Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 33
Filter
1.
Behav Brain Sci ; 45: e135, 2022 07 25.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35875969

ABSTRACT

Evidence that women voluntarily expose themselves to some threats more than men do challenges the generalizability of the claim that women exceed men in self-protective responses. Examples include women's higher rates of living organ donation and rescuing Jews during the holocaust. In general, women's efforts to keep other people alive can take precedence over their efforts to protect themselves.


Subject(s)
Holocaust , Female , Humans , Male
2.
Child Dev ; 89(6): 1943-1955, 2018 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29557555

ABSTRACT

This meta-analysis, spanning 5 decades of Draw-A-Scientist studies, examined U.S. children's gender-science stereotypes linking science with men. These stereotypes should have weakened over time because women's representation in science has risen substantially in the United States, and mass media increasingly depict female scientists. Based on 78 studies (N = 20,860; grades K-12), children's drawings of scientists depicted female scientists more often in later decades, but less often among older children. Children's depictions of scientists therefore have become more gender diverse over time, but children still associate science with men as they grow older. These results may reflect that children observe more male than female scientists in their environments, even though women's representation in science has increased over time.


Subject(s)
Psychology, Child , Science , Stereotyping , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Mass Media , Sexism , United States
4.
Behav Brain Sci ; 40: e31, 2017 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28327236

ABSTRACT

This commentary makes three points: (1) the existing evidence does not consistently favor the proposed sex difference in attractiveness preferences, nor the fitness-related outcomes of attractiveness; (2) the neglected association of perceived attractiveness and trustworthiness allowed the authors to incorrectly attribute many findings solely to attractiveness, and (3) the importance accorded attractiveness in mate preferences is culturally shaped and likely evolutionarily novel.


Subject(s)
Motivation , Psychology, Social , Bias , Female , Humans , Interdisciplinary Studies , Male , Sexual Behavior
5.
Behav Brain Sci ; 38: e141, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26786762

ABSTRACT

Duarte et al.'s arguments for increasing political diversity in social psychology are based on mischaracterizations of social psychology as fundamentally flawed in understanding stereotype accuracy and the effects of attitudes on information processing. I correct their misunderstandings while agreeing with their view that political diversity, along with other forms of diversity, stands to benefit social psychology.


Subject(s)
Goals , Psychology, Social , Attitude , Humans , Politics , Psychology
6.
Behav Brain Sci ; 35(6): 431-2, 2012 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23164355

ABSTRACT

The analysis offered by Dixon et al. fails to acknowledge that the attitudes that drive prejudice are attitudes that are constructed in particular contexts. These attitudes (e.g., toward men as childcare workers) can diverge strongly from attitudes toward the group in general. Social change is thus best achieved through challenging the requirements of roles and by changing group stereotypes.


Subject(s)
Group Processes , Interpersonal Relations , Prejudice , Social Identification , Humans
7.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; 17(5): 1339-1358, 2022 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35532752

ABSTRACT

Whether women and men are psychologically very similar or quite different is a contentious issue in psychological science. This article clarifies this issue by demonstrating that larger and smaller sex/gender differences can reflect differing ways of organizing the same data. For single psychological constructs, larger differences emerge from averaging multiple indicators that differ by sex/gender to produce scales of a construct's overall typicality for women versus men. For example, averaging self-ratings on personality traits more typical of women or men yields much larger sex/gender differences on measures of the femininity and masculinity of personality. Sex/gender differences on such broad-gauge, thematic variables are large relative to differences on their component indicators. This increased effect magnitude for aggregated scales reflects gains in both their reliability and validity as indicators of sex/gender. In addition, in psychological domains such as vocational interests that are composed of many variables, at least some of which differ by sex/gender, the multivariate distance between women and men is typically larger than the differences on the component variables. These analyses encourage recognition of the interdependence of sex/gender similarity and difference in psychological data.


Subject(s)
Masculinity , Personality , Female , Gender Identity , Humans , Male , Reproducibility of Results
8.
Am Psychol ; 75(3): 301-315, 2020 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31318237

ABSTRACT

This meta-analysis integrated 16 nationally representative U.S. public opinion polls on gender stereotypes (N = 30,093 adults), extending from 1946 to 2018, a span of seven decades that brought considerable change in gender relations, especially in women's roles. In polls inquiring about communion (e.g., affectionate, emotional), agency (e.g., ambitious, courageous), and competence (e.g., intelligent, creative), respondents indicated whether each trait is more true of women or men, or equally true of both. Women's relative advantage in communion increased over time, but men's relative advantage in agency showed no change. Belief in competence equality increased over time, along with belief in female superiority among those who indicated a sex difference in competence. Contemporary gender stereotypes thus convey substantial female advantage in communion and a smaller male advantage in agency but also gender equality in competence along with some female advantage. Interpretation emphasizes the origins of gender stereotypes in the social roles of women and men. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Femininity , Masculinity , Public Opinion , Stereotyping , Femininity/history , History, 20th Century , History, 21st Century , Humans , Masculinity/history , Public Opinion/history , United States
9.
Psychol Bull ; 135(4): 555-88, 2009 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19586162

ABSTRACT

A meta-analysis assessed whether exposure to information is guided by defense or accuracy motives. The studies examined information preferences in relation to attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors in situations that provided choices between congenial information, which supported participants' pre-existing attitudes, beliefs, or behaviors, and uncongenial information, which challenged these tendencies. Analyses indicated a moderate preference for congenial over uncongenial information (d=0.36). As predicted, this congeniality bias was moderated by variables that affect the strength of participants' defense motivation and accuracy motivation. In support of the importance of defense motivation, the congeniality bias was weaker when participants' attitudes, beliefs, or behaviors were supported prior to information selection; when participants' attitudes, beliefs, or behaviors were not relevant to their values or not held with conviction; when the available information was low in quality; when participants' closed-mindedness was low; and when their confidence in the attitude, belief, or behavior was high. In support of the importance of accuracy motivation, an uncongeniality bias emerged when uncongenial information was relevant to accomplishing a current goal.


Subject(s)
Attitude , Cognitive Dissonance , Culture , Defense Mechanisms , Judgment , Motivation , Social Behavior , Social Values , Awareness , Choice Behavior , Health Knowledge, Attitudes, Practice , Health Promotion , Humans , Models, Psychological , Problem Solving , Social Conformity
10.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 35(4): 403-14, 2009 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19164705

ABSTRACT

In two experiments, female and male participants envisioned themselves as a married person with children who is either a homemaker or a provider. Participants who envisioned themselves as a future homemaker regarded a potential mate's provider qualities as more important and homemaker qualities as less important, compared with participants who envisioned themselves as a future provider. Envisioning oneself as a homemaker also shifted preferences toward an older spouse, compared with envisioning oneself as a provider. In the control conditions of the experiments, in which participants freely envisioned their own future marriage, the less provider responsibility anticipated for the wife, the more traditional were mate preferences. These experiments support the social role theory view that the roles anticipated by men and women influence their choice of mates.


Subject(s)
Gender Identity , Marriage , Spouses , Female , Humans , Illinois , Male , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
11.
Harv Bus Rev ; 85(9): 62-71, 146, 2007 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17886484

ABSTRACT

Two decades ago, people began using the "glass ceiling" catchphrase to describe organizations' failure to promote women into top leadership roles. Eagly and Carli, of Northwestern University and Wellesley College, argue in this article (based on a forthcoming book from Harvard Business School Press) that the metaphor has outlived its usefulness. In fact, it leads managers to overlook interventions that would attack the problem at its roots, wherever it occurs. A labyrinth is a more fitting image to help organizations understand and address the obstacles to women's progress. Rather than depicting just one absolute barrier at the penultimate stage of a distinguished career, a labyrinth conveys the complexity and variety of challenges that can appear along the way. Passage through a labyrinth requires persistence, awareness of one's progress, and a careful analysis of the puzzles that lie ahead. Routes to the center exist but are full of twists and turns, both expected and unexpected. Vestiges of prejudice against women, issues of leadership style and authenticity, and family responsibilities are just a few of the challenges. For instance, married mothers now devote even more time to primary child care per week than they did in earlier generations (12.9 hours of close interaction versus 10.6), despite the fact that fathers, too, put in a lot more hours than they used to (6.5 versus 2.6). Pressures for intensive parenting and the increasing demands of most high-level careers have left women with very little time to socialize with colleagues and build professional networks--that is, to accumulate the social capital that is essential to managers who want to move up. The remedies proposed--such as changing the long-hours culture, using open-recruitment tools, and preparing women for line management with appropriately demanding assignments--are wide ranging, but together they have a chance of achieving leadership equity in our time.


Subject(s)
Administrative Personnel , Career Mobility , Leadership , Commerce , Female , Humans , United States
12.
J Appl Psychol ; 102(3): 434-451, 2017 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28125257

ABSTRACT

Although in the early years of the Journal leadership research was rare and focused primarily on traits differentiating leaders from nonleaders, subsequent to World War II the research area developed in 3 major waves of conceptual, empirical, and methodological advances: (a) behavioral and attitude research; (b) behavioral, social-cognitive, and contingency research; and (c) transformational, social exchange, team, and gender-related research. Our review of this work shows dramatic increases in sophistication from early research focusing on personnel issues associated with World War I to contemporary multilevel models and meta-analyses on teams, shared leadership, leader-member exchange, gender, ethical, abusive, charismatic, and transformational leadership. Yet, many of the themes that characterize contemporary leadership research were also present in earlier research. (PsycINFO Database Record


Subject(s)
Leadership , Psychological Theory , Psychology, Applied/methods , Research , History, 20th Century , History, 21st Century , Humans , Psychology, Applied/history , Research/history
13.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; 11(6): 899-904, 2016 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27899733

ABSTRACT

Women are sparsely represented among psychologists honored for scientific eminence. However, most currently eminent psychologists started their careers when far fewer women pursued training in psychological science. Now that women earn the majority of psychology Ph.D.'s, will they predominate in the next generation's cadre of eminent psychologists? Comparing currently active female and male psychology professors on publication metrics such as the h index provides clues for answering this question. Men outperform women on the h index and its two components: scientific productivity and citations of contributions. To interpret these gender gaps, we first evaluate whether publication metrics are affected by gender bias in obtaining grant support, publishing papers, or gaining citations of published papers. We also consider whether women's chances of attaining eminence are compromised by two intertwined sets of influences: (a) gender bias stemming from social norms pertaining to gender and to science and (b) the choices that individual psychologists make in pursuing their careers.


Subject(s)
Achievement , Authorship , Choice Behavior , Gender Identity , Science , Attention , Female , Humans , Psychology
14.
Psychol Bull ; 128(5): 699-727, 2002 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12206191

ABSTRACT

This article evaluates theories of the origins of sex differences in human behavior. It reviews the cross-cultural evidence on the behavior of women and men in nonindustrial societies, especially the activities that contribute to the sex-typed division of labor and patriarchy. To explain the cross-cultural findings, the authors consider social constructionism, evolutionary psychology, and their own biosocial theory. Supporting the biosocial analysis, sex differences derive from the interaction between the physical specialization of the sexes, especially female reproductive capacity, and the economic and social structural aspects of societies. This biosocial approach treats the psychological attributes of women and men as emergent given the evolved characteristics of the sexes, their developmental experiences, and their situated activity in society.


Subject(s)
Culture , Sex Differentiation/genetics , Social Behavior , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Female , Humans , Male , Social Control, Formal
15.
Psychol Bull ; 129(4): 569-91, 2003 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12848221

ABSTRACT

A meta-analysis of 45 studies of transformational, transactional, and laissez-faire leadership styles found that female leaders were more transformational than male leaders and also engaged in more of the contingent reward behaviors that are a component of transactional leadership. Male leaders were generally more likely to manifest the other aspects of transactional leadership (active and passive management by exception) and laissez-faire leadership. Although these differences between male and female leaders were small, the implications of these findings are encouraging for female leadership because other research has established that all of the aspects of leadership style on which women exceeded men relate positively to leaders' effectiveness whereas all of the aspects on which men exceeded women have negative or null relations to effectiveness.


Subject(s)
Administrative Personnel , Gender Identity , Industry , Female , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Motivation , Organizational Objectives , Personnel Management , United States
16.
Psychol Rev ; 109(3): 573-98, 2002 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12088246

ABSTRACT

A role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders proposes that perceived incongruity between the female gender role and leadership roles leads to 2 forms of prejudice: (a) perceiving women less favorably than men as potential occupants of leadership roles and (b) evaluating behavior that fulfills the prescriptions of a leader role less favorably when it is enacted by a woman. One consequence is that attitudes are less positive toward female than male leaders and potential leaders. Other consequences are that it is more difficult for women to become leaders and to achieve success in leadership roles. Evidence from varied research paradigms substantiates that these consequences occur, especially in situations that heighten perceptions of incongruity between the female gender role and leadership roles.


Subject(s)
Conflict, Psychological , Gender Identity , Leadership , Prejudice , Role , Female , Humans , Personnel Management , Psychological Theory
17.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 87(6): 796-816, 2004 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15598107

ABSTRACT

This research examined the proposition that differential role occupancy by women and men fosters gender gaps in sociopolitical attitudes. Analyses of the General Social Survey (J. A. Davis & T. W. Smith, 1998) and a community sample showed that women, more than men, endorsed policies that are socially compassionate, traditionally moral, and supportive of equal rights for women and for gays and lesbians. To clarify the sources of these gaps, the research examined (a) similarities between gender gaps and gaps associated with other respondent attributes such as race and parenthood, (b) interactions between respondent sex and other attributes, (c) the temporal patterning of gender gaps, and (d) the mediation of attitudinal gender gaps by 3 ideological variables--commitment to equality, group-based dominance, and conservatism versus liberalism.


Subject(s)
Attitude , Politics , Psychology, Social/methods , Social Behavior , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Morals , Public Policy , Sex Factors , Surveys and Questionnaires
18.
Am Psychol ; 59(3): 163-78, 2004 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15222859

ABSTRACT

Heroism consists of actions undertaken to help others, despite the possibility that they may result in the helper's death or injury. The authors examine heroism by women and men in 2 extremely dangerous settings: the emergency situations in which Carnegie medalists rescued others and the holocaust in which some non-Jews risked their lives to rescue Jews. The authors also consider 3 risky but less dangerous prosocial actions: living kidney donations, volunteering for the Peace Corps, and volunteering for Doctors of the World. Although the Carnegie medalists were disproportionately men, the other actions yielded representations of women that were at least equal to and in most cases higher than those of men. These findings have important implications for the psychology of heroism and of gender.


Subject(s)
Altruism , Social Behavior , Female , Holocaust , Humans , Male , Risk-Taking , Sex Factors
19.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 29(10): 1245-58, 2003 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15189586

ABSTRACT

This research examined the hypothesis that gender gaps in voting stem from differences in the extent to which men and women agree with candidates' issue stances. Two initial experiments portraying candidates by their sex and attitudes and a third experiment that also included information about political party produced the predicted attitudinal gender-congeniality effect: Participants of each sex reported greater likelihood, compared with participants of the other sex, of voting for the candidate who endorsed positions typically favored more by their own sex than the other sex. In addition, this gender-congeniality effect was present among Republican and independent participants but absent among Democratic participants because Democratic men as well as women favored candidates who advocated the positions typically favored by women. Interpretation invoked the importance of group interest based on gender as an influence on women's voting.


Subject(s)
Attitude , Politics , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Male , Sex Factors , United States
20.
Am Psychol ; 69(7): 685-702, 2014 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25046701

ABSTRACT

Starting in the 1960s, many of the critiques of psychological science offered by feminist psychologists focused on its methods and epistemology. This article evaluates the current state of psychological science in relation to this feminist critique. The analysis relies on sources that include the PsycINFO database, the Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (American Psychological Association, 2010), and popular psychology methods textbooks. After situating the feminist critique within the late-20th-century shift of science from positivism to postpositivism, the inquiry examines feminists' claims of androcentric bias in (a) the underrepresentation of women as researchers and research participants and (b) researchers' practices in comparing women and men and describing their research findings. In most of these matters, psychology manifests considerable change in directions advocated by feminists. However, change is less apparent in relation to some feminists' criticisms of psychology's reliance on laboratory experimentation and quantitative methods. In fact, the analyses documented the rarity in high-citation journals of qualitative research that does not include quantification. Finally, the analysis frames feminist methodological critiques by a consideration of feminist epistemologies that challenge psychology's dominant postpositivism. Scrutiny of methods textbooks and journal content suggests that within psychological science, especially as practiced in the United States, these alternative epistemologies have not yet gained substantial influence.


Subject(s)
Feminism , Postmodernism , Psychology/standards , Feminism/history , History, 20th Century , Humans , Postmodernism/history , Psychology/history
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL