Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 20
Filter
1.
Cortex ; 169: 174-190, 2023 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37939510

ABSTRACT

Although evaluative judgments are a central component of everyday decision making little is known about the temporal dynamics of the processes used to make them. The present study used the high temporal resolution of event-related brain potentials (ERPs) to test Cunningham and Zelazo's (2007) posited differences in the timing of attitude tag retrieval relative to stimulus categorization for 'attitudes' and 'evaluations,' as well as tenets of their Iterative Reprocessing (IR) loop model. Participants made agree/disagree decisions about their attitudes and You/Not You decisions about their autobiographical memories in separate reaction time (RT) tasks while brain activity was recorded from 32 scalp sites. A median-split analysis on RT was used to separate fast and slow decisions. Decisions about autobiographical stimuli produced the typical results in which retrieval and stimulus categorization occurred together just before the response regardless of decision difficulty. By contrast, the relative timing of tag retrieval and categorization differed with difficulty for attitude decisions as predicted by the model. Fast attitude decisions were processed similarly to fast You decisions with retrieval and categorization timing coupled to the response. Slow attitude decisions, however, differed because, while tag retrieval timing was the same as for fast attitude decisions, post-retrieval processing delayed stimulus categorization and a response by 450 msec. ERP activity over dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) in the pre-response interval was asymmetrical, with greater activity for attitude and autobiographical decisions over left and right hemispheres, respectively, while amplitude and duration increased with decision difficulty for both. Slow attitude decisions alone elicited a reduced pre-response positivity, a correlate of goal-directed response selection. The results provide empirical support for key aspects of Cunningham and Zelazo's (2007) attitude-evaluation dichotomy and the timing of the posited component processes in their IR model as well as novel information about the roles of stored tags and reflective processes in different attitude decisions.


Subject(s)
Evoked Potentials , Memory, Episodic , Humans , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Attitude , Brain/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Brain Mapping
2.
Anesthesiology ; 110(2): 295-312, 2009 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19194157

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Intravenous drugs active via gamma-aminobutyric acid receptors to produce memory impairment during conscious sedation. Memory function was assessed using event-related potentials (ERPs) while drug was present. METHODS: The continuous recognition task measured recognition of photographs from working (6 s) and long-term (27 s) memory while ERPs were recorded from Cz (familiarity recognition) and Pz electrodes (recollection recognition). Volunteer participants received sequential doses of one of placebo (n = 11), 0.45 and 0.9 microg/ml propofol (n = 10), 20 and 40 ng/ml midazolam (n = 12), 1.5 and 3 microg/ml thiopental (n = 11), or 0.25 and 0.4 ng/ml dexmedetomidine (n = 11). End-of-day yes/no recognition 225 min after the end of drug infusion tested memory retention of pictures encoded on the continuous recognition tasks. RESULTS: Active drugs increased reaction times and impaired memory on the continuous recognition task equally, except for a greater effect of midazolam (P < 0.04). Forgetting from continuous recognition tasks to end of day was similar for all drugs (P = 0.40), greater than placebo (P < 0.001). Propofol and midazolam decreased the area between first presentation (new) and recognized (old, 27 s later) ERP waveforms from long-term memory for familiarity (P = 0.03) and possibly for recollection processes (P = 0.12). Propofol shifted ERP amplitudes to smaller voltages (P < 0.002). Dexmedetomidine may have impaired familiarity more than recollection processes (P = 0.10). Thiopental had no effect on ERPs. CONCLUSION: Propofol and midazolam impaired recognition ERPs from long-term memory but not working memory. ERP measures of memory revealed different pathways to end-of-day memory loss as early as 27 s after encoding.


Subject(s)
Conscious Sedation/psychology , Hypnotics and Sedatives/pharmacology , Memory/drug effects , Midazolam/pharmacology , Propofol/pharmacology , Adolescent , Adult , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Dexmedetomidine/pharmacology , Dose-Response Relationship, Drug , Electroencephalography/drug effects , Evoked Potentials/drug effects , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall/drug effects , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/drug effects , Reaction Time/drug effects , Thiopental/pharmacology , Young Adult
3.
Anesthesiology ; 109(2): 213-24, 2008 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18648230

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Propofol may produce amnesia by affecting encoding. The hypothesis that propofol weakens encoding was tested by measuring regional cerebral blood flow during verbal encoding. METHODS: Seventeen volunteer participants (12 men; aged 30.4 +/- 6.5 yr) had regional cerebral blood flow measured using H2O positron emission tomography during complex and simple encoding tasks (deep vs. shallow level of processing) to identify a region of interest in the left inferior prefrontal cortex (LIPFC). The effect of either propofol (n = 6, 0.9 microg/ml target concentration), placebo with a divided attention task (n = 5), or thiopental at sedative doses (n = 6, 3 microg/ml) on regional cerebral blood flow activation in the LIPFC was tested. The divided attention task was expected to decrease activation in the LIPFC. RESULTS: Propofol did not impair encoding performance or reaction times, but impaired recognition memory of deeply encoded words 4 h later (median recognition of 35% [interquartile range, 17-54%] of words presented during propofol vs. 65% [38-91%] before drug; P < 0.05). Statistical parametric mapping analysis identified a region of interest of 6.6 cm in the LIPFC (T = 7.44, P = 0.014). Regional cerebral blood flow response to deep encoding was present in this region of interest in each group before drug (T > 4.41, P < 0.04). During drug infusion, only the propofol group continued to have borderline significant activation in this region (T = 4.00, P = 0.063). CONCLUSIONS: If the amnesic effect of propofol were solely due to effects on encoding, activation in the LIPFC should be minimal. Because LIPFC activation was not totally eliminated by propofol, the amnesic action of propofol must be present in other brain regions and/or affect other memory processes.


Subject(s)
Amnesia/chemically induced , Cerebrum/blood supply , Hypnotics and Sedatives/adverse effects , Prefrontal Cortex/drug effects , Propofol/adverse effects , Psychomotor Performance/drug effects , Adult , Dose-Response Relationship, Drug , Electroencephalography/drug effects , Female , Humans , Hypnotics and Sedatives/pharmacology , Male , Positron-Emission Tomography , Propofol/pharmacology , Thiopental/adverse effects , Thiopental/pharmacology
4.
Neurosci Lett ; 432(2): 151-6, 2008 Feb 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18226452

ABSTRACT

Although it has been reported repeatedly that retrieval-related processes decline with aging, the influence of well-documented age-related encoding deficiencies on the observed changes at retrieval have not been ruled out as a contributing factor. Here, we disentangle this confound by using a serendipitous finding reported by Nessler et al. [D. Nessler, R. Johnson Jr., M. Bersick, D. Friedman, On why the elderly have normal semantic retrieval but deficient episodic encoding: a study of left inferior frontal ERP activity, Neuroimage 30 (2006) 299-312]. In that study, age-related differences in the magnitude of left inferior frontal brain activity at encoding and subsequent recognition memory performance were eliminated when a deeper level of semantic encoding in the older adults was compared with a shallow level in the young. Based on this earlier result, the present paper is concerned with the question of whether the matched recognition performance resulting from age-equivalent ERP encoding activity was also accompanied by age-invariant retrieval-related brain activity. The results in the young were consistent with dual-process models of recognition memory due to the presence of ERP activity linked previously to familiarity (frontal EM effect) and recollection (parietal EM effect). By contrast the older adults only showed evidence of familiarity-based processes. Thus, despite age-equivalent brain activity at encoding and subsequent recognition performance, older relative to young adults appeared to base their old-new decisions on a qualitatively different pattern of retrieval processes (i.e., more on familiarity and less on recollection). Consequently, these data suggest that the age-related changes in retrieval observed here are independent of, and likely occur in addition to, any age-related changes in encoding processes.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Cognition/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Memory/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Brain Mapping , Electroencephalography , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neural Pathways/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Reaction Time/physiology
5.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17963090

ABSTRACT

Older adults have difficulty when executive control must be brought on line to coordinate ongoing behavior. To assess age-related alterations in executive processing, task-switching performance and event-related potential (ERP) activity were compared in young and older adults on switch, post-switch, pre-switch, and no-switch trials, ordered in demand for executive processes from greatest to least. In stimulus-locked averages for young adults, only switch trials elicited fronto-central P3 components, indicative of task-set attentional reallocation, whereas in older adults, three of the four trial types evinced frontal potentials. In response-locked averages, the amplitude of a medial frontal negativity (MFN), a component reflecting conflict monitoring and detection, increased as a function of executive demands in the ERPs of the young but not those of the older adults. These data suggest altered executive processing in older adults resulting in persistent recruitment of prefrontal processes for conditions that do not require them in the young.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Reaction Time , Transfer, Psychology , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Attention , Cognition Disorders/epidemiology , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Severity of Illness Index
6.
Neuroreport ; 18(17): 1837-40, 2007 Nov 19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18090322

ABSTRACT

Aging differentially affects retrieval processes underlying recognition memory: familiarity is maintained, whereas recollection declines. We determined whether word repetition across two study-test phases enhanced older adults' use of recollection. During Test 1, frontal episodic memory effects, suggestive of familiarity-based processes, were age invariant, whereas only the young showed a parietal episodic memory effect, suggestive of recollection. Repetition did not modulate the frontal episodic memory effect in either group, but increased the parietal episodic memory effect in the young. Importantly, older adults showed a parietal episodic memory effect at Test 2, suggesting that repetition did enable recollection. Only older adults, however, showed a left frontal negativity, implying that they may have used additional processes to recover episodic memories.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Memory/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Analysis of Variance , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Female , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology
7.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 24(3): 386-404, 2005 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16099352

ABSTRACT

Behavior and event-related potentials (ERP) were recorded while participants made truthful and deceptive responses about previously memorized words under three instructional conditions: consistent truthful, consistent deceptive, and random deceptive. To determine if practice affected the deception-related activity we reported previously [R. Johnson, Jr., J. Barnhardt, J. Zhu, The deceptive response: effects of response conflict and strategic monitoring on the late positive component and episodic memory-related brain activity. Biol. Psychol., 64 (2003) 217-253; R. Johnson, Jr., J. Barnhardt, J. Zhu, The contribution of executive processes to deceptive responding. Neuropsychologia, 42 (2004) 878-901], participants performed two blocks of 145 trials of each condition. In the consistent truthful condition, practice benefited performance as indicated by decreased reaction time (RT) and RT variability. In addition, practice increased P300 amplitude and decreased the amplitude of a medial frontal negativity (MFN), which is believed to index the use of response-monitoring processes. However, a different pattern of results obtained in the two deception conditions. Although practice decreased RTs by almost as much as in the consistent truthful condition, the extent to which deceptive response in both conditions were slower than those in the consistent truthful condition actually increased slightly. Hence, the component of RT reflecting processing of conflicting response information did not decrease. In accord with the RT results, MFN amplitudes in the consistent deceptive and random deceptive conditions were unaffected by practice, suggesting that the amount of executive processes required to make and/or monitor deceptive responses was undiminished by practice. Although P300 amplitude increased slightly in the consistent deceptive condition, there was no change in the random deceptive condition. Thus, a major finding here is that, unlike truthful responses, the conceptually driven response conflicts underlying deceptive responses appear to be as resistant to practice-induced changes as described previously for perceptually driven response conflicts.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Lie Detection/psychology , Practice, Psychological , Adult , Event-Related Potentials, P300/physiology , Female , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Humans , Male , Memory/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology
8.
Neuropsychology ; 19(1): 5-17, 2005 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15656758

ABSTRACT

A multitarget visual cancellation test was administered to patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD) and age-matched healthy controls (HC). Attentional loads--physical similarity (number of features shared by target and distractors; 3 levels) and density (number of items per page; 3 levels)--were varied systematically. As physical similarity increased, both groups slowed their search, but whereas the HC group maintained accuracy, the AD group increased commission errors and tended to miss more targets. Increased density yielded slower search and more target omissions in the AD group. Commission errors are additional indicators of higher order attentional deficits, especially in early disease. The findings suggest that patients with AD may rely increasingly on physical features of stimuli during a search, leading to inefficient bottom-up processing strategies.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Attention/physiology , Cognition Disorders/physiopathology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Analysis of Variance , Case-Control Studies , Demography , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology , Reproducibility of Results
9.
Neuropsychologia ; 42(7): 878-901, 2004.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14998703

ABSTRACT

We measured behavioral responses (RT) and recorded event-related brain potentials (ERPs) when participants made truthful and deceptive responses about perceived and remembered stimuli. Participants performed an old/new recognition test under three instructional conditions: Consistent Truthful, Consistent Deceptive and Random Deceptive. Compared to Consistent Truthful responses, Consistent Deceptive responses to both perceived and remembered stimuli produced the same pattern of less accurate, slower and more variable responses and larger medial frontal negativities (MFN). The MFN is thought to reflect activity in anterior cingulate cortex, a brain area involved in monitoring actions and resolving conflicting response tendencies. The Random Deceptive condition required participants to strategically monitor their long-term response patterns to accommodate a deceptive strategy. Even compared to the Consistent Deceptive condition, RTs in the Random Deceptive condition were significantly slower and more variable and MFN activity increased significantly. MFN scalp distribution results revealed the presence of three different patterns of brain activity; one each for truthful responses, deceptive responses and strategic monitoring. Thus, the data indicate that anterior cingulate cortex plays a key role in making deceptive responses.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Deception , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Perception/physiology , Problem Solving/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Brain Mapping/methods , Cerebral Cortex/anatomy & histology , Electroencephalography/methods , Female , Humans , In Vitro Techniques , Lie Detection/psychology , Male , Memory/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology
10.
Pain ; 38(3): 303-312, 1989 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2812841

ABSTRACT

In order to determine the effects of attention and distraction on painful and non-painful stimuli, the amplitude changes of 3 components (N150, P200, P300) of the somatosensory event-related potential (SERP) elicited by painful and non-painful electrical stimuli were investigated. Painful and non-painful stimuli were determined using a visual analog scale. SERPs were recorded from 16 healthy volunteers at 5 midline and 4 left and 4 right hemispheric sites. The differences between the amplitudes of attended and ignored stimuli were quantified with a baseline-to-peak measure. ANOVA results revealed no significant attention or stimulus intensity effects for N150 but highly significant differences in P200 and P300 amplitudes between attended and ignored stimuli. In addition, P200 and P300 amplitudes were larger for strong stimuli than for weak stimuli, with no significant differences between non-painful and painful stimuli. These findings are consistent with the existence of a relative, rather than an absolute, relationship between SERP component amplitudes and subjective pain reports. Furthermore, the data give evidence that attentional manipulations represent a powerful method to decrease the perception of pain and that, when used with subjective and behavioral measures, the SERP represents a valuable asset in the multidimensional approach to pain measurement and assessment.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Somatosensory/physiology , Pain Measurement/methods , Pain/physiopathology , Adult , Electric Stimulation , Female , Humans , Male
11.
Biol Psychol ; 64(3): 217-53, 2003 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14630405

ABSTRACT

The cognitive processes and neural mechanisms underlying deceptive responses were studied using behavioral responses (RT) and event-related brain potentials (ERPs) while participants made truthful and deceptive responses about perceived and remembered stimuli. Memorized words were presented in a recognition paradigm under three instructional conditions: Consistent Truthful, Consistent Deceptive, Random Deceptive. Responses that conflicted with the truth about both perceived and remembered items produced the same pattern of slower RTs and decreased LPC amplitudes. When long-term response patterns were monitored, RTs became much slower and LPC amplitudes decreased greatly. The different behavioral and ERP changes in the two deception conditions suggested that two dissociable executive control processes, each requiring additional processing resources, can contribute to deceptive responses. The parietal episodic memory (EM) effect, thought to reflect recollection, was unaffected by whether participants responded truthfully or deceptively suggesting that it provides a measure of guilty knowledge.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Lie Detection , Memory/physiology , Truth Disclosure , Adolescent , Adult , Electroencephalography , Female , Guilt , Humans , Knowledge , Male
12.
Child Neuropsychol ; 20(2): 196-209, 2014 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23387525

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has been associated with deficits in self-regulatory cognitive processes, some of which are thought to lie at the heart of the disorder. Slowing of reaction times (RTs) for correct responses following errors made during decision tasks has been interpreted as an indication of intact self-regulatory functioning and has been shown to be attenuated in school-aged children with ADHD. This study attempted to examine whether ADHD symptoms are associated with an early-emerging deficit in posterror slowing. METHOD: A computerized two-choice RT task was administered to an ethnically diverse sample of preschool-aged children classified as either "control" (n = 120) or "hyperactive/inattentive" (HI; n = 148) using parent- and teacher-rated ADHD symptoms. Analyses were conducted to determine whether HI preschoolers exhibit a deficit in this self-regulatory ability. RESULTS: HI children exhibited reduced posterror slowing relative to controls on the trials selected for analysis. Supplementary analyses indicated that this may have been due to a reduced proportion of trials following errors on which HI children slowed rather than due to a reduction in the absolute magnitude of slowing on all trials following errors. CONCLUSIONS: High levels of ADHD symptoms in preschoolers may be associated with a deficit in error processing as indicated by posterror slowing. The results of supplementary analyses suggest that this deficit is perhaps more a result of failures to perceive errors than of difficulties with executive control.


Subject(s)
Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/psychology , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Psychomotor Performance , Reaction Time/physiology , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/diagnosis , Child , Child, Preschool , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Executive Function , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests
13.
Psychol Aging ; 28(2): 443-56, 2013 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23276214

ABSTRACT

Nessler, Johnson, Bersick, and Friedman (D. Nessler, R. Johnson, Jr., M. Bersick, & D. Friedman, 2006, On why the elderly have normal semantic retrieval but deficient episodic encoding: A study of left inferior frontal ERP activity, NeuroImage, Vol. 30, pp. 299-312) found that, compared with young adults, older adults show decreased event-related brain potential (ERP) activity over posterior left inferior prefrontal cortex (pLIPFC) in a 400- to 1,400-ms interval during episodic encoding. This altered brain activity was associated with significantly decreased recognition performance and reduced recollection-related brain activity at retrieval (D. Nessler, D. Friedman, R. Johnson, Jr., & M. Bersick, 2007, Does repetition engender the same retrieval processes in young and older adults? NeuroReport, Vol. 18, pp. 1837-1840). To test the hypothesis that older adults' well-documented episodic retrieval deficit is related to reduced pLIPFC activity at encoding, we used a novel divided attention task in healthy young adults that was specifically timed to disrupt encoding in either the 1st or 2nd half of a 300- to 1,400-ms interval. The results showed that diverting resources for 550 ms during either half of this interval reproduced the 4 characteristic aspects of the older participants' retrieval performance: normal semantic retrieval during encoding, reduced subsequent episodic recognition and recall, reduced recollection-related ERP activity, and the presence of "compensatory" brain activity. We conclude that part of older adults' episodic memory deficit is attributable to altered pLIPFC activity during encoding due to reduced levels of available processing resources. Moreover, the findings also provide insights into the nature and timing of the putative "compensatory" processes posited to be used by older adults in an attempt to compensate for age-related decline in cognitive function. These results support the scaffolding account of compensation, in which the recruitment of additional cognitive processes is an adaptive response across the life span.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Attention/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Memory Disorders/physiopathology , Memory, Episodic , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Task Performance and Analysis , Adult , Aged , Cognition/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Semantics , Time Factors , Young Adult
14.
Biol Psychol ; 91(2): 245-62, 2012 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22820040

ABSTRACT

This task-switching ERP study of 16 young participants investigated whether increased RT slowing on stay trials and faster RTs on switch trials for frequent than infrequent switching are explained by an activation or preparation account. The activation account proposes that task sets are maintained at a higher baseline activation level for frequent switching, necessitating increased task-set updating, as reflected by a larger and/or longer lasting early parietal positivity. The preparation account assumes advance (pre-cue) switch preparation (i.e., task-set reconfiguration), preceding stay and switch trials for frequent switching, as reflected by pre-cue and post-cue late parietal positivities. By and large, the data support the activation account. However, we also found increased, pre-cue task-set updating on frequent stay trials and pre-cue, task-set reconfiguration prior to predictable, frequent switches. These results lead us to propose an extended activation account to explain the effects of switch probability on the executive processes underlying task-switching behavior.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Executive Function/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Attention/physiology , Cues , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Probability , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology
15.
Neuropsychologia ; 49(5): 945-960, 2011 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21262245

ABSTRACT

Event-related potentials (ERPs) are unique in their ability to provide information about the timing of activity in the neural networks that perform complex cognitive processes. Given the dearth of extant data from normal controls on the question of whether attitude representations are stored in episodic or semantic memory, the goal here was to study the nature of the memory representations used during conscious attitude evaluations. Thus, we recorded ERPs while participants performed three tasks: attitude evaluations (i.e., agree/disagree), autobiographical cued recall (i.e., You/Not You) and semantic evaluations (i.e., active/inactive). The key finding was that the parietal episodic memory (EM) effect, a well-established correlate of episodic recollection, was elicited by both attitude evaluations and autobiographical retrievals. By contrast, semantic evaluations of the same attitude items elicited less parietal activity, like that elicited by Not You cues, which only access semantic memory. In accord with hemodynamic results, attitude evaluations and autobiographical retrievals also produced overlapping patterns of slow potential (SP) activity from 500 to 900ms preceding the response over left and right inferior frontal, anterior medial frontal and occipital brain areas. Significantly different patterns of SP activity were elicited in these locations for semantic evaluations and Not You cues. Taken together, the results indicate that attitude representations are stored in episodic memory. Retrieval timing varied as a function of task, with earlier retrievals in both evaluation conditions relative to those in the autobiographical condition. The differential roles and timing of memory retrieval in evaluative judgment and memory retrieval tasks are discussed.


Subject(s)
Attitude , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Judgment/physiology , Memory/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Brain Mapping , Cues , Electroencephalography/methods , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall , Neuropsychological Tests , Reaction Time , Semantics , Young Adult
16.
Neuroimage ; 39(1): 469-82, 2008 Jan 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17919934

ABSTRACT

This study sought to extend previous results regarding deceptions about specific memories by investigating the role of executive processes in deceptions about evaluative judgments. In addition, given that previous studies of deception have not included valence manipulations, we also wanted to determine whether the goodness/badness aspect of the items would affect the processes used during deception. Thus, we compared behavioral and event-related potential (ERP) activity while participants made truthful and directed lie (i.e., press opposite of the truth) responses about attitude items with which they either strongly agreed or disagreed. Consistent with previous results, deceptive responses required greater cognitive control as indicated by slower RTs, larger medial frontal negativities (MFN) and smaller late positive components than truthful responses. Furthermore, the magnitude of these deception-related effects was dependent on the valence that participants assigned to the items (i.e., agree/disagree). Directed lie responses about attitudes also resulted in greatly reduced pre-response positivities, an indication that participants strategically monitored their responses even in the absence of explicit task demands. Item valence also differentially affected the amplitude of three ERP components in a 650 ms pre-response interval, independently of whether truthful or deceptive responses were made. Analyses using dipole locations based on results from fMRI studies of evaluative judgments and deception indicated a high degree of overlap between the ERP and fMRI results and revealed the possible temporal characteristics of the hemodynamic activations.


Subject(s)
Attitude , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Conflict, Psychological , Deception , Decision Making/physiology , Judgment/physiology , Lie Detection , Adolescent , Adult , Brain Mapping , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Female , Humans , Male
17.
Clin EEG Neurosci ; 38(1): 2-7, 2007 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17319586

ABSTRACT

Decline in episodic memory, the encoding and retrieval of autobiographical events, is a hallmark of normal cognitive aging. Although the primary causes of this decline remain elusive, event-related brain potential (ERP) studies have contributed to an understanding of age-related episodic memory failure. These data reveal that, although the retrieval-based episodic memory (EM) effect does not differ dramatically between young and older adults, the acquisition-related data suggest a decline in episodic encoding (i.e., semantic elaboration) with increasing age. We conclude that, at the current state of knowledge, encoding deficiencies are more important than retrieval deficits in understanding the causes of episodic memory decline in the older adult.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Brain/physiology , Clinical Trials as Topic/trends , Electroencephalography/methods , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Memory/physiology , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Brain Mapping/methods , Humans
18.
Neurobiol Aging ; 28(11): 1769-82, 2007 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16930775

ABSTRACT

Evidence indicates age-related performance decreases in the presence of response conflict, but the underlying mechanisms are not understood. Multiple processes are active, including those that detect and monitor response conflict, which, if necessary, signal the upregulation of cognitive control. To assess which of these executive processes is affected by aging, behavioral and brain responses were measured for compatible and incompatible responses to words ("LEFT"; "RIGHT"). After a correct response, the overall increase in response conflict on incompatible trials engendered similarly decreased accuracy and increased medial frontal negativities (MFN) for both age groups. Age-invariance was also present for momentary increases in response conflict on post-error compatible trials. Hence, the processes for detecting, monitoring and managing response conflict are functionally intact when people age. However, when response conflict was greatest (post-error incompatible), decreased accuracy and increased MFNs were observed only for the elderly. Thus, because the elderly were able to detect and monitor response conflict even at the highest levels, the reason for their performance decrease most likely lies in compromised upregulation of cognitive control.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Conflict, Psychological , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Aging/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychomotor Performance/physiology
19.
Neuroimage ; 30(1): 299-312, 2006 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16242350

ABSTRACT

Age-related left inferior prefrontal cortex (LIPFC) blood flow reductions during semantic retrieval are associated with reduced subsequent episodic recognition memory performance but are inconsistent with age-invariant semantic retrieval performance. Therefore, we compared brain activity in young and elderly persons during low- and high-selection versions of a semantic task using ERPs recorded at 62 scalp locations. In an early interval (400-800 ms), both age groups showed more negativity over left inferior frontal scalp while performing the high- compared to the low-selection task. This early semantic selection-related negativity was associated with age-equivalent accuracy and reaction time on the semantic tasks. Further, in the early time interval, the ERPs of the young were more negative than those of the elderly. In addition, only the young showed a late selection-related negativity (1,200--1,400 ms). These age-related differences in left frontal ERP activity were associated with significant decrements in subsequent recognition for the elderly. Moreover, additional analyses revealed that larger amounts of negativity over left inferior frontal scalp locations during semantic retrieval were correlated with enhanced subsequent episodic recognition in both groups. In sum, the data suggest that (1) semantic retrieval and episodic encoding processes overlap temporally in similar brain regions, most likely including the LIPFC, and (2) the failure to recruit these regions in the service of episodic encoding underlies the age-related deficit in episodic memory.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Life Change Events , Mental Recall/physiology , Retention, Psychology/physiology , Semantics , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Contingent Negative Variation , Female , Humans , Male , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Reaction Time/physiology , Reference Values
20.
Anesthesiology ; 101(4): 831-41, 2004 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15448514

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Sedative-hypnotic drugs impair memory, but details regarding the nature of this effect are unknown. The influences of propofol, thiopental, and dexmedetomidine on the performance of a task that isolates specific components of episodic memory function were measured. METHODS: Working (1 intervening item, 6 s) and long-term memory (10 intervening items, 33 s) were tested using auditory words in a continuous recognition task before and during drug administration. Eighty-three volunteer participants were randomly assigned to receive a constant target concentration of drug or placebo, producing sedative effects from imperceptible to unresponsiveness. Responsive participants were categorized as high or low performers, using a median split of long-term memory performance during drug administration. Recognition of words at the end of the study day was assessed. RESULTS: High performers had acquisition of material into long-term memory when drug was present at the same level as placebo. Retention of this material at 225 min was significantly less for propofol (39 +/- 23% loss of material) than for other drugs (17-23% loss; P < 0.01). Greater sedation in low performers was evident in multiple measures. Memory for words presented before drug was no different from that associated with placebo for all groups. CONCLUSIONS: Lack of retention of material acquired into long-term memory during propofol administration, associated with minimal sedation, seems to define drug-induced amnesia. Sedation seems to impair the acquisition or encoding of material into long-term memory. Therefore, the putative targets of drug-induced amnesia by propofol are processes associated with retention of material in long-term memory.


Subject(s)
Dexmedetomidine/adverse effects , Memory Disorders/chemically induced , Propofol/adverse effects , Thiopental/adverse effects , Electroencephalography/drug effects , Female , Humans , Male , Pain Measurement , Reaction Time
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL