Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 67
Filter
Add more filters

Country/Region as subject
Publication year range
1.
Psychol Sci ; : 9567976241246709, 2024 Jun 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38913829

ABSTRACT

Working memory (WM) is a goal-directed memory system that actively maintains a limited amount of task-relevant information to serve the current goal. By this definition, WM maintenance should be terminated after the goal is accomplished, spontaneously removing no-longer-relevant information from WM. Past studies have failed to provide direct evidence of spontaneous removal of WM content by allowing participants to engage in a strategic reallocation of WM resources to competing information within WM. By contrast, we provide direct neural and behavioral evidence that visual WM content can be largely removed less than 1 s after it becomes obsolete, in the absence of a strategic allocation of resources (total N = 442 adults). These results demonstrate that visual WM is intrinsically a goal-directed system, and spontaneous removal provides a means for capacity-limited WM to keep up with ever-changing demands in a dynamic environment.

2.
Mem Cognit ; 2024 Jul 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38956012

ABSTRACT

Although access to the seemingly infinite capacity of our visual long-term memory (VLTM) can be restricted by visual working memory (VWM) capacity at encoding and retrieval, access can be improved with repeated encoding. This leads to the multiple encoding benefit (MEB), the finding that VLTM performance improves as the number of opportunities to encode the same information increases over time. However, as the number of encoding opportunities increases, so do other factors such as the number of identical encoded VWM representations and chances to engage in successful retrieval during each opportunity. Thus, across two experiments, we disentangled the contributions of each of these factors to the MEB by having participants encode a varying number of identical objects across multiple encoding opportunities. Along with behavioural data, we also examined two established EEG correlates that track the number of maintained VWM representations, namely the posterior alpha suppression and the negative slow wave. Here, we identified that the primary mechanism behind the MEB was the number of encoding opportunities. That is, recognition memory performance was higher following an increase in the number of encoding opportunities, and this could not be attributed solely to an increase in the number of encoded VWM representations or successful retrieval. Our results thus contribute to the understanding of the fundamental mechanisms behind the influence of VWM on VLTM encoding.

3.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 35(4): 603-627, 2023 04 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36626358

ABSTRACT

Despite its unlimited capacity, not all visual information we encounter is encoded into visual long-term memory. Traditionally, variability in encoding success has been ascribed to variability in the types and efficacy of an individual's cognitive processes during encoding. Accordingly, past studies have identified several neural correlates of variability in encoding success, namely, frontal positivity, occipital alpha amplitude, and frontal theta amplitude, by contrasting the electrophysiological signals recorded during successful and failed encoding processes (i.e., subsequent memory). However, recent research demonstrated individuals remember and forget consistent sets of stimuli, thereby elucidating stimulus-intrinsic factors (i.e., memorability) that determine the ease of memory encoding independent of individual-specific variability in encoding processes. The existence of memorability raises the possibility that canonical EEG correlates of subsequent memory may reflect variability in stimulus-intrinsic factors rather than individual-specific encoding processes. To test this, we recorded the EEG correlates of subsequent memory while participants encoded 600 images of real-world objects and assessed the unique contribution of individual-specific and stimulus-intrinsic factors on each EEG correlate. Here, we found that frontal theta amplitude and occipital alpha amplitude were only influenced by individual-specific encoding success, whereas frontal positivity was influenced by stimulus-intrinsic and individual-specific encoding success. Overall, our results offer novel interpretations of canonical EEG correlates of subsequent memory by demonstrating a dissociable impact of stimulus-intrinsic and individual-specific factors of memory encoding success.


Subject(s)
Memory, Long-Term , Mental Recall , Humans , Electroencephalography
4.
Biochem Biophys Res Commun ; 673: 44-50, 2023 09 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37356144

ABSTRACT

Cancer incidence is increasing annually, and the invasion of cancer into the stroma significantly affects cancer metastasis. The stroma primarily comprises an abundant extracellular matrix (ECM) that interacts closely with cancer cells. Cancer cells use the ECM as a scaffold to migrate from a tumor via mechanical actions such as pushing and pulling the fibers. The purpose of this study is to clarify the effects of elastic modulus differences on cell migration behavior based on the same ECM fiber structure. We observe temporal changes in the morphology of cancer cells and the surrounding ECM to elucidate the relationship between changes in the mechanical properties of the ECM and the invasive behavior of cancer cells. We analyze the shape and migration distance of cancer cells and the displacement field of the ECM by varying the fiber elastic modulus but fixing the ECM density. Increasing the elastic modulus results in a protruding cell shape, which indicates the maximum displacement of the ECM around the cell. Additionally, differences in cell migration speed and dispersion based on the elastic modulus are observed. The behavior of cells with increasing elasticity is classified via cluster analysis. Owing to the chemical cross-linking of the fibers, some cells cannot deform the surrounding tissue. This is attributable to the gel state of the ECM and microscopic fluctuations in the fiber density around the cells. We successfully assessed the effect of changes in the ECM modulus on cell mortality and morphology to reveal the mechanism of cancer invasion.


Subject(s)
Extracellular Matrix , Neoplasms , Humans , Elasticity , Elastic Modulus , Cell Movement
5.
Psychol Sci ; 33(5): 816-829, 2022 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35452332

ABSTRACT

Visual information around us is rarely static. To perform a task in such a dynamic environment, we often have to compare current visual input with our working memory (WM) representation of the immediate past. However, little is known about what happens to a WM representation when it is compared with perceptual input. To test this, we asked young adults (N = 170 total in three experiments) to compare a new visual input with a WM representation prior to reporting the WM representation. We found that the perceptual comparison biased the WM report, especially when the input was subjectively similar to the WM representation. Furthermore, using computational modeling and individual-differences analyses, we found that this similarity-induced memory bias was driven by representational integration, rather than incidental confusion, between the WM representation and subjectively similar input. Together, our findings highlight a novel source of WM distortion and suggest a general mechanism that determines how WM interacts with new visual input.


Subject(s)
Memory, Short-Term , Humans , Young Adult
6.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 114(20): 5306-5311, 2017 05 16.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28461479

ABSTRACT

Human memory is thought to consist of long-term storage and short-term storage mechanisms, the latter known as working memory. Although it has long been assumed that information retrieved from long-term memory is represented in working memory, we lack neural evidence for this and need neural measures that allow us to watch this retrieval into working memory unfold with high temporal resolution. Here, we show that human electrophysiology can be used to track information as it is brought back into working memory during retrieval from long-term memory. Specifically, we found that the retrieval of information from long-term memory was limited to just a few simple objects' worth of information at once, and elicited a pattern of neurophysiological activity similar to that observed when people encode new information into working memory. Our findings suggest that working memory is where information is buffered when being retrieved from long-term memory and reconcile current theories of memory retrieval with classic notions about the memory mechanisms involved.


Subject(s)
Memory, Long-Term/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Electroencephalography/methods , Female , Humans , Male , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods
8.
Mem Cognit ; 47(8): 1481-1497, 2019 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31236821

ABSTRACT

We are capable of storing a virtually infinite amount of visual information in visual long-term memory (VLTM) storage. At the same time, the amount of visual information we can encode and maintain in visual short-term memory (VSTM) at a given time is severely limited. How do these two memory systems interact to accumulate vast amount of VLTM? In this series of experiments, we exploited interindividual and intraindividual differences VSTM capacity to examine the direct involvement of VSTM in determining the encoding rate (or "bandwidth") of VLTM. Here, we found that the amount of visual information encoded into VSTM at a given moment (i.e., VSTM capacity), but neither the maintenance duration nor the test process, predicts the effective encoding "bandwidth" of VLTM.


Subject(s)
Memory, Long-Term/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Color Perception/physiology , Humans , Individuality , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Young Adult
9.
Mem Cognit ; 47(2): 351-364, 2019 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30341544

ABSTRACT

Visual long-term memory allows us to store a virtually infinite amount of visual information (Brady, Konkle, Alvarez, & Oliva in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(38), 14325-14329, 2008; Standing in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25(2), 207-222, 1973). However, our ability to encode new visual information fluctuates from moment to moment. In Experiment 1, we tested the hypothesis that we have voluntary control over these periodic fluctuations in our ability to encode representations into visual long-term memory using a precueing paradigm combined with behavioral and electrophysiological indices of memory encoding. We found that visual memory encoding can be up-regulated, but it was much more difficult, if not impossible, to down-regulate encoding on a trial-by-trial basis. In Experiment 2, we tested the hypothesis that voluntary up-regulation of visual memory encoding for an item incurs a cost to memory encoding of other items by manipulating the cueing probability. Here, we found that, although the cueing benefit was constant for both low (20%) and high (50%) cueing probabilities, the benefit in the high cueing probability condition came with the overall impairment of memory encoding. Taken together, our findings demonstrate that top-down control of visual long-term memory encoding may be primarily to prioritize certain memories, but this prioritization has a cost and should not be overused to avoid its negative consequences.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Cues , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Memory, Long-Term/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Adult , Electroencephalography , Humans , Young Adult
10.
J Neurosci ; 35(41): 14009-16, 2015 Oct 14.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26468201

ABSTRACT

Traditionally, electrophysiological correlates of visual working memory (VWM) capacity have been characterized using a lateralized VWM task in which participants had to remember items presented on the cued hemifield while ignoring the distractors presented on the other hemifield. Though this approach revealed a lateralized parieto-occipital negative slow wave (i.e., the contralateral delay activity) and lateralized α power modulation as neural correlates of VWM capacity that may be mechanistically related, recent evidence suggested that these measures might be reflecting individuals' ability to ignore distractors rather than their ability to maintain VWM representations. To better characterize the neural correlates of VWM capacity, we had human participants perform a whole-field VWM task in which they remembered all the items on the display. Here, we found that both the parieto-occipital negative slow wave and the α power suppression showed the characteristics of VWM capacity in the absence of distractors, suggesting that they reflect the maintenance of VWM representations rather than filtering of distractors. Furthermore, the two signals explained unique portions of variance in individual differences of VWM capacity and showed differential temporal characteristics. This pattern of results clearly suggests that individual differences in VWM capacity are determined by dissociable neural mechanisms reflected in the ERP and the oscillatory measures of VWM capacity. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT: Our work demonstrates that there exist event-related potential and oscillatory correlates of visual working memory (VWM) capacity even in the absence of task-irrelevant distractors. This clearly shows that the two neural correlates are directly linked to maintenance of task-relevant information rather than filtering of task-irrelevant information. Furthermore, we found that these two correlates show differential temporal characteristics. These results are inconsistent with proposals that the two neural correlates are byproducts of asymmetric α power suppression and indicate that they reflect dissociable neural mechanisms subserving VWM.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Retention, Psychology , Spectrum Analysis , Students , Time Factors , Universities , Visual Perception/physiology
11.
J Neurophysiol ; 116(4): 1715-1727, 2016 10 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27440249

ABSTRACT

Visual working memory (VWM) allows humans to actively maintain a limited amount of information. Whereas previous electrophysiological studies have found that lateralized event-related potentials (ERPs) track the maintenance of information in VWM, recent imaging experiments have shown that spatially global representations can be read out using the activity across the visual cortex. The goal of the present study was to determine whether both lateralized and spatially global electrophysiological signatures coexist. We first show that it is possible to simultaneously measure lateralized ERPs that track the number of items held in VWM from one visual hemfield and parietooccipital α (8-12 Hz) power over both hemispheres indexing spatially global VWM representations. Next, we replicated our findings and went on to show that this bilateral parietooccipital α power as well as the contralaterally biased ERP correlate of VWM carries a signal that can be used to decode the identity of the representations stored in VWM. Our findings not only unify observations across electrophysiology and imaging techniques but also suggest that ERPs and α-band oscillations index different neural mechanisms that map on to lateralized and spatially global representations, respectively.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Attention/physiology , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation , Regression Analysis
12.
Ann Vasc Surg ; 35: 205.e5-8, 2016 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27241868

ABSTRACT

Endovascular treatment of chronic mesenteric ischemia in patients with impaired renal function or allergy to contrast media is a challenging procedure because angiography requires iodinated contrast media for accurate diagnosis and interventional procedures. Superior mesenteric artery stenting without contrast angiography is described in an 81-year-old woman with cardiovascular disease and chronic kidney disease (CKD) stage 5. Balloon angioplasty and stent placement were performed successfully using intravascular ultrasound, pressure wire, surface ultrasound, and fluoroscopy. Intervention without contrast media has wide applicability to patients with advanced CKD and/or those with allergy to iodinated contrast media.


Subject(s)
Angioplasty, Balloon/instrumentation , Mesenteric Artery, Superior , Mesenteric Ischemia/therapy , Renal Insufficiency, Chronic/complications , Stents , Ultrasonography, Interventional , Aged, 80 and over , Chronic Disease , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Angiography , Mesenteric Artery, Superior/diagnostic imaging , Mesenteric Ischemia/diagnostic imaging , Renal Insufficiency, Chronic/diagnosis , Treatment Outcome
13.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 27(5): 853-65, 2015 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25436671

ABSTRACT

A great deal of prior research has examined the relation between estimates of working memory and cognitive abilities. Yet, the neural mechanisms that account for these relations are still not very well understood. The current study explored whether individual differences in working memory delay activity would be a significant predictor of cognitive abilities. A large number of participants performed multiple measures of capacity, attention control, long-term memory, working memory span, and fluid intelligence, and latent variable analyses were used to examine the data. During two working memory change detection tasks, we acquired EEG data and examined the contralateral delay activity. The results demonstrated that the contralateral delay activity was significantly related to cognitive abilities, and importantly these relations were because of individual differences in both capacity and attention control. These results suggest that individual differences in working memory delay activity predict individual differences in a broad range of cognitive abilities, and this is because of both differences in the number of items that can be maintained and the ability to control access to working memory.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Individuality , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Attention/physiology , Brain/physiology , Color Perception , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Motion Perception , Neuropsychological Tests , Orientation , Photic Stimulation , Random Allocation , Signal Detection, Psychological , Space Perception , Young Adult
14.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 27(8): 1601-16, 2015 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25811710

ABSTRACT

Attentional control and working memory capacity are important cognitive abilities that substantially vary between individuals. Although much is known about how attentional control and working memory capacity relate to each other and to constructs like fluid intelligence, little is known about how trial-by-trial fluctuations in attentional engagement impact trial-by-trial working memory performance. Here, we employ a novel whole-report memory task that allowed us to distinguish between varying levels of attentional engagement in humans performing a working memory task. By characterizing low-performance trials, we can distinguish between models in which working memory performance failures are caused by either (1) complete lapses of attention or (2) variations in attentional control. We found that performance failures increase with set-size and strongly predict working memory capacity. Performance variability was best modeled by an attentional control model of attention, not a lapse model. We examined neural signatures of performance failures by measuring EEG activity while participants performed the whole-report task. The number of items correctly recalled in the memory task was predicted by frontal theta power, with decreased frontal theta power associated with poor performance on the task. In addition, we found that poor performance was not explained by failures of sensory encoding; the P1/N1 response and ocular artifact rates were equivalent for high- and low-performance trials. In all, we propose that attentional lapses alone cannot explain individual differences in working memory performance. Instead, we find that graded fluctuations in attentional control better explain the trial-by-trial differences in working memory that we observe.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Brain/physiology , Individuality , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Alpha Rhythm/physiology , Computer Simulation , Electroencephalography , Executive Function/physiology , Humans , Models, Psychological , Monte Carlo Method , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation , Theta Rhythm/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology
15.
Psychol Sci ; 26(7): 1026-37, 2015 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26040757

ABSTRACT

Although people are capable of storing a virtually infinite amount of information in memory, their ability to encode new information is far from perfect. The quality of encoding varies from moment to moment and renders some memories more accessible than others. Here, we were able to forecast the likelihood that a given item will be later recognized by monitoring two dissociable fluctuations of the electroencephalogram during encoding. Next, we identified individual items that were poorly encoded, using our electrophysiological measures in real time, and we successfully improved the efficacy of learning by having participants restudy these items. Thus, our memory forecasts using multiple electrophysiological signals demonstrate the feasibility and the effectiveness of using real-time monitoring of the moment-to-moment fluctuations of the quality of memory encoding to improve learning.


Subject(s)
Electrophysiological Phenomena , Learning , Memory , Adolescent , Adult , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , ROC Curve , Young Adult
16.
J Neurosci ; 33(19): 8257-63, 2013 May 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23658165

ABSTRACT

Visual working memory is an online workspace for temporarily representing visual information from the environment. The two most prevalent empirical characteristics of working memory are that it is supported by sustained neural activity over a delay period and it has a severely limited capacity for representing multiple items simultaneously. Traditionally, such delay activity and capacity limits have been considered to be exclusive for maintaining information about objects that are no longer visible to the observers. Here, by contrast, we provide both neurophysiological and psychophysical evidence that the sustained neural activity and capacity limits for items that are continuously visible to the human observer are indistinguishable from those measured for items that are no longer visible. This holds true even when the observers know that the objects will not disappear from the visual field. These results demonstrate that our explicit representation of objects that are still "in view" is far more limited than previously assumed.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Cues , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time , Students , Universities
17.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 14(1): 62-77, 2014 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24217849

ABSTRACT

A classic question concerns whether humans can attend multiple locations or objects at once. Although it is generally agreed that the answer to this question is "yes," the limits on this ability are subject to extensive debate. According to one view, attentional resources can be flexibly allocated to a variable number of locations, with an inverse relationship between the number of selected locations and the quality of information processing at each location. Alternatively, these resources might be quantized in a "discrete" fashion that enables concurrent access to a small number of locations. Here, we report a series of experiments comparing these alternatives. In each experiment, we cued participants to attend a variable number of spatial locations and asked them to report the orientation of a single, briefly presented target. In all experiments, participants' orientation report errors were well-described by a model that assumes a fixed upper limit in the number of locations that can be attended. Conversely, report errors were poorly described by a flexible-resource model that assumes no fixed limit on the number of locations that can be attended. Critically, we showed that these discrete limits were predicted by cue-evoked neural activity elicited before the onset of the target array, suggesting that performance was limited by selection processes that began prior to subsequent encoding and memory storage. Together, these findings constitute novel evidence supporting the hypothesis that human observers can attend only a small number of discrete locations at an instant.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Brain/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Cues , Electroencephalography , Electrooculography , Evoked Potentials , Humans , Models, Neurological , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation , Task Performance and Analysis , Young Adult
18.
Cogn Psychol ; 71: 1-26, 2014 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24531497

ABSTRACT

Several theories have been put forth to explain the relation between working memory (WM) and gF. Unfortunately, no single factor has been shown to fully account for the relation between these two important constructs. In the current study we tested whether multiple factors (capacity, attention control, and secondary memory) would collectively account for the relation. A large number of participants performed multiple measures of each construct and latent variable analyses were used to examine the data. The results demonstrated that capacity, attention control, and secondary memory were uniquely related to WM storage, WM processing, and gF. Importantly, the three factors completely accounted for the relation between WM (both processing and storage) and gF. Thus, although storage and processing make independent contributions to gF, both of these contributions are accounted for by variation in capacity, attention control and secondary memory. These results are consistent with the multifaceted view of WM, suggesting that individual differences in capacity, attention control, and secondary memory jointly account for individual differences in WM and its relation with gF.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Intelligence/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Models, Psychological , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Individuality , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
19.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 153(1): 56-69, 2024 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37733008

ABSTRACT

Ensemble coding (the brain's ability to rapidly extract summary statistics from groups of items) has been demonstrated across a range of low-level (e.g., average color) to high-level (e.g., average facial expression) visual features, and even on information that cannot be gleaned solely from retinal input (e.g., object lifelikeness). There is also evidence that ensemble coding can interact with other cognitive systems such as long-term memory (LTM), as observers are able to derive the average cost of items. We extended this line of research to examine if different sensory modalities can interact during ensemble coding. Participants made judgments about the average sweetness of groups of different visually presented foods. We found that, when viewed simultaneously, observers were limited in the number of items they could incorporate into their cross-modal ensemble percepts. We speculate that this capacity limit is caused by the cross-modal translation of visual percepts into taste representations stored in LTM. This was supported by findings that (a) participants could use similar stimuli to form capacity-unlimited ensemble representations of average screen size and (b) participants could extract the average sweetness of displays when items were viewed in sequence, with no capacity limitation (suggesting that spatial attention constrains the number of necessary visual cues an observer can integrate in a given moment to trigger cross-modal retrieval of taste). Together, the results of our study demonstrate that there are limits to the flexibility of ensemble coding, especially when multiple cognitive systems need to interact to compress sensory information into an ensemble representation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Cues , Memory, Long-Term , Humans , Attention , Facial Expression , Perception , Visual Perception
20.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 153(1): 38-55, 2024 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37650822

ABSTRACT

Comparing a visual memory with new visual stimuli can bias memory content, especially when the new stimuli are perceived as similar. Perceptual comparisons of this kind may play a mechanistic role in memory updating and can explain how memories can become erroneous in daily life. To test this possibility, we investigated whether comparisons can produce other types of memory distortion beyond memory bias that are commonly implicated in erroneous memories (e.g., memory misattribution). We hypothesized that the type of memory distortion induced during a comparison depends on the perceived overlap between the memory and incoming stimulus-when the input is perceived as similar, it biases memory content; when perceived as the same, it replaces memory content. Participants completed a delayed estimation task in which they compared their memories of color (Experiment 1) and shape stimuli (Experiment 2) to probe stimuli before reporting memory content. We found systematic errors in participants' memory reports following perceived similarity and sameness that were toward the probes and larger following perceived sameness. Simulations confirmed that these errors were not explained by noisy encoding processes that occurred before comparisons. Instead, computational modeling suggested that these errors were likely explained by the probabilistic replacement of the memory by the probe following perceived sameness and integration between the memory and the probe following perceived similarity. Together, these findings suggest that perceptual comparisons can prompt distinct forms of memory updating that have been described previously and may explain how memories become erroneous during their use in everyday behavior. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Judgment , Memory , Humans , Computer Simulation
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL