Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 32
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Annu Rev Entomol ; 69: 481-501, 2024 Jan 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37788437

RESUMEN

Aquatic environments are an unusual habitat for most arthropods. Nevertheless, many arthropod species that were once terrestrial dwelling have transitioned back to marine and freshwater environments, either as semiaquatic or, more rarely, as fully aquatic inhabitants. Transition to water from land is exceptional, and without respiratory modifications to allow for extended submergence and the associated hypoxic conditions, survival is limited. In this article, we review marine-associated species that have made this rare transition in a generally terrestrial group, spiders. We include several freshwater spider species for comparative purposes. Marine-associated spiders comprise less than 0.3% of spider species worldwide but are found in over 14% of all spider families. As we discuss, these spiders live in environments that, with tidal action, hydraulic forces, and saltwater, are more extreme than freshwater habitats, often requiring physiological and behavioral adaptations to survive. Spiders employ many methods to survive inundation from encroaching tides, such as air bubble respiration, airtight nests, hypoxic comas, and fleeing incoming tides. While airway protection is the primary survival strategy, further survival adaptations include saltwater-induced osmotic regulation, dietary composition, predator avoidance, reproduction, locomotory responses, and adaptation to extreme temperatures and hydrostatic pressures that challenge existence in marine environments.


Asunto(s)
Artrópodos , Arañas , Humanos , Animales , Ecosistema , Hipoxia , Reproducción
2.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33885956

RESUMEN

Jumping spiders (Salticidae) are diurnal visual predators known for elaborate, vision-mediated behaviour achieved through the coordinated work of four pairs of camera-type eyes. One pair ('principal' eyes) is responsible for colour and high spatial acuity vision, while three pairs ('secondary' eyes) are mostly responsible for motion detection. Based on its unusual capacity to visually discriminate specific prey in very low, but also under bright light settings, we investigated the structure of the principal and one pair of secondary eyes (antero-lateral eyes) of Cyrba algerina to determine how these eyes achieve the sensitivity, while maintaining spatial acuity, needed to sustain behaviour in low light. Compared to salticids that live in bright light, the principal eyes of C. algerina have a short focal length, and wide contiguous twin rhabdomeres that support optical pooling, overall favouring sensitivity (0.39 µm2), but without fully compromising acuity (12.4 arc min). The antero-lateral eye retinae have large receptors surrounded by pigment granules, providing effective shielding from scattered light. These adaptations may be beneficial for a xeric salticid species with a 'blended' lifestyle: generally living and hunting under stones in the dark, but sometimes venturing above them, in dramatically different light conditions.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Locomoción , Células Fotorreceptoras de Invertebrados/fisiología , Conducta Predatoria , Conducta Espacial , Arañas/fisiología , Visión Ocular , Agudeza Visual , Adaptación Fisiológica , Animales , Femenino , Luz , Especificidad de la Especie
3.
Learn Behav ; 49(3): 276-291, 2021 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33443650

RESUMEN

Several non-mutually exclusive hypotheses have been proposed to explain the evolution of cognition in animals. Broadly, these hypotheses fall under two categories: those that pertain to the selective pressures exerted either by sociality or by the ecological niche in which animals live. We review these ideas and then discuss why the highly visual jumping spiders (Salticidae) are excellent models for investigating how cognitive ability evolves. With few exceptions, these behaviorally complex spiders are non-social, making them ideal candidates to explore ideas pertaining to selection based on habitat complexity and selection based on predatory behavior (foraging niche hypotheses). With the exception of Antarctica, salticids are found in all habitats on Earth, ranging from very complex to barren and simple. While many species are generalist predators, a minority also have specialized predatory behavior and prey specialization on dangerous prey, which has been proposed as an explanation for advanced cognitive ability. As this large group has a diversity of habitats in which it lives, diverse predatory behavior, as well as some "social" species, we argue that salticids are ideal candidates for comparative studies to explore the myriad selection factors acting upon a group well known for their cognitive prowess, despite having miniature brains.


Asunto(s)
Arañas , Animales , Cognición , Conducta Predatoria
4.
J Exp Biol ; 222(Pt 9)2019 05 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31019068

RESUMEN

Jumping spiders (family Salticidae) are known for their intricate vision-based behavior during encounters with prey and conspecific individuals. This is achieved using eyes specialized for discerning fine detail, but there has been minimal research on the capacity that salticids might have for visual performance under low ambient light levels. Here, we investigated the capacity of two salticid species, Cyrba algerina from Portugal and Cyrbaocellata from Kenya, to perform two specific visual tasks under low ambient light levels. We used lures made from spiders and midges in prey-identification experiments and mirror images (virtual conspecifics) in rival-identification experiments. These experiments were implemented under a range of ambient light levels (234, 1.35, 0.54, 0.24 cd m-2). In most instances, Calgerina and Cocellata were proficient at performing both of these visual tasks when ambient light was 234 and 1.35 cd m-2, and a minority performed these tasks at 0.54 cd m-2, but none succeeded when the light level was 0.24 cd m-2Cyrbaalgerina and C. ocellata showed vision-based discrimination under low ambient light levels previously associated with nocturnal species.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Predatoria , Arañas/fisiología , Percepción Visual , Animales , Kenia , Portugal , Visión Ocular
5.
Naturwissenschaften ; 106(5-6): 17, 2019 Apr 24.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31020391

RESUMEN

Light-based stimuli elicited acoustic responses in male Hylesinus aculeatus Say (Curculionidae: Scolytinae: Hylesinina) instantaneously, with 100% reliability. Stridulations were elicited with a white light beam in a dark environment and recorded with an ultrasonic microphone. Acoustic responses were consistent, and, when compared with sounds produced under stressful conditions (i.e. physical stimulation), no significant differences were found. Hylesinus aculeatus possess an elytro-tergal stridulatory organ and acoustic communication is only present in males. This is also the first report of acoustic communication for this species. Instantaneous light-elicited acoustic communication has potential applications in the development of electronic traps and real-time acoustic detection and identification of beetles, border biosecurity, and noise-reduction in acoustic data collection.


Asunto(s)
Luz , Vocalización Animal/fisiología , Gorgojos/fisiología , Animales , Masculino
6.
Anim Cogn ; 21(6): 787-794, 2018 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30167926

RESUMEN

The inability to maintain signal detection performance with time on task, or vigilance decrement, is widely studied in people because of its profound implications on attention-demanding tasks over sustained periods of time (e.g., air-traffic control). According to the resource depletion (overload) theory, a faster decrement is expected in tasks that are cognitively demanding or overstimulating, while the underload theory predicts steeper decrements in tasks that provide too little cognitive load, or understimulation. Using Trite planiceps, a jumping spider which is an active visual hunter, we investigated vigilance decrement to repetitive visual stimuli. Spiders were tethered in front of two stimulus presentation monitors and were given a polystyrene ball to hold. Movement of this ball indicates an attempt to turn towards a visual stimulus presented to a pair of laterally facing (anterior lateral) eyes for closer investigation with high acuity forward-facing (anterior median) eyes. Vigilance decrement is easily measured, as moving visual stimuli trigger clear optokinetic responses. We manipulated task difficulty by varying the contrast of the stimulus and the degree of 'noise' displayed on the screen over which the stimulus moved, thus affecting the signal:noise ratio. Additionally, we manipulated motivation by paired testing of hungry and sated spiders. All factors affected the vigilance decrement, but the key variable affecting decrement was stimulus contrast. Spiders exhibited a steeper decrement in the harder tasks, aligning with the resource depletion theory.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Arañas/fisiología , Animales , Conducta Apetitiva/fisiología
7.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 140(5): 3739, 2016 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27908073

RESUMEN

Vocal communication requires the sender to produce a sound, which transmits through the environment and is perceived by the receiver. Perception is dependent on the quality of the received signal and the receiver's frequency and amplitude sensitivity; hearing sensitivity of animals can be tested using behavioural detection tasks, showing the physical limitations of sound perception. Kea parrots (Nestor notabilis) were tested for their ability to hear sounds that varied in terms of both frequency and amplitude by means of a simple auditory detection task. Audiograms for three kea were similar, with the region of highest sensitivity (1-5 kHz) corresponding to the frequency of the highest amplitude in kea calls. Compared with other parrots and other bird taxa, the overall shape of the kea audiogram follows a similar pattern. However, two potentially interesting differences to the audiograms of other birds were found: an increase in sensitivity at approximately 12 kHz and a decreased sensitivity to frequencies below 1 kHz.


Asunto(s)
Loros , Animales , Vocalización Animal
8.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 15532, 2024 Jul 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38969671

RESUMEN

Acoustic communication is widespread in beetles, is often sexually dimorphic, and plays a significant role in behaviours such as premating recognition, courtship, and copulation. However, the factors that determine the presence or absence of acoustic signalling in a given species remain unclear. We examined acoustic communication in bark beetles (Scolytinae) and pinhole borers (Platypodinae), which are two speciose groups with widespread sound production capabilities. We show that body size along with the sequence of host colonisation predict the presence of acoustic communication, and report, for the first time in the animal kingdom, a size limit-1.9 mm-below which acoustic signalling ceases to be present.


Asunto(s)
Tamaño Corporal , Escarabajos , Animales , Escarabajos/fisiología , Comunicación Animal , Acústica , Femenino , Masculino , Vocalización Animal/fisiología
9.
Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc ; 98(5): 1548-1563, 2023 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37127535

RESUMEN

The nature and evolution of positive emotion is a major question remaining unanswered in science and philosophy. The study of feelings and emotions in humans and animals is dominated by discussion of affective states that have negative valence. Given the clinical and social significance of negative affect, such as depression, it is unsurprising that these emotions have received more attention from scientists. Compared to negative emotions, such as fear that leads to fleeing or avoidance, positive emotions are less likely to result in specific, identifiable, behaviours being expressed by an animal. This makes it particularly challenging to quantify and study positive affect. However, bursts of intense positive emotion (joy) are more likely to be accompanied by externally visible markers, like vocalisations or movement patterns, which make it more amenable to scientific study and more resilient to concerns about anthropomorphism. We define joy as intense, brief, and event-driven (i.e. a response to something), which permits investigation into how animals react to a variety of situations that would provoke joy in humans. This means that behavioural correlates of joy are measurable, either through newly discovered 'laughter' vocalisations, increases in play behaviour, or reactions to cognitive bias tests that can be used across species. There are a range of potential situations that cause joy in humans that have not been studied in other animals, such as whether animals feel joy on sunny days, when they accomplish a difficult feat, or when they are reunited with a familiar companion after a prolonged absence. Observations of species-specific calls and play behaviour can be combined with biometric markers and reactions to ambiguous stimuli in order to enable comparisons of affect between phylogenetically distant taxonomic groups. Identifying positive affect is also important for animal welfare because knowledge of positive emotional states would allow us to monitor animal well-being better. Additionally, measuring if phylogenetically and ecologically distant animals play more, laugh more, or act more optimistically after certain kinds of experiences will also provide insight into the mechanisms underlying the evolution of joy and other positive emotions, and potentially even into the evolution of consciousness.


Asunto(s)
Bienestar del Animal , Emociones , Animales , Emociones/fisiología
10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22457074

RESUMEN

The modular visual system of jumping spiders (Salticidae) divides characteristics such as high spatial acuity and wide-field motion detection between different pairs of eyes. A large pair of telescope-like anterior-median (AM) eyes is supported by 2-3 pairs of 'secondary' eyes, which provide almost 360 degrees of visual coverage at lower resolution. The AM retinae are moveable and can be pointed at stimuli within their range of motion, but salticids have to turn to bring targets into this frontal zone in the first place. We describe how the front-facing pair of secondary eyes (anterior lateral, AL) mediates this through a series of whole-body 'tracking saccades' in response to computer-generated stimuli. We investigated the 'response area' of the AL eyes and show a clear correspondence between the physical margins of the retina and stimulus position at the onset of the first saccade. Saccade frequency is maximal at the margin of AL and AM fields of view. Furthermore, spiders markedly increase the velocity with which higher magnitude tracking saccades are carried out. This has the effect that the time during which vision is impaired due to motion blur is kept at an almost constant low level, even during saccades of large magnitude.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Ojo , Percepción de Movimiento , Retina/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos , Arañas/fisiología , Animales , Ojo/anatomía & histología , Femenino , Estimulación Luminosa , Retina/anatomía & histología , Arañas/anatomía & histología , Factores de Tiempo
11.
J Exp Biol ; 215(Pt 13): 2255-61, 2012 Jul 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22675186

RESUMEN

Evarcha culicivora is an East African jumping spider that feeds indirectly on vertebrate blood by choosing blood-fed female Anopheles mosquitoes as prey. Previous studies have shown that this predator can identify its preferred prey even when restricted to using only visual cues. Here, we used lures and virtual mosquitoes to investigate the optical cues underlying this predator's prey-choice behaviour. We made lures by dissecting and then reconstructing dead mosquitoes, combining the head plus thorax with different abdomens. Depending on the experiment, lures were either moving or motionless. Findings from the lure experiments suggested that, for E. culicivora, seeing a blood-fed female mosquito's abdomen on a lure was a necessary, but not sufficient, cue by which preferred prey was identified, as cues from the abdomen needed to be paired with cues from the head and thorax of a mosquito. Conversely, when abdomens were not visible or were identical, spiders based their decisions on the appearance of the head plus thorax of mosquitoes, choosing prey with female characteristics. Findings from a subsequent experiment using animated 3D virtual mosquitoes suggest that it is specifically the mosquito's antennae that influence E. culicivora's prey-choice decisions. Our results show that E. culicivora uses a complex process for prey classification.


Asunto(s)
Culicidae/anatomía & histología , Conducta Predatoria , Arañas/fisiología , Animales , Anopheles/anatomía & histología , Señales (Psicología) , Culex/anatomía & histología , Conducta Alimentaria , Femenino , Masculino
12.
Anim Cogn ; 15(4): 699-710, 2012 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22526693

RESUMEN

Although a wide range of vertebrates have been considered in research on numerical competence, little is known about the role of number-related decisions in the predatory strategies of invertebrates. Here, we investigate how numerical competence is expressed in a highly specialized predatory strategy adopted by the small juveniles of Portia africana when practicing communal predation, with the prey being another spider, Oecobius amboseli. Two or more P. africana juveniles sometimes settle by the same oecobiid nest and then share the meal after one individual captures the oecobiid. Experiments were designed to clarify how these predators use number-related cues in conjunction with non-numerical cues when deciding whether to settle at a nest. We used lures (dead spiders positioned in lifelike posture) arranged in a series of 24 different scenes defined by the type, configuration and especially number of lures. On the whole, our findings suggest that P. africana juveniles base settling decisions on the specific number of already settled conspecific juveniles at the nest and express a preference for settling when the number is one instead of zero, two or three. By varying the size of the already settled juveniles and their positions around the nest, we show that factors related to continuous variables and stimulus configuration are unlikely explanations for our findings.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Predatoria , Arañas , Animales , Toma de Decisiones , Femenino , Conducta Predatoria/fisiología , Percepción del Tamaño , Conducta Social , Arañas/fisiología
13.
Anim Cogn ; 15(4): 461-71, 2012 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22286279

RESUMEN

Communal predators may often need to make especially intricate foraging decisions, as a predator's success may depend on the actions of its neighbours. Here,we consider the decisions made by Portia africana, a jumping spider (Salticidae) that preys on other spiders, including Oecobius amboseli (Oecobiidae), a small prey spider that lives under small sheets of silk (nests) on the walls of buildings. P. africana juveniles settle near oecobiid nests and then ambush oecobiids as they leave or enter the nest. Two or more P. africana juveniles sometimes settle at the same nest and, when an oecobiid is captured, the P. africana juveniles may share the meal. We investigated the joining decisions made by naïve P. africana juveniles. Experiments were based on using lures (dead spiders positioned in lifelike posture) arranged in a series of 17 different scenes defined by the presence/absence of a nest, the lure types present and the configuration of the lures and the nest. Our findings imply that P. africana juveniles make remarkably precise predatory decisions, with the variables that matter including whether a nest is present, the identity of spiders inside and outside a nest and how spiders are positioned relative to each other and the nest.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Predatoria , Arañas , Animales , Femenino , Conducta Predatoria/fisiología , Conducta Social , Arañas/fisiología
14.
Conserv Biol ; 26(1): 142-9, 2012 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22010952

RESUMEN

Agricultural landscapes are becoming an important focus of animal conservation, although initiatives to conserve predators to date have rarely provided economic benefits to agricultural producers. We examined whether introduction to vineyards of the New Zealand Falcon (Falco novaeseelandiae), a species listed as threatened by the New Zealand Department of Conservation, affected the abundance of 4 species of Passeriformes that are considered vineyard pests or affected the amount of economic loss due to grape (Vitis vinifera) damage. Three of the species were introduced and remove whole grapes from bunches (Blackbird [Turdus merula], Song Thrush [Turdus philomelos], and Starling [Sturnus vulgaris]), whereas the one native species (Silvereye [Zosterops lateralis]) pecks holes in grapes. The introduction of falcons to vineyards was associated with a significant decrease in the abundance of introduced passerines and with a 95% reduction in the number of grapes removed relative to vineyards without falcons. Falcon presence was not associated with a change in the number of Silvereyes, but there was a 55% reduction in the number of grapes pecked in vineyards with falcons. Our results indicate that, relative to damage in vineyards without falcons, the presence of a falcon could potentially result in savings of US$234/ha for the Sauvignon Blanc variety of grapes and $326/ha for Pinot Noir variety of grapes.


Asunto(s)
Conservación de los Recursos Naturales/métodos , Especies en Peligro de Extinción , Falconiformes/fisiología , Passeriformes/fisiología , Vitis , Animales , Agentes de Control Biológico , Nueva Zelanda , Densidad de Población , Dinámica Poblacional
15.
J Chem Ecol ; 38(9): 1081-92, 2012 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22914959

RESUMEN

Evarcha culicivora, an East African jumping spider (Salticidae), is the only spider for which there is evidence of innate olfactory affinity for particular plant species. Evarcha culicivora also actively chooses as preferred prey the females of Anopheles mosquitoes, and both sexes of Anopheles are known to visit plants for nectar meals. Here, we identified compounds present in the headspace of one of these species in Kenya, Lantana camara, and then used 11 of these compounds in olfactometer experiments. Our findings show that three terpenes [(E)-ß-caryophyllene, α-humulene and 1,8 cineole] can be discriminated by, and are salient to, E. culicivora. The spiders experienced no prior training with plants or the compounds we used. This is the first experimental demonstration of specific phytochemicals being innately attractive to a spider, a group normally characterized as predators.


Asunto(s)
Lantana/química , Arañas/efectos de los fármacos , Compuestos Orgánicos Volátiles/análisis , Compuestos Orgánicos Volátiles/farmacología , Animales , Señales (Psicología) , Ambiente , Femenino , Masculino
16.
Behav Processes ; 196: 104603, 2022 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35131360

RESUMEN

Jumping spiders (Salticidae) use exceptional vision, largely mediated by their forward-facing anterior lateral (AL) and anterior medial (AM) eyes, to pounce on prey from a distance. We evaluated depth perception through the use of 'texture density' (depth estimation through surface texture comparisons, with greater distances having higher textural density) in the salticid Trite planiceps. In visual cliff experiments, spiders tended to choose an area with a false 'low drop' over a false 'high drop' with the same texture densities, but showed no preference for either area when presented with substrates with different texture densities at a constant height. This was corroborated when T. planiceps did not avoid jumping over an illusion resembling a trench compared to a no-illusion control pattern. We then selectively occluded both AL and 1 AM eye (monocular treatment), both AL eyes (ALE-occluded binocular treatment) or no eyes (control), and induced spiders to jump across a gap at different heights. Neither control spiders, spiders with binocular cues from the AM eyes or monocular treatment spiders exhibited a height preference. These results suggest that while T. planiceps accurately perceives depth, it does not appear to rely on texture gradients as a depth cue.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones , Arañas , Animales , Señales (Psicología) , Visión Ocular
17.
R Soc Open Sci ; 9(7): 220217, 2022 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35911201

RESUMEN

The propagation of animal vocalizations in water and in air is a well-studied phenomenon, but sound produced by bark and wood-boring insects, which feed and reproduce inside trees, is poorly understood. Often being confined to the dark and chemically saturated habitat of wood, many bark- and woodborers have developed stridulatory mechanisms to communicate acoustically. Despite their ecological and economic importance and the unusual medium used for acoustic communication, very little is known about sound production in these insects, or their acoustic interactions inside trees. Here, we use bark beetles (Scolytinae) as a model system to study the effects of wooden tissue on the propagation of insect stridulations and propose algorithms for their automatic identification. We characterize distance dependence of the spectral parameters of stridulatory sounds, propose data-based models for the power decay of the stridulations in both outer and inner bark, provide optimal spectral ranges for stridulation detectability and develop automatic methods for their detection and identification. We also discuss the acoustic discernibility of species cohabitating the same log. The species tested can be acoustically identified with 99% of accuracy at distances up to 20 cm and detected to the greatest extent in the 2-6 kHz frequency band. Phloem was a better medium for sound transmission than bark.

18.
J Exp Biol ; 213(Pt 14): 2372-8, 2010 Jul 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20581266

RESUMEN

Jumping spiders, or salticids, sample their environment using a combination of two types of eyes. The forward-facing pair of 'principal' eyes have narrow fields of view, but exceptional spatial resolution, while the two or three pairs of 'secondary' eyes have wide fields of view and function especially well as motion analysers. Motion detected by the secondary eyes may elicit an orienting response, whereupon the object of interest is examined further using the high-acuity principal eyes. The anterior lateral (AL) eyes are particularly interesting, as they are the only forward-facing pair of secondary eyes. In this study, we aimed to determine characteristics of stimuli that elicit orienting responses mediated by the AL eyes. After covering all eyes except the AL eyes, we measured orienting responses to dot stimuli that varied in size and contrast, and moved at different speeds. We found that all stimulus parameters had significant effects on orientation propensity. When tethered flies were used as prey, we found that visual information from the AL eyes alone was sufficient to elicit stalking behaviour. These results suggest that, in terms of overall visual processing, the relevance of spatial vision in the AL eyes has been underestimated in the literature. Our results also show that female spiders are significantly more responsive than males. We found that hunger caused similar increases in orientation propensity in the two sexes, but females responded more often than males both when sated and when hungry. A higher propensity by females to orient toward moving objects may be related to females tending to experience higher nutritional demands than males.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Conducta Predatoria/fisiología , Arañas/fisiología , Animales , Femenino , Hambre/fisiología , Masculino , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Células Fotorreceptoras de Invertebrados/fisiología , Caracteres Sexuales , Arañas/anatomía & histología , Percepción Visual
19.
J Insect Sci ; 10: 82, 2010.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20673067

RESUMEN

The prey choice behavior and predatory strategies of two East African assassin bugs, Scipinnia repax (Stäl 1961) and Nagusta sp. (Hemiptera: Reduviidae), were investigated in the field and the laboratory. Both of these species are from the subfamily Harpactorinae and specialize in eating spiders. They prey especially often on social jumping spiders (Salticidae) that build nest complexes (nests connected by silk) in vegetation near the shoreline of Lake Victoria. Both reduviid species associate with these nest complexes and prey on the resident salticids. Nagusta sp., but not S. repax, form groups on nest complexes with 2-3 individuals of Nagusta sometimes feeding together on a single salticid. In addition to social salticids, Nagusta sp. preys on Portia africana, an araneophagic salticid that often invades the same nest complexes. S. repax preys on salticid eggs and also on Nagusta. Although they avoid ants, Nagusta and especially S. repax prey on ant-mimicking salticids, suggesting that sensory modalities other than vision play a dominant role in prey detection.


Asunto(s)
Hemípteros/fisiología , Conducta Predatoria/fisiología , Animales , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Dieta , Femenino , Masculino , Comportamiento de Nidificación , Densidad de Población , Arañas/fisiología
20.
Curr Zool ; 66(3): 263-273, 2020 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32440287

RESUMEN

To take an indirect route (detour) in order to reach a specific target requires complex cognitive processes. Yet more demanding, from the cognitive point of view, is when the goal is only visible at the beginning of the detour. In spiders from the family Salticidae, vision is a key sensory modality mediating navigation and prey search. Their acute vision allows them to perform complicated detours, possibly as a consequence of the multitude of potential routes in their typically complex 3-dimensional habitats. We used a 4-route choice test, in which routes differed in being either short or long and in the presence or absence of a lure of a prey item, to investigate route assessment in 2 salticid species, Trite planiceps and Marpissa marina. Although both species showed evidence of motivation to follow lured-routes, judging by the number of times they re-oriented toward them while detouring, we found that Trite chose short routes in preference to long routes, but did not prefer the lured-routes. In contrast, Marpissa exhibited random route choice, although it oriented toward lured-routes more often than control routes (lure absent). Our results suggest that decision-making processes about which route to take occurs before embarking on a route, but this is cognitively challenging. Spiders exhibited cognitive limitations in which the lack of visibility of the goal affected success. However, the severity of cognitive limitations depended on species. We suggest that variability in spatial ability across the Salticidae may be related to the habitat complexity inhabited by each species.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA