Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 33
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Dev Sci ; 25(2): e13159, 2022 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34240533

RESUMEN

Studies of reward effects on behavior in adolescence typically rely on performance metrics that confound myriad cognitive and non-cognitive processes, making it challenging to determine which process is impacted by reward. The present longitudinal study applied the diffusion decision model to a reward task to isolate the influence of reward on response caution from influences of processing and motor speed. Participants completed three annual assessments from early to middle adolescence (N = 387, 55% female, Mage  = 12.1 at Wave 1; Mage  = 13.1 at Wave 2, Mage  = 14.1 at Wave 3) and three annual assessments in late adolescence (Mages  = 17.8, 18.9, 19.9). At each assessment, participants completed a two-choice reaction time task under conditions of no-reward and a block in which points were awarded for speeded accuracy. Reward reduced response caution at all waves, as expected, but had a greater impact as teens moved from early to middle adolescence. Simulations to identify optimal response caution showed that teens were overly cautious in early adolescence but became too focused on speed over accuracy by middle adolescence. By late adolescence, participants adopted response styles that maximized reward. Further, response style was associated with both internalizing and externalizing symptoms in early-to-middle adolescence, providing evidence for the construct validity of a diffusion model approach in this developmental period.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones , Recompensa , Adolescente , Simulación por Computador , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 115(11): 2607-2612, 2018 03 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29531092

RESUMEN

We describe and demonstrate an empirical strategy useful for discovering and replicating empirical effects in psychological science. The method involves the design of a metastudy, in which many independent experimental variables-that may be moderators of an empirical effect-are indiscriminately randomized. Radical randomization yields rich datasets that can be used to test the robustness of an empirical claim to some of the vagaries and idiosyncrasies of experimental protocols and enhances the generalizability of these claims. The strategy is made feasible by advances in hierarchical Bayesian modeling that allow for the pooling of information across unlike experiments and designs and is proposed here as a gold standard for replication research and exploratory research. The practical feasibility of the strategy is demonstrated with a replication of a study on subliminal priming.


Asunto(s)
Investigación Biomédica/normas , Proyectos de Investigación/normas , Teorema de Bayes , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Humanos , Distribución Aleatoria
3.
Psychol Res ; 83(6): 1172-1183, 2019 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29181583

RESUMEN

The goal of this study was to replicate findings of diffusion model parameters capturing emotion effects in a lexical decision task and investigating whether these findings extend to other tasks of implicit emotion processing. Additionally, we were interested in the stability of diffusion model parameters across emotional stimuli and tasks for individual subjects. Responses to words in a lexical decision task were compared with responses to faces in a gender categorization task for stimuli of the emotion categories: happy, neutral and fear. Main effects of emotion as well as stability of emerging response style patterns as evident in diffusion model parameters across these tasks were analyzed. Based on earlier findings, drift rates were assumed to be more similar in response to stimuli of the same emotion category compared to stimuli of a different emotion category. Results showed that emotion effects of the tasks differed with a processing advantage for happy followed by neutral and fear-related words in the lexical decision task and a processing advantage for neutral followed by happy and fearful faces in the gender categorization task. Both emotion effects were captured in estimated drift rate parameters-and in case of the lexical decision task also in the non-decision time parameters. A principal component analysis showed that contrary to our hypothesis drift rates were more similar within a specific task context than within a specific emotion category. Individual response patterns of subjects across tasks were evident in significant correlations regarding diffusion model parameters including response styles, non-decision times and information accumulation.


Asunto(s)
Emociones/fisiología , Individualidad , Adolescente , Adulto , Toma de Decisiones , Femenino , Alemania , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Teóricos , Adulto Joven
4.
Appetite ; 128: 167-179, 2018 09 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29890186

RESUMEN

The reward surfeit model of overeating suggests that heightened brain response to rewards contributes to overeating and subsequent weight gain. However, previous studies have not tested whether brain response to reward is associated with food intake, particularly during childhood, a period of dynamic development in reward and inhibitory control neurocircuitry. We conducted functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with 7-11-year-old children (n = 59; healthy weight, n = 31; overweight, n = 28; 54% female) while they played a modified card-guessing paradigm to examine blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) response to anticipating and winning rewards (food, money, neutral). Food intake was assessed at three separate meals that measured different facets of eating behavior: 1) typical consumption (baseline), 2) overindulgence (palatable buffet), and 3) eating in the absence of hunger (EAH). A priori regions of interest included regions implicated in both reward processing and inhibitory control. Multiple stepwise regressions were conducted to examine the relationship between intake and BOLD response to rewards. Corrected results showed that a greater BOLD response in the medial prefrontal cortex for anticipating food compared to money positively correlated with how much children ate at the baseline and palatable buffet meals. BOLD response in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex for winning food compared to money was positively correlated with intake at the palatable buffet meal and EAH. All aforementioned relationships were independent of child weight status. Findings support the reward surfeit model by showing that increased brain response to food compared to money rewards positively correlates with laboratory measures of food intake in children.


Asunto(s)
Ingestión de Alimentos/psicología , Hiperfagia/psicología , Comidas/psicología , Obesidad Infantil/psicología , Recompensa , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Hiperfagia/diagnóstico por imagen , Hiperfagia/fisiopatología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Obesidad Infantil/diagnóstico por imagen , Obesidad Infantil/fisiopatología , Corteza Prefrontal/diagnóstico por imagen , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiopatología , Análisis de Regresión , Aumento de Peso
5.
Cogn Emot ; 32(2): 249-258, 2018 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28271732

RESUMEN

There is great interest in understanding whether and how mood influences affective processing. Results in the literature have been mixed: some studies show mood-congruent processing but others do not. One limitation of previous work is that decision components for affective processing and responses biases are not dissociated. The present study explored the roles of affective processing and response biases using a drift-diffusion model (DDM) of simple choice. In two experiments, participants decided if words were emotionally positive or negative while listening to music that induced positive or negative mood. The behavioural results showed weak, inconsistent mood-congruency effects. In contrast, the DDM showed consistent effects that were selectively driven by an a-priori bias in response expectation, suggesting that music-induced mood influences expectations about the emotionality of upcoming stimuli, but not the emotionality of the stimuli themselves. Implications for future studies of emotional classification and mood are subsequently discussed.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Afecto/fisiología , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Emociones/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Música/psicología , Estudiantes/psicología , Adulto Joven
6.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 38(11): 5616-5627, 2017 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28758287

RESUMEN

This study was designed to explore the electrophysiological correlates of the diffusion models drift rate parameter in cognitive decision making. Eighty-two participants completed a lexical decision task while their stimulus-dependent event-related potentials (ERP) and theta frequency band power were measured. A mass univariate approach was applied to examine the timeline at which correlations were evident. Individual differences in drift rate parameter and condition-wise within-subject differences in drift rates for word emotionality and item repetition were found to be related to amplitude differences in the late positive complex (LPC). No relations to theta frequency band power changes were obtained. The drift rate parameter captures information accumulation of noisy evidence, while LPC amplitudes are discussed to reflect the strength of a memory trace. While these results point to a common underlying cognitive mechanism to explain drift rates and LPC modulation, they also provide a new angle on the timeline of visual word processing following word identification. Further confirmations of the results are needed to approve the LPC as neurophysiological marker of information accumulation. Hum Brain Mapp 38:5616-5627, 2017. © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Modelos Neurológicos , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Lectura , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Difusión , Potenciales Evocados , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto Joven
7.
Cogn Emot ; 30(8): 1446-1460, 2016 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26243532

RESUMEN

Individuals with high anxiety show bias for threatening information, but it is unclear whether this bias affects memory. Recognition memory studies have shown biases for recognising and rejecting threatening items in anxiety, prompting the need to identify moderating factors of this effect. This study focuses on the role of semantic similarity: the use of many semantically related threatening words could increase familiarity for those items and obscure anxiety-related differences in memory. To test this, two recognition memory experiments varied the proportion of threatening words in lists to manipulate the semantic-similarity effects. When similarity effects were reduced, participants with high trait anxiety were biased to respond "new" to threatening words, whereas when similarity effects were strong there was no effect of anxiety on memory bias. Analysis of the data with the drift diffusion model showed that the bias was due to differences in processing of the threatening stimuli rather than a simple response bias. These data suggest that the semantic similarity of the threatening words significantly affects the presence or absence of anxiety-related threat bias in recognition memory. The results indicate that trait anxiety is associated with a bias to decide that threatening stimuli were not previously studied, but only when semantic-similarity effects are controlled. Implications for theories of anxiety and future studies in this domain are discussed.

8.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 26(8): 1601-14, 2014 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24405185

RESUMEN

The stop-signal task, in which participants must inhibit prepotent responses, has been used to identify neural systems that vary with individual differences in inhibitory control. To explore how these differences relate to other aspects of decision making, a drift-diffusion model of simple decisions was fitted to stop-signal task data from go trials to extract measures of caution, motor execution time, and stimulus processing speed for each of 123 participants. These values were used to probe fMRI data to explore individual differences in neural activation. Faster processing of the go stimulus correlated with greater activation in the right frontal pole for both go and stop trials. On stop trials, stimulus processing speed also correlated with regions implicated in inhibitory control, including the right inferior frontal gyrus, medial frontal gyrus, and BG. Individual differences in motor execution time correlated with activation of the right parietal cortex. These findings suggest a robust relationship between the speed of stimulus processing and inhibitory processing at the neural level. This model-based approach provides novel insight into the interrelationships among decision components involved in inhibitory control and raises interesting questions about strategic adjustments in performance and inhibitory deficits associated with psychopathology.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Neuroimagen Funcional/métodos , Individualidad , Inhibición Psicológica , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Persona de Mediana Edad , Modelos Psicológicos , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Adulto Joven
9.
Cogn Emot ; 28(5): 867-80, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24303902

RESUMEN

Recognition memory studies often find that emotional items are more likely than neutral items to be labelled as studied. Previous work suggests this bias is driven by increased memory strength/familiarity for emotional items. We explored strength and bias interpretations of this effect with the conjecture that emotional stimuli might seem more familiar because they share features with studied items from the same category. Categorical effects were manipulated in a recognition task by presenting lists with a small, medium or large proportion of emotional words. The liberal memory bias for emotional words was only observed when a medium or large proportion of categorised words were presented in the lists. Similar, though weaker, effects were observed with categorised words that were not emotional (animal names). These results suggest that liberal memory bias for emotional items may be largely driven by effects of category membership.


Asunto(s)
Emociones/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Análisis de Varianza , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Humanos , Curva ROC , Estudiantes/psicología , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
10.
J Neurosci ; 32(47): 16716-24, 2012 Nov 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23175825

RESUMEN

A critical component of decision making is the ability to adjust criteria for classifying stimuli. fMRI and drift diffusion models were used to explore the neural representations of perceptual criteria in decision making. The specific focus was on the relative engagement of perceptual- and decision-related neural systems in response to adjustments in perceptual criteria. Human participants classified visual stimuli as big or small based on criteria of different sizes, which effectively biased their choices toward one response over the other. A drift diffusion model was fit to the behavioral data to extract estimates of stimulus size, criterion size, and difficulty for each participant and condition. These parameter values were used as modulated regressors to create a highly constrained model for the fMRI analysis that accounted for several components of the decision process. The results show that perceptual criteria values were reflected by activity in left inferior temporal cortex, a region known to represent objects and their physical properties, whereas stimulus size was reflected by activation in occipital cortex. A frontoparietal network of regions, including dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and superior parietal lobule, corresponded to the decision variables resulting from the downstream stimulus-criterion comparison, independent of stimulus type. The results provide novel evidence that perceptual criteria are represented in stimulus space and serve as inputs to be compared with the presented stimulus, recruiting a common network of decision regions shown to be active in other simple decisions. This work advances our understanding of the neural correlates of decision flexibility and adjustments of behavioral bias.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Percepción/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Toma de Decisiones , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Modelos Lineales , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos , Estimulación Luminosa , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
11.
Mem Cognit ; 40(8): 1189-99, 2012 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22736423

RESUMEN

Criss (Cognitive Psychology 59:297-319, 2009) reported that subjective ratings of memory strength showed a mirror effect pattern in which strengthening the studied words increased ratings for targets and decreased ratings for lures. She interpreted the effect on lure items as evidence for differentiation, a process whereby lures produce a poorer match to strong than to weak memory traces. However, she also noted that participants might use different mappings between memory evidence and levels of the rating scale when they expected strong versus weak targets; that is, the effect might be produced by decision processes rather than differentiation. We report two experiments designed to distinguish these accounts. Some participants studied pure lists of weak or strong items (presented once or five times, respectively), while others studied mixed lists of half weak and half strong items. The participants from both groups had pure-strength tests: Only strong or only weak items were tested, and the participants were informed of which it would be before the test. The results showed that strength ratings for lures were lower when strong versus weak targets were tested, regardless of whether the study list was pure or mixed. In the mixed-study condition, the effect was produced even after identical study lists, and thus the same degree of differentiation in the studied traces. Therefore, our results suggest that the strength-rating mirror effect is produced by changes in decision processes.


Asunto(s)
Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Adulto , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Distribución Aleatoria , Adulto Joven
12.
Cogn Psychol ; 63(4): 210-38, 2011 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21964663

RESUMEN

The present study tested diffusion models of processing in the flanker task, in which participants identify a target that is flanked by items that indicate the same (congruent) or opposite response (incongruent). Single- and dual-process flanker models were implemented in a diffusion-model framework and tested against data from experiments that manipulated response bias, speed/accuracy tradeoffs, attentional focus, and stimulus configuration. There was strong mimcry among the models, and each captured the main trends in the data for the standard conditions. However, when more complex conditions were used, a single-process spotlight model captured qualitative and quantitative patterns that the dual-process models could not. Since the single-process model provided the best balance of fit quality and parsimony, the results indicate that processing in the simple versions of the flanker task is better described by gradual rather than discrete narrowing of attention.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Humanos , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
13.
Front Aging Neurosci ; 11: 41, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30906257

RESUMEN

Hypertension accelerates brain aging, resulting in cognitive dysfunction with advancing age. Exercise is widely recommended for adults with hypertension to attenuate cognitive dysfunction. Whether acute exercise benefits cognitive function in this at-risk population is unknown. The purpose of this study was to compare the effects of acute aerobic exercise on cognitive function in 30 middle-aged hypertensive (HTN) and 30 age, sex, and body mass index (BMI)-matched non-HTN adults (56 ± 6 years, BMI 28.2 ± 2.9 kg/m2; 32 men). Subjects underwent cognitive testing pre/post 30-min cycling (≈55% peak oxygen consumption). Cognition was assessed using standard metrics of accuracy and reaction time (RT) across memory recognition, 2-back, and Flanker tasks. Behavioral data was further analyzed using drift-diffusion modeling to examine underlying components of decision-making (strength of evidence, caution, bias) and RT (non-decision time). Exercise elicited similar changes in cognitive function in both HTN and non-HTN groups (p > 0.05). Accuracy was unaltered for Flanker and 2-back tasks, while hits and false alarms increased for memory recognition post-exercise (p < 0.05). Modeling results indicated changes in memory hits/false alarms were due to significant changes in stimulus bias post-exercise. RT decreased for Flanker and memory recognition tasks and was driven by reductions in post-exercise non-decision time (p < 0.05). Our data indicate acute exercise resulted in similar, beneficial cognitive responses in both middle-age HTN and non-HTN adults, marked by unaltered task accuracy, and accelerated RT post-exercise. Additionally, drift-diffusion modeling revealed that beneficial acceleration of cognitive processing post-exercise (RT) is driven by changes in non-decision components (encoding/motor response) rather than the decision-making process itself.

14.
Physiol Behav ; 201: 139-145, 2019 03 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30611763

RESUMEN

High altitude sojourn is broadly associated with impaired cognitive function, although there are inconsistencies within the literature. Incorporation of mathematical modeling to gain insight into latent aspects of decision-making may strengthen the ability to characterize changes in cognitive function during high altitude sojourn. This study sought to examine the effects of high altitude on cognitive function and underlying constructs of decision-making during an 11-d incremental ascent to 5160 m in 18 healthy adults (26 ±â€¯12 yrs). Participants underwent cognitive testing at 116 m, 3440 m, 4240 m, and 5160 m. Cognitive function was assessed using standard metrics of accuracy and reaction time (RT) during working memory (2-back) and attention (Flanker) tasks. Behavioral data were additionally analyzed using drift-diffusion modeling to interrogate latent neural (strength of evidence, non-decision time) and behavioral (caution, bias) processes of decision-making. Flanker accuracy was unaltered during incremental ascent to high altitude, while 2-back accuracy decreased at 5160 m (p < 0.01). RT was faster at 4240 m for the Flanker, and faster at all altitudes compared to 116 m for the 2-back (p < 0.01). Incremental ascent to high altitude elicited modest reductions in caution and non-decision time, increases in bias and strength of evidence for non-match items during the 2-back (0.04 ≥ p > 0.01). These data indicate that while RT may appear to improve during incremental ascent to high altitude, increases in speed may be driven by participants 1) accumulating less evidence before initiating a response (i.e., less cautious) and 2) preferentially attending to (more biased), and extracting more evidence from, frequent/easier stimuli, rather than improved processing per se. Taken together, changes in cognitive function during incremental ascent to high altitude may reflect subtle changes in neural and behavioral components of decision-making intended to reduce cognitive load and conserve brain resources under challenging environmental conditions.


Asunto(s)
Altitud , Cognición/fisiología , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Atención/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Caracteres Sexuales , Adulto Joven
15.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 26(4): 1051-1069, 2019 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29450793

RESUMEN

Most data analyses rely on models. To complement statistical models, psychologists have developed cognitive models, which translate observed variables into psychologically interesting constructs. Response time models, in particular, assume that response time and accuracy are the observed expression of latent variables including 1) ease of processing, 2) response caution, 3) response bias, and 4) non-decision time. Inferences about these psychological factors, hinge upon the validity of the models' parameters. Here, we use a blinded, collaborative approach to assess the validity of such model-based inferences. Seventeen teams of researchers analyzed the same 14 data sets. In each of these two-condition data sets, we manipulated properties of participants' behavior in a two-alternative forced choice task. The contributing teams were blind to the manipulations, and had to infer what aspect of behavior was changed using their method of choice. The contributors chose to employ a variety of models, estimation methods, and inference procedures. Our results show that, although conclusions were similar across different methods, these "modeler's degrees of freedom" did affect their inferences. Interestingly, many of the simpler approaches yielded as robust and accurate inferences as the more complex methods. We recommend that, in general, cognitive models become a typical analysis tool for response time data. In particular, we argue that the simpler models and procedures are sufficient for standard experimental designs. We finish by outlining situations in which more complicated models and methods may be necessary, and discuss potential pitfalls when interpreting the output from response time models.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Modelos Psicológicos , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Estadísticos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Método Simple Ciego
17.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 25(1): 286-301, 2018 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28357629

RESUMEN

Researchers and clinicians are interested in estimating individual differences in the ability to process conflicting information. Conflict processing is typically assessed by comparing behavioral measures like RTs or error rates from conflict tasks. However, these measures are hard to interpret because they can be influenced by additional processes like response caution or bias. This limitation can be circumvented by employing cognitive models to decompose behavioral data into components of underlying decision processes, providing better specificity for investigating individual differences. A new class of drift-diffusion models has been developed for conflict tasks, presenting a potential tool to improve analysis of individual differences in conflict processing. However, measures from these models have not been validated for use in experiments with limited data collection. The present study assessed the validity of these models with a parameter-recovery study to determine whether and under what circumstances the models provide valid measures of cognitive processing. Three models were tested: the dual-stage two-phase model (Hübner, Steinhauser, & Lehle, Psychological Review, 117(3), 759-784, 2010), the shrinking spotlight model (White, Ratcliff, & Starns, Cognitive Psychology, 63(4), 210-238, 2011), and the diffusion model for conflict tasks (Ulrich, Schröter, Leuthold, & Birngruber, Cogntive Psychology, 78, 148-174, 2015). The validity of the model parameters was assessed using different methods of fitting the data and different numbers of trials. The results show that each model has limitations in recovering valid parameters, but they can be mitigated by adding constraints to the model. Practical recommendations are provided for when and how each model can be used to analyze data and provide measures of processing in conflict tasks.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Cognición , Conflicto Psicológico , Tiempo de Reacción , Recolección de Datos , Toma de Decisiones , Humanos , Individualidad , Modelos Psicológicos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
18.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 14(5): 1007-9, 2007 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18087974

RESUMEN

Chen, Hale, and Myerson (2007) recently reported a test of the difference engine model (Myerson, Hale, Zheng, Jenkins, & Widaman, 2003). This test evaluated whether the standard deviation (SD) is proportional to the amount of processing-that is, mean reaction time (RT)--in a speeded cognitive task. We show that this evaluation is not a test of the model because its finding is a consequence of relationships in the data. We argue any model structure that produces increasing values of RT as a function of difficulty, with different slopes for different individuals, necessarily produces a correlation between SD and mean RT We illustrate this with a different model structure-that is, the diffusion model proposed by Ratcliff (1978)--showing that it produces a fan out between fast- and slow-group means and produces the correlation between SD and mean RT that matches the empirical result.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Pruebas Psicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción , Humanos
19.
J Abnorm Child Psychol ; 45(5): 911-920, 2017 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27787672

RESUMEN

The current studies utilized drift diffusion modeling (DDM) to examine how reinforcement and stimulant medication affect cognitive task performance in children with ADHD. In Study 1, children with (n = 25; 88 % male) and without ADHD (n = 33; 82 % male) completed a 2-choice discrimination task at baseline (100 trials) and again a week later under alternating reinforcement and no-reinforcement contingencies (400 trials total). In Study 2, participants with ADHD (n = 29; 72 % male) completed a double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of 0.3 and 0.6 mg/kg methylphenidate and completed the same task utilized in Study 1 at baseline (100 trials). Children with ADHD accumulated information at a much slower rate than controls, as evidenced by a lower drift rate. Groups were similar in nondecision time and boundary separation. Both reinforcement and stimulant medication markedly improved drift rate in children with ADHD (ds = 0.70 and 0.95 for reinforcement and methylphenidate, respectively); both treatments also reduced boundary separation (ds = 0.70 and 0.39). Reinforcement, which emphasized speeded accuracy, reduced nondecision time (d = 0.37), whereas stimulant medication increased nondecision time (d = 0.38). These studies provide initial evidence that frontline treatments for ADHD primarily impact cognitive performance in youth with ADHD by improving the speed/efficiency of information accumulation. Treatment effects on other DDM parameters may vary between treatments or interact with task parameters (number of trials, task difficulty). DDM, in conjunction with other approaches, may be helpful in clarifying the specific cognitive processes that are disrupted in ADHD, as well as the basic mechanisms that underlie the efficacy of ADHD treatments.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad , Estimulantes del Sistema Nervioso Central/farmacología , Disfunción Cognitiva , Metilfenidato/farmacología , Desempeño Psicomotor/efectos de los fármacos , Refuerzo en Psicología , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/complicaciones , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/fisiopatología , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/terapia , Estimulantes del Sistema Nervioso Central/administración & dosificación , Niño , Disfunción Cognitiva/etiología , Disfunción Cognitiva/fisiopatología , Disfunción Cognitiva/terapia , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Metilfenidato/administración & dosificación , Resultado del Tratamiento
20.
Alcohol ; 61: 1-8, 2017 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28599712

RESUMEN

Alcohol consumption may lead to deficits in the executive functions that govern self-regulation. These deficits could lead to risk-taking behaviors; therefore, it is important to determine the magnitude of these deficits on executive functioning. The purpose of this experiment was to investigate the acute effects of alcohol on three of the executive functions that are hypothesized to affect self-regulation, which are inhibition, set shifting, and working memory, using a mixed-methods study design. The participants were 75 moderate or heavy drinkers between the ages of 21 and 35 who were randomized into one of three beverage conditions (control, placebo, or 0.65-g alcohol dose/kg body weight). Performance on working memory, set shifting, and inhibition were measured pre- and post-beverage consumption. The results showed only a significant interaction in the working memory data, as there was an increase in performance post-beverage relative to pre-beverage for the control participants as compared to the alcohol and placebo participants. It was concluded that the dose of alcohol (BAC = 0.063%) given to moderate to heavy drinkers was not sufficient to cause significant impairment in the executive functions tested. The results were further discussed and methodological concerns were considered, such as the low BAC achieved, practice effects, and insensitivity of tasks.


Asunto(s)
Intoxicación Alcohólica/fisiopatología , Conducta/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/efectos de los fármacos , Autocontrol , Adulto , Conducta/efectos de los fármacos , Relación Dosis-Respuesta a Droga , Etanol/administración & dosificación , Etanol/sangre , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/efectos de los fármacos , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Placebos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA