Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 127
Filter
Add more filters

Publication year range
1.
Neuropsychol Rehabil ; : 1-22, 2024 Feb 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38346126

ABSTRACT

Prism adaptation training (PAT) as a treatment for visuospatial neglect (VSN) involves two components: recalibration and realignment. We conducted a randomized controlled trial with PAT protocols requiring different degrees of recalibration and realignment, by using a single or multi-step protocol and varying visibility of the pointing movement. Twenty-five VSN patients received an alertness treatment without prisms, followed by four PAT protocols, encompassing a multi- or single-step procedure with terminal exposure, a single-step procedure with concurrent exposure, and sham PAT, presented in random order. The primary outcome parameter was the mean response time (RT) to left-sided targets in an endogenous variant of the Posner task, and we also measured the sensorimotor aftereffect. The two protocols without visibility of most of the movement trajectory produced significant aftereffects. The single-step protocol without movement visibility resulted in shorter RTs to left-sided targets. Hence, aftereffects depended on the partial invisibility of the movement. Moreover, only allowing VSN patients to recalibrate several times and direct feedback from the pointing errors had a beneficial effect on non-motor leftward visuospatial attention. We provide preliminary evidence that maximizing the conscious experience of movement errors may be an important component for remediating VSN.Trial registration: German Clinical Trials Register identifier: DRKS00025938.

2.
Brain Cogn ; 169: 106000, 2023 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37253302

ABSTRACT

Unilateral brain lesions can lead to impaired contralesional attention and reduced ipsilesional and enhanced contralesional superior colliculus (SC) activity. We aimed to investigate whether modulation of SC activation via monocular eye patching can improve contralesional attention. Twenty left-hemispheric (LH) and 20 right-hemispheric (RH) patients with an acute or subacute middle cerebral artery (MCA) stroke completed an endogenous version of the Posner cueing task twice, while the left or right eye was covered with an eye patch. The LH and RH patients showed significantly slower reactions to contralesional than to ipsilesional stimuli. In addition, the eye patch modulated responses to invalidly but not those to validly cued stimuli. Post hoc analyses could not discriminate whether this effect pertained to a particular target side or eye patch position. However, exploratory analyses indicated that the observed eye patch effect might affect the RH group more than the LH group. As predicted 36 years ago, monocular eye patching modulates visuospatial attention, presumably due to differences in SC activation between the two eye patch conditions. However, this modulation seems too weak and unspecific, and therefore possibly not strong enough to be a treatment option for patients with visuospatial attention impairments.


Subject(s)
Perceptual Disorders , Stroke , Humans , Functional Laterality/physiology , Infarction, Middle Cerebral Artery , Attention/physiology
3.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry ; 28(2): 85-101, 2023 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36472235

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The relation between confabulations and intrusions in patients with Korsakoff's syndrome (KS) and patients with alcohol-related cognitive impairments (ARCI) remains under debate. This study examines (1) differences in the production of confabulations and intrusions between patients with KS and ARCI, (2) whether an altered fairy tale induces more intrusions, and (3) whether different types of intrusions were significantly related to confabulations. METHODS: Twenty-three patients with KS and twenty-two patients with ARCI recalled three different types of stories: a novel story, a fairy tale, and a modified fairy tale. Different types of intrusions were correlated with confabulation measures. RESULTS: Patients with KS produced more intrusions in the modified fairy tale condition than patients with ARCI, but these were unrelated to confabulations. Only unrelated intrusions were related to provoked confabulations. CONCLUSIONS: The results of this study indicate that researchers and clinicians must be aware that in general, intrusions on memory tests should not be interpreted as confabulations. Especially spontaneous confabulations appear to be something completely different from intrusions on any type of story recall. When measuring confabulations it is crucial to use validated instruments.


Subject(s)
Cognitive Dysfunction , Korsakoff Syndrome , Female , Humans , Police , Neuropsychological Tests , Memory Disorders/psychology , Korsakoff Syndrome/psychology , Ethanol
4.
Neuropsychol Rehabil ; 32(1): 131-147, 2022 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32851896

ABSTRACT

MS related fatigue might be related to autonomous nervous system (ANS) dysfunctions or to inflammation related vagal (hyper-) activation. Consequently, influencing ANS status may lead to relieve of fatigue. We used two opposite biofeedback interventions to either increase sympathetic ("self-alert training", SAT) or parasympathetic activation ("progressive muscle relaxation", PMR). We recorded fatigue status of patients before and after a challenging vigilance task, their behavioural performance on this task, their skin conductance response (SCR), and parameters indicating parasympathetic activity concerning heart rate variability (HRV). We repeated these recordings after the biofeedback training sessions. Patients of the SAT group were able to learn to increase their SCR voluntarily. Patients of the PMR group showed increasing parameters indicating parasympathetic modulation of the HRV. The vigilance task increased their feeling of fatigue. However, there was no effect of biofeedback training on either fatigue status or performance on the vigilance task. Our results show that MS patients can learn to change voluntarily their ANS activity using biofeedback instructions based on SCR and this can be used in future studies to test the postulated link between ANS and fatigue. However, in this experimental intervention we were unable to document a relation between ANS activity and fatigue.Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT03268187.


Subject(s)
Biofeedback, Psychology , Fatigue , Fatigue/etiology , Fatigue/therapy , Heart Rate , Humans , Wakefulness
5.
J Hist Behav Sci ; 58(2): 183-203, 2022 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34687562

ABSTRACT

Franz Joseph Gall's (1758-1828) doctrine of many faculties of mind with corresponding cortical organs led him to be accused of materialism, fatalism, and even atheism. Yet little has been written about the specific charges he felt forced to respond to in Vienna, while visiting the German States, or in Paris, where he published his books. This article examines these accusations and Gall's responses. It also looks at what Gall wrote about a cortical faculty for God and religion and seeing intelligent design in the functional organization of the brain. Additionally, it presents what can be gleaned about his private thoughts on God and organized religion. We conclude that Gall was sincere in his admiration for and belief in God the Creator, but that as an enlightened scientist was recognizing the need to separate metaphysics from the laws of nature when presenting his new science of man.


Subject(s)
Phrenology , Brain , Emotions , Faculty , History, 19th Century , Humans , Male , Religion
6.
Brain Cogn ; 151: 105732, 2021 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33895466

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Most neglect treatment studies focus on automatic re-orientation procedures, assuming a deficit in automatic processes. We compare an automatic- and a controlled procedure, using the endogenous and exogenous variants of Posner's covert shift of attention task. METHOD: In two experiments, neglect patients and patients with a right hemispherical stroke without neglect performed three blocks of Posner's covert shift of attention task (Posner Task) on two days. In Study 1 we used endogenous cues, in Study 2, exogenous cues. RESULTS: In the endogenous task, neglect patients improved significantly with valid left-sided cues between block 1 and 2 on Day 1, subsequently showing a plateauing. They also showed a gradual improvement on invalid trials on both days. In the exogenous condition, all participants responded only increasingly faster on trials with a long stimulus onset asynchrony. Practicing on both tasks led to fewer omissions for left-sided targets, minimally in the exogenous and clearly in the endogenous condition. CONCLUSION: In line with prior neuroanatomical studies, our study shows that practicing an endogenous, but not an exogenous, visuospatial attention task leads to significant improvements in neglect patients, especially for invalid trials, suggesting that neglect treatments based on top-down strategies should be given more attention.


Subject(s)
Orientation , Stroke , Cues , Humans , Reaction Time
7.
J Hist Behav Sci ; 56(1): 7-19, 2020 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31498446

ABSTRACT

Franz Joseph Gall (1758-1828) is remembered for his claims that behavior results from a large number of independent mental faculties, and that these faculties are associated with cortical organs. Apart from the 26 faculties he localized in the cerebrum, he also recognized one faculty (reproductive drive) in the cerebellum. This picture, however, is based on Gall's presentations in his well-known later works, his four volume Anatomie et Physiologie. These books reflect the outcomes of Gall's thinking. They were steered by the observations and feedback he received in Vienna and while presenting his theories in the German states and neighboring countries between 1805 and 1807. Examining his lists before what he published in Paris shows how his faculties were changing. Notably, and as shown here, he had previously included several faculties associated with brainstem structures, in addition to the cerebellum, which he would continue to associate with some reproductive behaviors.


Subject(s)
Neuroanatomy/history , Animals , Brain/anatomy & histology , Brain/physiology , Europe , History, 18th Century , History, 19th Century , Humans
8.
Neuropsychol Rehabil ; 28(4): 515-526, 2018 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27380545

ABSTRACT

Visuospatial neglect is a disabling syndrome resulting in impaired activities of daily living and in longer durations of inpatient rehabilitation. Effective interventions to remediate neglect are still needed. The combination of tDCS and an optokinetic task might qualify as a treatment method. A total of 32 post-acute patients with left (n = 20) or right-sided neglect were allotted to an intervention or a control group (both groups n = 16). The intervention group received eight sessions of 1.5-2.0 mA parietal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) during the performance of an optokinetic task distributed over two weeks. Additionally they received standard therapy for five hours per day. The control group received only the standard therapy. Patients were examined twice before (with 3-4 days between examinations) and twice after treatment (5-6 days between examinations). Compared to the control group and controlling for spontaneous remission, the intervention group improved on spontaneous body orientation and the Clock Drawing Test. Intragroup comparisons showed broad improvements on egocentric but not on allocentric symptoms only for the intervention group. A short additional application of tDCS during an optokinetic task led to improvements of severe neglect compared to a standard neurological early rehabilitation treatment. Improvements seem to concern primarily egocentric rather than allocentric neglect.


Subject(s)
Perceptual Disorders/rehabilitation , Psychomotor Performance , Stroke/complications , Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation , Aged , Combined Modality Therapy , Eye Movements , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Treatment Outcome
9.
Mult Scler ; 21(4): 376-81, 2015 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25257615

ABSTRACT

The compensatory approach of fatigue argues that it is a state caused by task load. The neuropsychiatric approach argues that fatigue is a trait (like depression), unrelated to environmental challenges. We propose that fatigue is an internal state that can be measured behaviorally only by applying specific cognitive tasks. PubMed was searched for articles concerning the relation between fatigue and cognitive performance or brain atrophy or functional MRI, distinguishing between the following cognitive domains: learning/memory, cognitive speed/selective attention, language, visuospatial processing, working memory, alerting/vigilance. Only tasks assessing alerting/vigilance are strongly related to fatigue. Areas with brain atrophy in fatigue patients overlap with brain regions activated in healthy controls performing alerting/vigilance tasks. Fatigue is not a compensatory state, nor a psychogenic trait. It is a feeling with behavioral effects that seems to be caused by brain atrophy or a neurochemical dysfunction of the alerting/vigilance system.


Subject(s)
Brain/pathology , Fatigue/physiopathology , Fatigue/psychology , Multiple Sclerosis/complications , Multiple Sclerosis/psychology , Atrophy/pathology , Attention/physiology , Brain/physiopathology , Cognition/physiology , Fatigue/pathology , Humans , Multiple Sclerosis/pathology
10.
Clin Psychol Psychother ; 22(2): 133-41, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24022877

ABSTRACT

Fatigue is a main feature of the burnout syndrome but also very common in other psychiatric disorders such as major depression and anxiety disorders. This raises the question of whether the level and appraisal of fatigue is experienced differently by individuals suffering from burnout than by those exhibiting anxiety disorders and major depression. If fatigue is experienced differently in burnout compared with other disorders, this may clarify why fatigue is the main feature of the burnout syndrome. This knowledge may lead to the application of specific therapeutic interventions aimed at the experience of fatigue in burnout. In the present study, we investigated whether fatigue is experienced differently in burnout patients than in patients suffering from anxiety disorders or major depression. We presented 73 burnout patients, 67 depressed patients, 57 patients with an anxiety disorder and 127 healthy participants with a rating scale containing statements about the fatigue-performance relationship, and we assessed the level of fatigue, depression and anxiety. The level of fatigue reported by burnout patients was high but did not differ from that of the other patient groups. The appraisal of fatigue also did not differ among the patient groups. The burnout patients did not appraise their fatigue as a result of unrewarding activities nor did they catastrophize fatigue in an exceptional way. Thus, the level of fatigue and the appraisal of fatigue may be less relevant to the understanding of the specific pathological processes associated with burnout than is often presumed.


Subject(s)
Burnout, Professional/diagnosis , Burnout, Professional/psychology , Diagnostic Self Evaluation , Fatigue/psychology , Adult , Anxiety Disorders/diagnosis , Anxiety Disorders/psychology , Anxiety Disorders/therapy , Burnout, Professional/therapy , Depressive Disorder, Major/diagnosis , Depressive Disorder, Major/psychology , Depressive Disorder, Major/therapy , Diagnosis, Differential , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Patient Admission , Psychometrics , Risk Factors , Surveys and Questionnaires
11.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 20(3): 333-41, 2014 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24559523

ABSTRACT

This study tested the hypothesis that part of the age-related decline in performance on executive function tasks is due to a decline in episodic memory. For this, we developed a rule induction task in which we manipulated the involvement of episodic memory and executive control processes; age effects and neuropsychological predictors of task performance were investigated. Twenty-six younger (mean age, 24.0; range, 19-35 years) and 27 community-dwelling older adults (mean age, 67.5; range, 50-91 years) participated. The neuropsychological predictors consisted of the performance on tests of episodic memory, working memory, switching, inhibition and flexibility. Performance of the older adults was worse for the learning and memorization of simple rules, as well as for the more demanding executive control condition requiring the manipulation of informational content. Episodic memory was the only predictor of performance on the simple learning and memorization task condition whereas an increase in rule induction complexity additionally engaged working memory processes. Together, these findings indicate that part of the age-related decline on rule induction tests may be the result of a decline in episodic memory. Further studies are needed that examine the role of episodic memory in other executive function tasks in aging.


Subject(s)
Aging , Memory Disorders/physiopathology , Memory, Episodic , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Executive Function/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Predictive Value of Tests , Statistics, Nonparametric , Young Adult
12.
J Neurol Sci ; 461: 123040, 2024 Jun 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38735103

ABSTRACT

Psychological treatments of MS-related fatigue mostly depend on energy conservation programs. We argue that the evidence for energy conservation training is weak - in contrast to some reviews on this topic. The reasons for our concerns are the use of informed passive control groups allowing negative placebo effects, the lack of predefined primary outcome parameter, statistically rather than clinically significant effects, and the use of insensitive fatigue questionnaires. We propose to base psychological interventions not on a view of fatigue as a constant loss of mental energy but as a subjective representation ("feeling") of an inflammatory state, which draws away attentional capacity. This conceptualization allows to develop a three-step treatment approach: Getting short-term control on fatigue, extinction to reduce fatigue-related avoidance behavior, and a systematic increase of activities by pacing. Our proposal depends on the techniques, that can interrupt ongoing feelings of fatigue and can serve as a basis for extinction. We propose that Progressive Muscle Relaxation might be such a technique. The advantage of our model is that it shares similarities with well-established treatments for phobias and chronic pain and we discuss the shared set of assumptions. Hopefully, this will help to improve the treatment of fatigue in future.


Subject(s)
Fatigue , Multiple Sclerosis , Humans , Fatigue/therapy , Fatigue/etiology , Multiple Sclerosis/complications , Multiple Sclerosis/psychology , Multiple Sclerosis/therapy
13.
Mult Scler ; 19(4): 490-7, 2013 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22933623

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: According to the compensation theory, fatigue in MS results from efforts to compensate for a reduction in capacity due to demyelination or neurodegeneration. Recently, it has been argued that fatigue may result from poor sleep. Both explanations predict a worsening of fatigue and a cognitive decline during sustained attention tasks (higher fatigability). METHOD: We compared MS patients with and without self-reported cognitive fatigue, in three sessions with a two-back working memory task, registering hits and response latencies as well as changes in fatigue. In the two breaks between the sessions, either a video instruction to relax or a stimulating video was presented. Subsequently, patients were divided into those with and those without self-reported sleep problems and the analyses were repeated. RESULTS: Patients with fatigue performed worse than healthy controls, irrespective of task duration and type of video during the break. The task-related increase of fatigue also did not differ between the groups and no differential effect of the videos was observed in the MS patients with fatigue. In contrast, patients with sleep problems did show a performance decline as predicted by the compensation theory. CONCLUSION: MS patients with fatigue were impaired in working memory, but did not show greater fatigability, whereas MS participants with self-reported sleep problems showed fatigability, which could be improved with a restorative rest period. Our data therefore do not support the compensation theory of fatigue, and we argue that sleep problems and fatigue in MS patients differ with respect to their functional consequences.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Fatigue/complications , Multiple Sclerosis, Relapsing-Remitting/complications , Sleep Wake Disorders/complications , Fatigue/physiopathology , Humans , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Middle Aged , Multiple Sclerosis, Relapsing-Remitting/physiopathology , Self Report , Sleep Wake Disorders/physiopathology , Surveys and Questionnaires
14.
Brain Cogn ; 81(1): 57-66, 2013 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23174429

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Adaptation to changing situations can be mediated by two strategies: (1) Evaluation of a response and (2) Evaluation of outcome values in relation to objects. Previous studies indicate that response shifting is associated with a network comprising the left frontal cortex and parietal cortex connected by the superior longitudinal fascicle, whereas outcome evaluation is associated with a network consisting of the orbitofrontal cortex, amygdala and uncinate fascicle. However, these studies rarely compared both kinds of adaptation directly and existing fMRI studies with healthy subjects are not informative about the role of the two fiber systems. METHODS: We analyzed stimulus response shifting and stimulus outcome shifting in two studies, one fMRI-study on healthy participants and one study on patients with MS involving structural MRI (Diffusion Tensor Imaging, Voxel Based Morphometry, Ventricular volumetry). Two tasks were used, identical in presentation but different in instruction, controlling for effects of lower level processing. In the SRS task, participants had to perform a "Go" or "NoGo" response depending on a stimulus change: if the stimulus remained the same, they had to continue with the former type of response, if it changed they had to adapt their response pattern. In the SOS task they had to perform a "Go" response only if the presented stimulus corresponded to that of an internal alternating series. RESULTS: fMRI findings showed that SRS is related to a bilateral parietal-premotor network. In the left hemisphere the prefrontal cortex was also involved. SOS was lateralized to the right hemisphere, particularly to the anterior temporal pole and amygdala, and the inferior parietal cortex. MS patients impaired on this task suffered from lesions in the right uncinate fascicle and showed an enlarged right frontal lateral ventricle. CONCLUSION: With physically identical tasks, a functional neuronal segregation can be demonstrated for stimulus response shifting (bilateral activations with a focus in the left prefrontal cortex) and stimulus outcome shifting (right anterior temporal lobe and right supramarginal gyrus).


Subject(s)
Functional Laterality/physiology , Multiple Sclerosis/physiopathology , Nerve Net , Adult , Brain Mapping/methods , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/methods , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Middle Aged , Nerve Net/physiology , Nerve Net/physiopathology , Reaction Time/physiology
15.
Br J Clin Psychol ; 51(4): 376-95, 2012 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23078209

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: The aim of this study was to investigate whether (a) overgeneralization is restricted to negative attributions directed at the self; or whether it also extends to positive self-attributions and to attributions of situations in the outside world, and (b) whether the valence and direction (positively or negatively, to the self- or across situations) of overgeneralization processes vary among different patient populations. METHODS: Patients with major depressive disorder (MDD, n = 34), borderline personality disorder (BPD, n = 18), or both (n = 35), and never-depressed non-patients (NPs; n = 50) completed various measures of overgeneralization. RESULTS: Patients with MDD show higher levels of negative overgeneralization but lower levels of positive overgeneralization to the self- and across situations than NPs. Patients with MDD show more negative than positive overgeneralization to the self: a negative bias. They, however, do show higher levels of positive than negative overgeneralization across situations. Patients with BPD show the same pattern for overgeneralization to the self, but their higher levels of negative overgeneralization across situations are not exceeded by their positive counterpart. CONCLUSIONS: Results indicate that patient groups differ from NPs not only with respect to negative, but also with respect to positive overgeneralization. Furthermore, the valence and direction of overgeneralization processes vary among MDD and BPD patient populations. More specifically, findings suggest that, as compared to never-depressed individuals, patients with BPD and patients with MDD alike, lack a buffer against negative overgeneralization directed at the self. In patients with BPD, not only the high level of overgeneralization to the self, but also the high level of overgeneralization across situations seems to be problematic, since both types of overgeneralization appear not to be buffered by their positive counterparts.


Subject(s)
Borderline Personality Disorder/psychology , Depressive Disorder, Major/psychology , Generalization, Psychological , Adolescent , Adult , Borderline Personality Disorder/physiopathology , Case-Control Studies , Depressive Disorder, Major/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Self Concept , Social Perception
16.
Hist Psychol ; 25(3): 211-244, 2022 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35266784

ABSTRACT

Phrenology is based on correlating character traits with visible or palpable cranial bumps (or depressions) thought to reflect underlying brain areas differing in size and levels of activity. Franz Joseph Gall, who introduced the doctrine during the 1790s, relied heavily on seeing and feeling skulls when he formulated his theory, as did Johann Spurzheim, who served as his assistant until 1813 and then set forth on his own. But Peter Mark Roget, a British critic of the doctrine, first assailed these methods as too subjective in 1818, and never changed his mind. George Combe, a Scotsman who admired Spurzheim, introduced calipers and other measuring instruments during the 1820s, hoping to make phrenology more like the admired physical sciences. In the United States, the Fowlers also called for more numbers, including measuring distances between the cortical sites above the organs of mind. Nonetheless, phrenologists realized they faced formidable barriers when it came to measuring the physical organs of mind, as opposed to basic skull dimensions. This essay examines the subjectivity that left phrenology open to criticism and shows how some phrenologists tried to overcome it. It also shows how vision and touch remained features of phrenological examinations throughout the numbers-obsessed 19th century. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Neurosciences , Phrenology , Brain , Goals , History, 19th Century , Humans , Male , Neurosciences/history , Phrenology/history , Skull , United States
17.
J Hist Neurosci ; 31(4): 524-557, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35412958

ABSTRACT

Whereas some of Gall's critics were quick to assail his organology as materialistic and fatalistic, others questioned his methods and scientific assumptions, especially his craniological tenets. The idea that the skull does not faithfully reflect the features of small, underlying brain areas was repeatedly brought up in the scientific debates. Critics pointed to the frontal sinuses above the eye orbits as evidence for the interior and exterior plates of the cranium not being in parallel-hence, for several or many phrenological organs being unknowable. This article traces the origins of the frontal sinus arguments and how Gall, Spurzheim, and later phrenologists responded to it. It reveals how the two sides fought and remained divided about the significance of the sinuses throughout the nineteenth century-that is, on whether the frontal sinus "problem" was an insurmountable obstacle or one that was merely an inconvenience.


Subject(s)
Frontal Sinus , Phrenology , Brain , Craniology , History, 19th Century , Humans , Skull
18.
Handb Clin Neurol ; 185: 3-24, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35078608

ABSTRACT

This chapter gives a broad overview of the description and theorizing of a wide range of language disorders resulting from brain damage, commonly classified under the umbrella term "aphasia." It covers works written in Antiquity up to the 20th century. Moreover, it looks at disturbances in various language modalities such as speech, language comprehension, reading, writing, and sign language. In addition, also forms of the more recently discovered primary progressive aphasia are discussed. Finally, important developments in the history of assessment and rehabilitation of language disorders are described. To properly characterize disorders of language, these developments are discussed from the perspectives of neurology, psychology, and linguistics.


Subject(s)
Agraphia , Aphasia , Humans , Language , Reading , Speech
20.
Mult Scler ; 17(9): 1088-97, 2011 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21546523

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The human hippocampus plays a role in episodic memory and depression. Recently, it has been shown, using manual tracings, that the hippocampus is smaller in volume in MS patients compared with healthy controls, and that, at least for depression, hippocampal atrophy correlates with symptom severity. METHODS: Because manual tracing of the hippocampus is time consuming, we used a semi-automatic procedure for temporal horn volumetry in 72 multiple sclerosis (MS) patients and 16 control subjects as an indirect measure of hippocampal volume. We analysed memory performance with the California Verbal Learning Test (using separate indices for encoding, consolidation and retrieval) and depressive mood with the Beck's Depression Inventory (distinguishing between psychic and somatic aspects). RESULTS: MS patients had significantly larger temporal horn volumes and volume correlated with psychic symptoms of depressive mood. Temporal horn volume was also associated with consolidation, in particular in the most impaired group. CONCLUSIONS: Temporal horn volume can be measured relatively easily and appears to correlate with two major clinical problems in MS patients: memory performance and depressive mood. The link between temporal horn volume, consolidation and depression may be hippocampal atrophy, as suggested by their adjacent neuroanatomical localization, and by the similarity in functional loss following impairment of these two structures.


Subject(s)
Depression/psychology , Hippocampus/pathology , Memory, Episodic , Multiple Sclerosis, Chronic Progressive/pathology , Multiple Sclerosis, Relapsing-Remitting/pathology , Adult , Female , Hippocampus/physiopathology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Multiple Sclerosis, Chronic Progressive/physiopathology , Multiple Sclerosis, Chronic Progressive/psychology , Multiple Sclerosis, Relapsing-Remitting/physiopathology , Multiple Sclerosis, Relapsing-Remitting/psychology , Neuropsychological Tests , Organ Size
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL