior temporal gyrus, and (2) higher fractional anisotropy in the corpus callosum, specifically in the crossing pathways connecting superior frontal, sensory, and motor segments. We conclude that music training induces macro and microstructural brain changes in school-age children, and that those changes are not attributable to pre-existing biological traits.


Subject(s)
Brain/growth & development , Music , Practice, Psychological , Acoustic Stimulation , Brain Mapping , Child , Diffusion Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male
7.
Neuroimage ; 174: 1-10, 2018 07 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29501874

ABSTRACT

Effective social functioning relies in part on the ability to identify emotions from auditory stimuli and respond appropriately. Previous studies have uncovered brain regions engaged by the affective information conveyed by sound. But some of the acoustical properties of sounds that express certain emotions vary remarkably with the instrument used to produce them, for example the human voice or a violin. Do these brain regions respond in the same way to different emotions regardless of the sound source? To address this question, we had participants (N = 38, 20 females) listen to brief audio excerpts produced by the violin, clarinet, and human voice, each conveying one of three target emotions-happiness, sadness, and fear-while brain activity was measured with fMRI. We used multivoxel pattern analysis to test whether emotion-specific neural responses to the voice could predict emotion-specific neural responses to musical instruments and vice-versa. A whole-brain searchlight analysis revealed that patterns of activity within the primary and secondary auditory cortex, posterior insula, and parietal operculum were predictive of the affective content of sound both within and across instruments. Furthermore, classification accuracy within the anterior insula was correlated with behavioral measures of empathy. The findings suggest that these brain regions carry emotion-specific patterns that generalize across sounds with different acoustical properties. Also, individuals with greater empathic ability have more distinct neural patterns related to perceiving emotions. These results extend previous knowledge regarding how the human brain extracts emotional meaning from auditory stimuli and enables us to understand and connect with others effectively.


Subject(s)
Auditory Cortex/physiology , Auditory Perception/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adolescent , Adult , Affect/physiology , Brain/physiology , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Young Adult
8.
Nat Rev Neurosci ; 14(2): 143-52, 2013 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23329161

ABSTRACT

Feelings are mental experiences of body states. They signify physiological need (for example, hunger), tissue injury (for example, pain), optimal function (for example, well-being), threats to the organism (for example, fear or anger) or specific social interactions (for example, compassion, gratitude or love). Feelings constitute a crucial component of the mechanisms of life regulation, from simple to complex. Their neural substrates can be found at all levels of the nervous system, from individual neurons to subcortical nuclei and cortical regions.


Subject(s)
Emotions/physiology , Nervous System Physiological Phenomena , Animals , Biological Evolution , Drive , Homeostasis , Humans , Neurons/physiology
9.
Brain ; 145(7): 2231-2235, 2022 07 29.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35640272
10.
Cereb Cortex ; 27(2): 1428-1438, 2017 02 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26744541

ABSTRACT

Narratives are an important component of culture and play a central role in transmitting social values. Little is known, however, about how the brain of a listener/reader processes narratives. A receiver's response to narration is influenced by the narrator's framing and appeal to values. Narratives that appeal to "protected values," including core personal, national, or religious values, may be particularly effective at influencing receivers. Protected values resist compromise and are tied with identity, affective value, moral decision-making, and other aspects of social cognition. Here, we investigated the neural mechanisms underlying reactions to protected values in narratives. During fMRI scanning, we presented 78 American, Chinese, and Iranian participants with real-life stories distilled from a corpus of over 20 million weblogs. Reading these stories engaged the posterior medial, medial prefrontal, and temporo-parietal cortices. When participants believed that the protagonist was appealing to a protected value, signal in these regions was increased compared with when no protected value was perceived, possibly reflecting the intensive and iterative search required to process this material. The effect strength also varied across groups, potentially reflecting cultural differences in the degree of concern for protected values.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Decision Making/physiology , Morals , Narration , Social Identification , Adult , China , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Female , Humans , Iran , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , United States , Young Adult
11.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 38(12): 6096-6106, 2017 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28940969

ABSTRACT

Drawing from a common lexicon of semantic units, humans fashion narratives whose meaning transcends that of their individual utterances. However, while brain regions that represent lower-level semantic units, such as words and sentences, have been identified, questions remain about the neural representation of narrative comprehension, which involves inferring cumulative meaning. To address these questions, we exposed English, Mandarin, and Farsi native speakers to native language translations of the same stories during fMRI scanning. Using a new technique in natural language processing, we calculated the distributed representations of these stories (capturing the meaning of the stories in high-dimensional semantic space), and demonstrate that using these representations we can identify the specific story a participant was reading from the neural data. Notably, this was possible even when the distributed representations were calculated using stories in a different language than the participant was reading. Our results reveal that identification relied on a collection of brain regions most prominently located in the default mode network. These results demonstrate that neuro-semantic encoding of narratives happens at levels higher than individual semantic units and that this encoding is systematic across both individuals and languages. Hum Brain Mapp 38:6096-6106, 2017. © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Comprehension/physiology , Multilingualism , Narration , Reading , Semantics , Adult , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain Mapping , Culture , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Psycholinguistics , Translating , Young Adult
12.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 36(9): 3629-40, 2015 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26047030

ABSTRACT

We continuously perceive objects in the world through multiple sensory channels. In this study, we investigated the convergence of information from different sensory streams within the cerebral cortex. We presented volunteers with three common objects via three different modalities-sight, sound, and touch-and used multivariate pattern analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data to map the cortical regions containing information about the identity of the objects. We could reliably predict which of the three stimuli a subject had seen, heard, or touched from the pattern of neural activity in the corresponding early sensory cortices. Intramodal classification was also successful in large portions of the cerebral cortex beyond the primary areas, with multiple regions showing convergence of information from two or all three modalities. Using crossmodal classification, we also searched for brain regions that would represent objects in a similar fashion across different modalities of presentation. We trained a classifier to distinguish objects presented in one modality and then tested it on the same objects presented in a different modality. We detected audiovisual invariance in the right temporo-occipital junction, audiotactile invariance in the left postcentral gyrus and parietal operculum, and visuotactile invariance in the right postcentral and supramarginal gyri. Our maps of multisensory convergence and crossmodal generalization reveal the underlying organization of the association cortices, and may be related to the neural basis for mental concepts.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Brain/physiology , Touch Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Brain Mapping/methods , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Physical Stimulation , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted
13.
Cereb Cortex ; 23(4): 833-46, 2013 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22473895

ABSTRACT

It has been convincingly established, over the past decade, that the human insular cortices are involved in processing both body feelings (such as pain) and feelings of emotion. Recently, however, an interpretation of this finding has emerged suggesting that the insular cortices are the necessary and sufficient platform for human feelings, in effect, the sole neural source of feeling experiences. In this study, we investigate this proposal in a patient whose insular cortices were destroyed bilaterally as a result of Herpes simplex encephalitis. The fact that all aspects of feeling were intact indicates that the proposal is problematic. The signals used to assemble the neural substrates of feelings hail from different sectors of the body and are conveyed by neural and humoral pathways to complex and topographically organized nuclei of the brain stem, prior to being conveyed again to cerebral cortices in the somatosensory, insular, and cingulate regions. We suggest that the neural substrate of feeling states is to be found first subcortically and then secondarily repeated at cortical level. The subcortical level would ensure basic feeling states while the cortical level would largely relate feeling states to cognitive processes such as decision-making and imagination.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/pathology , Emotions/physiology , Encephalitis, Herpes Simplex/pathology , Sensation/physiology , Aged, 80 and over , Cerebral Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Cerebral Cortex/virology , Encephalitis, Herpes Simplex/psychology , Food Preferences/psychology , Humans , Imaging, Three-Dimensional , Longitudinal Studies , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychological Tests , Recognition, Psychology , Spouses/psychology , Tomography, X-Ray Computed
14.
J Neurosci ; 32(47): 16629-36, 2012 Nov 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23175818

ABSTRACT

People can identify objects in the environment with remarkable accuracy, regardless of the sensory modality they use to perceive them. This suggests that information from different sensory channels converges somewhere in the brain to form modality-invariant representations, i.e., representations that reflect an object independently of the modality through which it has been apprehended. In this functional magnetic resonance imaging study of human subjects, we first identified brain areas that responded to both visual and auditory stimuli and then used crossmodal multivariate pattern analysis to evaluate the neural representations in these regions for content specificity (i.e., do different objects evoke different representations?) and modality invariance (i.e., do the sight and the sound of the same object evoke a similar representation?). While several areas became activated in response to both auditory and visual stimulation, only the neural patterns recorded in a region around the posterior part of the superior temporal sulcus displayed both content specificity and modality invariance. This region thus appears to play an important role in our ability to recognize objects in our surroundings through multiple sensory channels and to process them at a supramodal (i.e., conceptual) level.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Analysis of Variance , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Imagination/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Pattern Recognition, Automated , Photic Stimulation
15.
Nature ; 446(7138): 908-11, 2007 Apr 19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17377536

ABSTRACT

The psychological and neurobiological processes underlying moral judgement have been the focus of many recent empirical studies. Of central interest is whether emotions play a causal role in moral judgement, and, in parallel, how emotion-related areas of the brain contribute to moral judgement. Here we show that six patients with focal bilateral damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPC), a brain region necessary for the normal generation of emotions and, in particular, social emotions, produce an abnormally 'utilitarian' pattern of judgements on moral dilemmas that pit compelling considerations of aggregate welfare against highly emotionally aversive behaviours (for example, having to sacrifice one person's life to save a number of other lives). In contrast, the VMPC patients' judgements were normal in other classes of moral dilemmas. These findings indicate that, for a selective set of moral dilemmas, the VMPC is critical for normal judgements of right and wrong. The findings support a necessary role for emotion in the generation of those judgements.


Subject(s)
Emotions/physiology , Judgment/physiology , Morals , Prefrontal Cortex/injuries , Prefrontal Cortex/physiopathology , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Decision Making/physiology , Empathy , Female , Guilt , Humans , Male , Models, Neurological , Models, Psychological , Prefrontal Cortex/pathology , Shame
16.
Neuron ; 111(21): 3337-3340, 2023 11 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37918346

ABSTRACT

Hanna and Antonio Damasio work at the intersection between neuroscience, neurology, philosophy, and psychology. They discuss the value of single case studies for neuroscience, consciousness research and the limits of AI, and the fascinating relationship between creativity and the brain.


Subject(s)
Neurology , Neurosciences , Consciousness , Brain , Philosophy
17.
Sci Robot ; 8(80): eabq3658, 2023 07 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37436969

ABSTRACT

Given the accelerating powers of artificial intelligence (AI), we must equip artificial agents and robots with empathy to prevent harmful and irreversible decisions. Current approaches to artificial empathy focus on its cognitive or performative processes, overlooking affect, and thus promote sociopathic behaviors. Artificially vulnerable, fully empathic AI is necessary to prevent sociopathic robots and protect human welfare.


Subject(s)
Artificial Intelligence , Robotics , Humans , Empathy
18.
Cereb Cortex ; 21(9): 2113-21, 2011 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21330469

ABSTRACT

There is increasing evidence to suggest that primary sensory cortices can become active in the absence of external stimulation in their respective modalities. This occurs, for example, when stimuli processed via one sensory modality imply features characteristic of a different modality; for instance, visual stimuli that imply touch have been observed to activate the primary somatosensory cortex (SI). In the present study, we addressed the question of whether such cross-modal activations are content specific. To this end, we investigated neural activity in the primary somatosensory cortex of subjects who observed human hands engaged in the haptic exploration of different everyday objects. Using multivariate pattern analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data, we were able to predict, based exclusively on the activity pattern in SI, which of several objects a subject saw being explored. Along with previous studies that found similar evidence for other modalities, our results suggest that primary sensory cortices represent information relevant for their modality even when this information enters the brain via a different sensory system.


Subject(s)
Somatosensory Cortex/physiology , Touch/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Motion Pictures , Photic Stimulation , Proprioception/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology
19.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 106(19): 8021-6, 2009 May 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19414310

ABSTRACT

In an fMRI experiment, participants were exposed to narratives based on true stories designed to evoke admiration and compassion in 4 distinct categories: admiration for virtue (AV), admiration for skill (AS), compassion for social/psychological pain (CSP), and compassion for physical pain (CPP). The goal was to test hypotheses about recruitment of homeostatic, somatosensory, and consciousness-related neural systems during the processing of pain-related (compassion) and non-pain-related (admiration) social emotions along 2 dimensions: emotions about other peoples' social/psychological conditions (AV, CSP) and emotions about others' physical conditions (AS, CPP). Consistent with theoretical accounts, the experience of all 4 emotions engaged brain regions involved in interoceptive representation and homeostatic regulation, including anterior insula, anterior cingulate, hypothalamus, and mesencephalon. However, the study also revealed a previously undescribed pattern within the posteromedial cortices (the ensemble of precuneus, posterior cingulate cortex, and retrosplenial region), an intriguing territory currently known for its involvement in the default mode of brain operation and in self-related/consciousness processes: emotions pertaining to social/psychological and physical situations engaged different networks aligned, respectively, with interoceptive and exteroceptive neural systems. Finally, within the anterior insula, activity correlated with AV and CSP peaked later and was more sustained than that associated with CPP. Our findings contribute insights on the functions of the posteromedial cortices and on the recruitment of the anterior insula in social emotions concerned with physical versus psychological pain.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping/methods , Emotions/physiology , Empathy , Neurons/metabolism , Adult , Behavior , Brain/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Middle Aged , Neural Pathways/physiology
20.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 17(12): 1082-1090, 2022 12 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35579186

ABSTRACT

Recent work using multivariate-pattern analysis (MVPA) on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data has found that distinct affective states produce correspondingly distinct patterns of neural activity in the cerebral cortex. However, it is unclear whether individual differences in the distinctiveness of neural patterns evoked by affective stimuli underlie empathic abilities such as perspective-taking (PT). Accordingly, we examined whether we could predict PT tendency from the classification of blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) fMRI activation patterns while participants (n = 57) imagined themselves in affectively charged scenarios. We used an MVPA searchlight analysis to map where in the brain activity patterns permitted the classification of four affective states: happiness, sadness, fear and disgust. Classification accuracy was significantly above chance levels in most of the prefrontal cortex and in the posterior medial cortices. Furthermore, participants' self-reported PT was positively associated with classification accuracy in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and insula. This finding has implications for understanding affective processing in the prefrontal cortex and for interpreting the cognitive significance of classifiable affective brain states. Our multivariate approach suggests that PT ability may rely on the grain of internally simulated affective representations rather than simply the global strength.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Emotions , Humans , Brain Mapping/methods , Emotions/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Brain/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL