Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 16 de 16
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
País de afiliación
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Behav Pharmacol ; 30(4): 363-369, 2019 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30272586

RESUMEN

Alcohol is the most commonly abused drug in the USA and many people suffer from alcohol use disorder. Many factors are associated with alcohol use disorder, but the causal role of comorbid nicotine use has not been extensively considered. Nicotine has reward-enhancing properties and may increase the value of alcohol. Monoamine oxidase inhibition increases nicotine self-administration and may increase the reward-enhancing effects of nicotine. We assessed the effect of nicotine and nicotine in combination with a commonly used monoamine oxidase inhibitor (tranylcypromine) on the value of alcohol using a progressive ratio schedule of reinforcement in rats. Nicotine administration increased the breakpoint for alcohol, but nicotine in combination with tranylcypromine decreased the breakpoint for alcohol. The current study adds to previous research showing that nicotine increases the value of alcohol. This finding has important implications for the etiology of addiction because of the comorbidity of smoking with many drugs of abuse. The finding that nicotine in combination with tranylcypromine reduces the value of alcohol warrants further investigation.


Asunto(s)
Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/tratamiento farmacológico , Nicotina/farmacología , Tranilcipromina/farmacología , Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/metabolismo , Animales , Conducta Adictiva/tratamiento farmacológico , Conducta Adictiva/metabolismo , Condicionamiento Operante/efectos de los fármacos , Etanol/metabolismo , Masculino , Inhibidores de la Monoaminooxidasa/metabolismo , Inhibidores de la Monoaminooxidasa/farmacología , Nicotina/metabolismo , Ratas , Ratas Long-Evans , Refuerzo en Psicología , Recompensa , Autoadministración , Tranilcipromina/metabolismo
2.
Aging Ment Health ; 19(8): 689-97, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25289681

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: Physiological reactivity (PR) describes the change in physiological functioning (e.g., heart rate, blood pressure, pulse pressure) that occurs after the induction of a stressful task. This study aims to understand the influence of mental health symptoms on patterns of PR during autobiographical narratives in an older adult sample. METHOD: Eighty older adults completed self-report measures regarding their symptoms of depression and anxiety. Next, their blood pressure was recorded while they completed two verbal autobiographical narratives. RESULTS: During the positive narrative, anxiety was positively associated with increased PR while depression was negatively associated with PR. During the negative narrative, a significant interaction occurred whereby anxiety was significantly positively associated with PR for those participants low in depression. DISCUSSION: The above results are explained in the context of the Tripartite Model of Depression and Anxiety, which predicts different patterns of PR as a function of mental health symptoms. Limitations and future directions are also discussed.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Ansiedad/psicología , Presión Sanguínea/fisiología , Depresión/psicología , Narrativas Personales como Asunto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Depresión/fisiopatología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria Episódica , Modelos Psicológicos
3.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 117(3): 576-596, 2022 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35467762

RESUMEN

Individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) tend to display restricted, repetitive behaviors and deficits in social interaction. Rats exposed to valproate (VPA) in utero have been shown to model symptoms of ASD. In previous research, VPA rats engaged in less social interaction and more repetitive responding than controls. The purpose of the present study was to further investigate behavioral variability in the VPA rat model of ASD by testing VPA and control rats in a reinforced-behavioral-variability operant task. In this procedure, rats emitted sequences of lever presses, some of which produced food. During baseline, food was delivered probabilistically, and variability was not required. Next, rats were exposed either to a variability contingency, in which food was only delivered following sequences that differed sufficiently from previous sequences (i.e., variability required), or to a yoked contingency, in which variability was not required. We hypothesized that VPA rats would behave less variably than controls in this task. However, VPA and control rats responded similarly variably when variability was required. Furthermore, VPA rats behaved slightly more variably than controls during baseline and yoked conditions, when variability was not required. These findings contribute to the complex literature surrounding the VPA rat model of ASD.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Efectos Tardíos de la Exposición Prenatal , Animales , Modelos Animales de Enfermedad , Femenino , Humanos , Embarazo , Ratas , Conducta Social , Ácido Valproico/farmacología
4.
Perspect Behav Sci ; 45(1): 209-237, 2022 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35342867

RESUMEN

Group-based experimental designs are an outgrowth of the logic of null-hypothesis significance testing and thus, statistical tests are often considered inappropriate for single-case experimental designs. Behavior analysts have recently been more supportive of efforts to include appropriate statistical analysis techniques to evaluate single-case experimental design data. One way that behavior analysts can incorporate statistical analyses into their practices with single-case experimental designs is to use Monte Carlo analyses. These analyses compare experimentally obtained behavioral data to simulated samples of behavioral data to determine the likelihood that the experimentally obtained results occurred due to chance (i.e., a p value). Monte Carlo analyses are more in line with behavior analytic principles than traditional null-hypothesis significance testing. We present an open-source Monte Carlo tool, created in shiny, for behavior analysts who want to use Monte Carlo analyses in addition as part of their data analysis.

5.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 115(2): 495-509, 2021 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33556201

RESUMEN

Delay discounting is the loss in value of an outcome as a function of its delay. The present study focused on examining a trait-like characteristic of delay discounting in a preclinical animal model. Specifically, we were interested in whether there was a positive relation between discounting of 2 different outcomes in rats. That is, would rats that discount delayed food steeply also discount delayed water steeply? In addition, we examined how session-to-session variability in discounting could be attributed to differences between subjects (trait variability) and to differences within subjects (state variability). Finally, we measured discounting from early- to mid-adulthood, allowing us to examine changes in discounting as a function of age. Overall, we found a moderate, positive correlation between discounting of food and discounting of water in rats, providing further evidence that the relative consistency with which individuals discount different outcomes is a trait-like characteristic. In addition, we found a high degree of within-subject variability in discounting, indicating strong state-like differences from session to session. Finally, overall, discounting decreased as a function of age; however, individual-subject data showed variability in how discounting changed across time. Overall, our results show that differences in delay discounting between individuals reflect variability in both trait- and state-like characteristics.


Asunto(s)
Descuento por Demora , Animales , Ratas , Recompensa , Agua
6.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 114(3): 381-393, 2020 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33179789

RESUMEN

The present study examined resurgence of reinforced variability in college students, who completed a 3-phase computer-based variability task. In the first phase, baseline, points were delivered for drawing rectangles that sufficiently differed from previous rectangles in terms of a target dimension (size or location, counterbalanced) but were sufficiently similar in terms of the alternative dimension. In the second phase, alternative, points were only delivered for rectangles that were sufficiently different in terms of the alternative dimension, but repetitive in terms of the target dimension. In the third phase, extinction, no points were delivered. In baseline, participants made rectangles that were highly varied in terms of the target dimension and less varied in terms of the alternative dimension, and vice versa in the alternative phase. During extinction, levels of variability increased for the target dimension, providing evidence for resurgence of reinforced variability of a specific dimension of behavior. However, levels of variability also remained high for the alternative dimension, indicating that extinction-induced response variability may also have impacted the results. Although future research is needed to explore other explanations, the results of this study replicate prior research with pigeons and provide some support for the notion of variability as an operant.


Asunto(s)
Extinción Psicológica , Refuerzo en Psicología , Condicionamiento Operante , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental , Modelos Psicológicos , Esquema de Refuerzo , Adulto Joven
7.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 113(3): 495-514, 2020 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32189362

RESUMEN

Although individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) tend to behave repetitively, certain reinforcement contingencies (e.g., lag schedules) can be used to increase behavioral variability. In a lag schedule, reinforcers only follow responses that differ from recent responses. The present study was designed to promote variable play behavior in preschoolers with ASD interacting with playsets and figurines and to assess preference for variability and repetition contingencies. Data have shown a preference for variability in pigeons and college students, but this effect has not been explored in clinical populations. In this experiment, preschoolers with ASD were taught to discriminate between variability and repetition contingencies. Only play behaviors that met a lag schedule were reinforced in the presence of one color, and only repetitive behaviors were reinforced in the presence of another. After differential performance was established, participants experienced a concurrent chains schedule. Participants chose between the colors taught in training and then completed a play session with the selected contingency. One participant selected variability and repetition equally. The other participants showed a slight preference for variability. These results indicate that some individuals with ASD may play repetitively, not because they prefer repetition, but because they require additional teaching to play variably.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/terapia , Terapia Conductista/métodos , Conducta de Elección , Juego e Implementos de Juego/psicología , Refuerzo en Psicología , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Conducta Infantil/psicología , Preescolar , Color , Humanos , Masculino
8.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 114(2): 203-215, 2020 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32852106

RESUMEN

Delay discounting is the process by which a commodity loses value as the delay to its receipt increases. Rapid discounting predicts various maladaptive behaviors including tobacco use. Typically, delay discounting of different outcomes has been compared between cigarette smokers and nonsmokers. To better understand the relationship of delay discounting to different modes of tobacco use, we examined the differences in delay discounting of different outcomes between cigarette smokers, smokeless tobacco users, e-cigarette users, and non-tobacco users. In the present study, all participants completed 8 titrating delay-discounting tasks: $100 gain, $500 gain, $500 loss, alcohol, entertainment, food, a temporary health gain, and a temporary cure from a disease. Non-tobacco users discounted most outcomes less than tobacco users overall; however, there were no differences in discounting among the different types of tobacco users. These results suggest that nicotine consumption of any kind is associated with a higher degree of impulsivity compared to non-tobacco users. Adoption of tobacco products that have been perceived as less harmful (e.g., e-cigarettes) is not associated with a baseline difference or decrease in delay discounting if adopted after a history of cigarette use.


Asunto(s)
Descuento por Demora , Sistemas Electrónicos de Liberación de Nicotina , Productos de Tabaco , Tabaco sin Humo , Humanos , Fumadores , Uso de Tabaco , Tabaco sin Humo/efectos adversos
9.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 113(3): 657-679, 2020 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32147840

RESUMEN

Steep delay discounting is characterized by a preference for small immediate outcomes relative to larger delayed outcomes and is predictive of drug abuse, risky sexual behaviors, and other maladaptive behaviors. Nancy M. Petry was a pioneer in delay discounting research who demonstrated that people discount delayed monetary gains less steeply than they discount substances with abuse liability. Subsequent research found steep discounting for not only drugs, but other nonmonetary outcomes such as food, sex, and health. In this systematic review, we evaluate the hypotheses proposed to explain differences in discounting as a function of the type of outcome and explore the trait- and state-like nature of delay discounting. We found overwhelming evidence for the state-like quality of delay discounting: Consistent with Petry and others' work, nonmonetary outcomes are discounted more steeply than monetary outcomes. We propose two hypotheses that together may account for this effect: Decreasing Future Preference and Decreasing Future Worth. We also found clear evidence that delay discounting has trait-like qualities: People who steeply discount monetary outcomes steeply discount nonmonetary outcomes as well. The implication is that changing delay discounting for one outcome could change discounting for other outcomes.


Asunto(s)
Descuento por Demora , Animales , Conducta de Elección , Humanos , Recompensa , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/psicología
10.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 111(2): 289-308, 2019 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30556581

RESUMEN

Relapse is the recovery of a previously suppressed response. Animal models have been useful in examining the mechanisms underlying relapse (e.g., reinstatement, renewal, reacquisition, resurgence). However, there are several challenges to analyzing relapse data using traditional approaches. For example, null hypothesis significance testing is commonly used to determine whether relapse has occurred. However, this method requires several a priori assumptions about the data, as well as a large sample size for between-subjects comparisons or repeated testing for within-subjects comparisons. Monte Carlo methods may represent an improved analytic technique, because these methods require no prior assumptions, permit smaller sample sizes, and can be tailored to account for all of the data from an experiment instead of some limited set. In the present study, we conducted reanalyses of three studies of relapse (Berry, Sweeney, & Odum, ; Galizio et al., ; Odum & Shahan, ) using Monte Carlo techniques to determine if relapse occurred and if there were differences in rate of response based on relevant independent variables (such as group membership or schedule of reinforcement). These reanalyses supported the previous findings. Finally, we provide general recommendations for using Monte Carlo methods in studies of relapse.


Asunto(s)
Condicionamiento Operante , Método de Montecarlo , Animales , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Modelos Animales de Enfermedad , Extinción Psicológica , Recurrencia , Prevención Secundaria
11.
PLoS One ; 13(8): e0202230, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30110388

RESUMEN

Alcohol is the most commonly used drug in the United States and alcohol abuse can lead to alcohol use disorder. Alcohol use disorder is a persistent condition and relapse rates following successful remission are high. Many factors have been associated with relapse for alcohol use disorder, but identification of these factors has not been well translated into preventative utility. One potentially important factor, concurrent nicotine use, has not been well investigated as a causal factor in relapse for alcohol use disorder. Nicotine increases the value of other stimuli in the environment and may increase the value of alcohol. If nicotine increases the value of alcohol, then nicotine use during and after treatment may make relapse more probable. In the current study, we investigated the effect of continuous nicotine exposure (using osmotic minipumps to deliver nicotine or saline, depending on group, at a constant rate for 28 days) on resurgence of alcohol seeking in rats. Resurgence is a type of relapse preparation that consists of three phases: Baseline, Alternative Reinforcement, and Resurgence Testing. During Baseline, target responses produced a dipper of alcohol. During Alternative Reinforcement, target responses were extinguished and responses on a chain produced a chocolate pellet. During Resurgence Testing, responses on the chain were also extinguished and a return to responding on the target lever was indicative of resurgence. Multilevel modeling was used to analyze the effect of nicotine on resurgence. Both the nicotine and saline group showed resurgence of alcohol seeking, but there was no difference in the degree of resurgence across groups. Future directions could involve testing alternative drug delivery techniques.


Asunto(s)
Alcoholismo/etiología , Nicotina/administración & dosificación , Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/psicología , Alcoholismo/psicología , Animales , Conducta Animal/efectos de los fármacos , Condicionamiento Operante , Modelos Animales de Enfermedad , Interacciones Farmacológicas , Comportamiento de Búsqueda de Drogas/efectos de los fármacos , Etanol/administración & dosificación , Etanol/efectos adversos , Humanos , Masculino , Nicotina/efectos adversos , Ratas , Ratas Long-Evans , Recurrencia , Refuerzo en Psicología
12.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 110(3): 412-429, 2018 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30203525

RESUMEN

We examined the effects of outcome framing on delay discounting. In Experiment 1, participants completed four delay-discounting tasks. In one monetary task, money was framed in units of dollars ($50), and in the other, money was framed in units of handfuls of quarters (equal to $50). In one food task, food was framed in clear units of food (e.g., 100 M&Ms), and in the other, food was framed in units of servings (e.g., 10 servings of M&Ms). When money was framed in units of dollars, participants discounted less by delay compared to discounting of handfuls of quarters. When food was framed as clear units, participants also discounted less compared to how they discounted servings. In Experiment 2, participants completed two delay-discounting tasks for dollars and quarters (e.g., $50 or 200 quarters) to determine if the results of Experiment 1 were due to the differences in handling costs. In one delay-discounting task, money was framed in units of dollars. In the other delay-discounting task, money was framed in units of quarters. There was no difference in how participants discounted delayed money framed as dollars or quarters. Clear unit framing may result in less discounting by delay than fuzzy unit framing.


Asunto(s)
Descuento por Demora , Recompensa , Adulto , Femenino , Alimentos , Humanos , Masculino
13.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 109(1): 210-237, 2018 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29380434

RESUMEN

The present study examined persistence and relapse of reinforced behavioral variability in pigeons. Pigeons emitted four-response sequences across two keys. Sequences produced food according to a lag schedule, in which a response sequence was followed by food if it differed from a certain number of previous sequences. In Experiment 1, food was delivered for sequences that satisfied a lag schedule in both components of a multiple schedule. When reinforcement was removed for one component (i.e., extinction), levels of behavioral variability decreased for only that component. In Experiment 2, food was delivered for sequences satisfying a lag schedule in one component of a multiple schedule. In the other component, food was delivered at the same rate, but without the lag variability requirement (i.e., yoked). Following extinction, levels of behavioral variability returned to baseline for both components after response-independent food delivery (i.e., reinstatement). In Experiment 3, one group of pigeons responded on a lag variability schedule, and the other group responded on a lag repetition schedule. For both groups, levels of behavioral variability increased when alternative reinforcement was suspended (i.e., resurgence). In each experiment, we observed some evidence for extinction-induced response variability and for variability as an operant dimension of behavior.


Asunto(s)
Condicionamiento Operante , Refuerzo en Psicología , Retención en Psicología , Animales , Columbidae , Esquema de Refuerzo
14.
Behav Processes ; 140: 202-215, 2017 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28512037

RESUMEN

Following training with verbal stimulus relations involving A is greater than B and B is greater than C, verbally-competent individuals reliably select A>C when asked "which is greater, A or C?" (i.e., verbal transitive inference). This result is easy to interpret. Nonhuman animals and humans with and without intellectual disabilities have been exposed to nonverbal transitive-inference procedures involving trained arbitrary stimulus relations. Following the training of A+B-, B+C-, C+D-, and D+E-, B reliably is selected over D (i.e., nonverbal transitive inference). Such findings are more challenging to interpret. The present research explored accounts of nonverbal transitive inference based in transitive inference per se, reinforcement, such as value-transfer theory, and operant stimulus control. In Experiment 1, college students selected B>G following the training of A+B-, B+C-, C+D-///E+F-, F+G-, and G+H- (where///signifies the omission of D+E-). In Experiment 2, college students selected B>G following the training of A+B-, B+C-, C+D-///E+F-, F+G-, and G+X- (where X refers to 10 stimuli that alternated across trials). In Experiment 3, college students selected G>B following the training of Y+B-, B+C-, C+D-///E+F-, F+G-, and G+X- (where Y and X refer to 10 stimuli, respectively, that alternated across trials). These findings are discussed in the context of operant stimulus control by offering an approach based in stimulus B typically acquiring only a select stimulus control topography.


Asunto(s)
Comunicación no Verbal/fisiología , Transferencia de Experiencia en Psicología/fisiología , Estudios de Cohortes , Condicionamiento Operante/fisiología , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Comunicación no Verbal/psicología , Estimulación Luminosa , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Recompensa , Adulto Joven
15.
J Vis Exp ; (107)2016 Jan 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26779747

RESUMEN

Delay discounting refers to a decline in the value of a reward when it is delayed relative to when it is immediately available. Delay discounting tasks are used to identify indifference points, which reflect equal preference for two dichotomous reward alternatives differing in both delay and magnitude. Indifference points are key to assessing the shape of a delay-discounting gradient because they allow us to isolate the effect of delay on value. For example, if at a 1 week delay and a maximum of $1,000, the indifference point is at $700 we know that, for that participant, a 1-week delay corresponds to a 30% reduction in value. This video outlines an adjusting amount delay-discounting task that identifies indifference points relatively quickly and is inexpensive and easy to administer. Once data have been collected, non-linear regression techniques are typically used to generate discounting curves. The steepness of the discounting curve reflects the degree of impulsive choice of a group or individual. These techniques have been used with a wide range of commodities and have identified populations that are relatively impulsive. For example, people with substance abuse problems discount delayed rewards more steeply than control participants. Although degree of discounting varies as a function of the commodity examined, discounting of one commodity correlates with discounting of other commodities, which suggests that discounting may be a persistent pattern of behavior(1).


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Descuento por Demora/fisiología , Recompensa , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos
16.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 104(3): 252-73, 2015 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26458865

RESUMEN

There is disagreement about how to characterize the environment-behavior relations involved in the reinforcement of behavioral variability. The present research examined some of these issues using food-maintained, 4-peck sequences in pigeons. Experiment 1 evaluated the claim that behavioral variability is not reinforced directly but, rather, is the byproduct of changing over within sequences. Considerably higher levels of behavioral variation occurred under a relative-frequency threshold contingency than under a contingency that required a changeover but not variability per se. These results are consistent with the argument that behavioral variability is reinforced directly. Experiment 2 assessed the effects on variation levels of manipulating inter-trial and inter-response intervals. Variability increased with longer inter-response intervals but not with longer inter-trial intervals. These results are consistent with multiple explanations, including the notion that remembering past behavior interferes with the emission of reinforced variation. Consequently, Experiment 3 examined more directly the relation between remembering and reinforced variation. Variation levels were not affected by a concurrent contingency that encouraged pigeons to remember their past behavior. The implications of this research are presented in the context of working towards an understanding of the environment-behavior relations involved in the reinforcement of behavioral variability.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje por Asociación , Condicionamiento Operante , Discriminación en Psicología , Recuerdo Mental , Refuerzo en Psicología , Aprendizaje Seriado , Animales , Conducta Apetitiva , Columbidae , Humanos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA