Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 678
Filtrar
Más filtros

Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 119(30): e2204379119, 2022 07 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35858450

RESUMEN

Prediction errors guide many forms of learning, providing teaching signals that help us improve our performance. Implicit motor adaptation, for instance, is thought to be driven by sensory prediction errors (SPEs), which occur when the expected and observed consequences of a movement differ. Traditionally, SPE computation is thought to require movement execution. However, recent work suggesting that the brain can generate sensory predictions based on motor imagery or planning alone calls this assumption into question. Here, by measuring implicit motor adaptation during a visuomotor task, we tested whether motor planning and well-timed sensory feedback are sufficient for adaptation. Human participants were cued to reach to a target and were, on a subset of trials, rapidly cued to withhold these movements. Errors displayed both on trials with and without movements induced single-trial adaptation. Learning following trials without movements persisted even when movement trials had never been paired with errors and when the direction of movement and sensory feedback trajectories were decoupled. These observations indicate that the brain can compute errors that drive implicit adaptation without generating overt movements, leading to the adaptation of motor commands that are not overtly produced.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje , Desempeño Psicomotor , Adaptación Fisiológica , Retroalimentación Sensorial , Humanos , Movimiento
2.
Neuroimage ; 297: 120746, 2024 Aug 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39033789

RESUMEN

The effectiveness of motor imagery (MI) training on sports performance is now well-documented. Recently, it has been proposed that a single session of MI combined with low frequency sound (LFS) might enhance muscle activation. However, the neural mechanisms underlying this effect remain unknown. We set up a test-retest intervention over the course of 2 consecutive days to evaluate the effect of (i) MI training (MI, n = 20), (ii) MI combined with LFS (MI + LFS, n = 20), and (iii) a control condition (CTRL, n = 20) on force torque produced across repeated maximal voluntary contractions of the quadriceps before (Pretest), after (Posttest) and at +12 h (Retention) post-intervention. We collected the integrated electromyograms of the quadriceps muscles, as well as brain electrical potentials during each experimental intervention. In the CTRL group, total force torque decreased from Pretest to Retention and from Posttest to Retention. By contrast, there was an increase between Posttest and Retention in both MI + LFS and MI groups (both ηP2 = 0.03, p < 0.05). Regression analyses further revealed a negative relationship between force performance and EEG activity in the MI + LFS group only. The data support a transient interference of LFS on cortical activity underlying the priming effects of MI practice on force performance. Findings are discussed in relation to the potential for motor reprogramming through MI combined with LFS.


Asunto(s)
Electromiografía , Músculo Cuádriceps , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Músculo Cuádriceps/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Imaginación/fisiología , Femenino , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Torque
3.
Neuroimage ; 295: 120621, 2024 Jul 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38797383

RESUMEN

Although one can recognize the environment by soundscape substituting vision to auditory signal, whether subjects could perceive the soundscape as visual or visual-like sensation has been questioned. In this study, we investigated hierarchical process to elucidate the recruitment mechanism of visual areas by soundscape stimuli in blindfolded subjects. Twenty-two healthy subjects were repeatedly trained to recognize soundscape stimuli converted by visual shape information of letters. An effective connectivity method called dynamic causal modeling (DCM) was employed to reveal how the brain was hierarchically organized to recognize soundscape stimuli. The visual mental imagery model generated cortical source signals of five regions of interest better than auditory bottom-up, cross-modal perception, and mixed models. Spectral couplings between brain areas in the visual mental imagery model were analyzed. While within-frequency coupling is apparent in bottom-up processing where sensory information is transmitted, cross-frequency coupling is prominent in top-down processing, corresponding to the expectation and interpretation of information. Sensory substitution in the brain of blindfolded subjects derived visual mental imagery by combining bottom-up and top-down processing.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva , Imaginación , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Imaginación/fisiología , Adulto , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Electroencefalografía , Magnetoencefalografía/métodos
4.
J Neurosci Res ; 102(1): e25262, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37849328

RESUMEN

Mental imagery enables people to simulate experiences in their minds without the presence of an external stimulus. The underlying biochemical mechanisms are poorly understood but there is vague evidence that dopamine may play a significant role. A better understanding at the biochemical level could help to unravel the mechanisms of mental imagery and related phenomena such as aphantasia (= lack of voluntary mental imagery), but also opens up possibilities for interventions to enhance or restore mental imagery. To test the hypothesis that acute dopamine depletion leads to a decrease in the strength of mental imagery, N = 22 male participants will be administered an amino acid mixture containing branched-chain amino acids (BCAAs) and tryptophan (TRP) to transiently reduce dopamine synthesis and further N = 22 male participants will receive a placebo. Plasma prolactin (PRL) levels are determined as a peripheral marker of brain dopamine function. The strength of mental imagery will be measured before and after ingestion of the BCAA/TRP mixture using the method of mental imagery priming. Additional exploratory analyses will use genetic data to investigate possible effects of variations on dopaminergic gene loci (e.g., DAT1) on dopamine levels and strength of mental imagery. The results show […].


Asunto(s)
Aminoácidos , Dopamina , Humanos , Masculino , Dopamina/metabolismo , Triptófano/metabolismo , Aminoácidos de Cadena Ramificada
5.
Proc Biol Sci ; 291(2018): 20232867, 2024 Mar 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38471562

RESUMEN

A delayed foveal mask affects perception of peripheral stimuli. The effect is determined by the timing of the mask and by the similarity with the peripheral stimulus. A congruent mask enhances performance, while an incongruent one impairs it. It is hypothesized that foveal masks disrupt a feedback mechanism reaching the foveal cortex. This mechanism could be part of a broader circuit associated with mental imagery, but this hypothesis has not as yet been tested. We investigated the link between mental imagery and foveal feedback. We tested the relationship between performance fluctuations caused by the foveal mask-measured in terms of discriminability (d') and criterion (C)-and the scores from two questionnaires designed to assess mental imagery vividness (VVIQ) and another exploring object imagery, spatial imagery and verbal cognitive styles (OSIVQ). Contrary to our hypotheses, no significant correlations were found between VVIQ and the mask's impact on d' and C. Neither the object nor spatial subscales of OSIVQ correlated with the mask's impact. In conclusion, our findings do not substantiate the existence of a link between foveal feedback and mental imagery. Further investigation is needed to determine whether mask interference might occur with more implicit measures of imagery.


Asunto(s)
Imaginación , Percepción Visual , Fóvea Central , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Personalidad
6.
Brain Cogn ; 177: 106163, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38685168

RESUMEN

Mounting evidence indicates a close correspondence between episodic memory, mental imagery, and oculomotor behaviour. It remains unclear, however, how oculomotor variables support endogenously driven forms of mental imagery and how this relationship changes across the adult lifespan. In this study we investigated age-related changes in oculomotor signatures during scene construction and explored how task complexity impacts these processes. Younger and cognitively healthy older participants completed a guided scene construction paradigm where scene complexity was manipulated according to the number of elements to be sequentially integrated. We recorded participants' eye movements and collected subjective ratings regarding their phenomenological experience. Overall, older adults rated their constructions as more vivid and more spatially integrated, while also generating more fixations and saccades relative to the younger group, specifically on control trials. Analyses of participants' total scan paths revealed that, in the early stages of scene construction, oculomotor behaviour changed as a function of task complexity within each group. Following the introduction of a second stimulus, older but not younger adults showed a significant decrease in the production of eye movements. Whether this shift in oculomotor behaviour serves a compensatory function to bolster task performance represents an important question for future research.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Envejecimiento Saludable , Humanos , Masculino , Anciano , Femenino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Envejecimiento Saludable/fisiología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Imaginación/fisiología , Memoria Episódica , Envejecimiento/fisiología
7.
Cereb Cortex ; 33(19): 10234-10244, 2023 09 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37526263

RESUMEN

Visual mental imagery refers to our ability to experience visual images in the absence of sensory stimulation. Studies have shown that visual mental imagery can improve episodic memory. However, we have limited understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying this improvement. Using electroencephalography, we examined the neural processes associated with the retrieval of previously generated visual mental images, focusing on how the vividness at generation can modulate retrieval processes. Participants viewed word stimuli referring to common objects, forming a visual mental image of each word and rating the vividness of the mental image. This was followed by a surprise old/new recognition task. We compared retrieval performance for items rated as high- versus low-vividness at encoding. High-vividness items were retrieved with faster reaction times and higher confidence ratings in the memory judgment. While controlling for confidence, neural measures indicated that high-vividness items produced an earlier decrease in alpha-band activity at retrieval compared with low-vividness items, suggesting an earlier memory reinstatement. Even when low-vividness items were remembered with high confidence, they were not retrieved as quickly as high-vividness items. These results indicate that when highly vivid mental images are encoded, the speed of their retrieval occurs more rapidly, relative to low-vivid items.


Asunto(s)
Memoria Episódica , Recuerdo Mental , Humanos , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Juicio , Imágenes en Psicoterapia , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Imaginación/fisiología
8.
Cereb Cortex ; 33(6): 3207-3220, 2023 03 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35945684

RESUMEN

Attention can be directed externally toward sensory information or internally toward self-generated information. Using electroencephalography (EEG), we investigated the attentional processes underlying the formation and encoding of self-generated mental images into episodic memory. Participants viewed flickering words referring to common objects and were tasked with forming visual mental images of the objects and rating their vividness. Subsequent memory for the presented object words was assessed using an old-new recognition task. Internally-directed attention during image generation was indexed as a reduction in steady-state visual evoked potentials (SSVEPs), oscillatory EEG responses at the frequency of a flickering stimulus. The results yielded 3 main findings. First, SSVEP power driven by the flickering word stimuli decreased as subjects directed attention internally to form the corresponding mental image. Second, SSVEP power returned to pre-imagery baseline more slowly for low- than high-vividness later remembered items, suggesting that longer internally-directed attention is required to generate subsequently remembered low-vividness images. Finally, the event-related-potential difference due to memory was more sustained for subsequently remembered low- versus high-vividness items, suggesting that additional conceptual processing may have been needed to remember the low-vividness visual images. Taken together, the results clarify the neural mechanisms supporting the encoding of self-generated information.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados Visuales , Memoria Episódica , Humanos , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Electroencefalografía
9.
Cereb Cortex ; 33(14): 8890-8903, 2023 07 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37197767

RESUMEN

Predictions are constantly generated from diverse sources to optimize cognitive functions in the ever-changing environment. However, the neural origin and generation process of top-down induced prediction remain elusive. We hypothesized that motor-based and memory-based predictions are mediated by distinct descending networks from motor and memory systems to the sensory cortices. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and a dual imagery paradigm, we found that motor and memory upstream systems activated the auditory cortex in a content-specific manner. Moreover, the inferior and posterior parts of the parietal lobe differentially relayed predictive signals in motor-to-sensory and memory-to-sensory networks. Dynamic causal modeling of directed connectivity revealed selective enabling and modulation of connections that mediate top-down sensory prediction and ground the distinctive neurocognitive basis of predictive processing.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Auditiva , Mapeo Encefálico , Lóbulo Parietal/diagnóstico por imagen , Cognición , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Corteza Auditiva/diagnóstico por imagen , Vías Nerviosas/diagnóstico por imagen
10.
Conscious Cogn ; 118: 103645, 2024 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38241954

RESUMEN

Aphantasia is a condition in which people are unable to experience visual imagery. Since visual imagery is thought to be key to language processing, we hypothesized the experience of a story would differ between individuals with aphantasia and controls. Forty-seven individuals with aphantasia were compared to fifty-one matched controls on their experience of reading a short story and their general reading habits. Aphantasics were less likely to be engaged with, interested in, and absorbed in the story, and experienced reduced emotional engagement with and sympathy for the story characters, compared to controls. Yet, aphantasics and controls did not differ in how much they liked or appreciated the story, and in general, the reading habits of the two groups also did not differ. Results have implications for embodied theories of language, suggesting visual imagery may influence how a story is experienced, but it is not the only route to story enjoyment.


Asunto(s)
Imágenes en Psicoterapia , Imaginación , Humanos , Imágenes en Psicoterapia/métodos , Lenguaje , Placer , Felicidad
11.
Conscious Cogn ; 120: 103679, 2024 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38564857

RESUMEN

Aphantasia is a condition that is often characterized as the impaired ability to create voluntary mental images. Aphantasia is assumed to selectively affect voluntary imagery mainly because even though aphantasics report being unable to visualize something at will, many report having visual dreams. We argue that this common characterization of aphantasia is incorrect. Studies on aphantasia are often not clear about whether they are assessing voluntary or involuntary imagery, but some studies show that several forms of involuntary imagery are also affected in aphantasia (including imagery in dreams). We also raise problems for two attempts to show that involuntary images are preserved in aphantasia. In addition, we report the results of a study about afterimages in aphantasia, which suggest that these tend to be less intense in aphantasics than in controls. Involuntary imagery is often treated as a unitary kind that is either present or absent in aphantasia. We suggest that this approach is mistaken and that we should look at different types of involuntary imagery case by case. Doing so reveals no evidence of preserved involuntary imagery in aphantasia. We suggest that a broader characterization of aphantasia, as a deficit in forming mental imagery, whether voluntary or not, is more appropriate. Characterizing aphantasia as a volitional deficit is likely to lead researchers to give incorrect explanations for aphantasia, and to look for the wrong mechanisms underlying it.


Asunto(s)
Imágenes en Psicoterapia , Imaginación , Humanos , Volición
12.
Conscious Cogn ; 117: 103598, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38086154

RESUMEN

Little is known about the perceptual characteristics of mental images nor how they vary across sensory modalities. We conducted an exhaustive survey into how mental images are experienced across modalities, mainly targeting visual and auditory imagery of a single stimulus, the letter "O", to facilitate direct comparisons. We investigated temporal properties of mental images (e.g. onset latency, duration), spatial properties (e.g. apparent location), effort (e.g. ease, spontaneity, control), movement requirements (e.g. eye movements), real-imagined interactions (e.g. inner speech while reading), beliefs about imagery norms and terminologies, as well as respondent confidence. Participants also reported on the five traditional senses and their prominence during thinking, imagining, and dreaming. Overall, visual and auditory experiences dominated mental events, although auditory mental images were superior to visual mental images on almost every metric tested except regarding spatial properties. Our findings suggest that modality-specific differences in mental imagery may parallel those of other sensory neural processes.


Asunto(s)
Imaginación , Sensación , Humanos , Percepción Visual , Imágenes en Psicoterapia , Percepción Auditiva
13.
Conscious Cogn ; 125: 103749, 2024 Sep 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39243493

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: The present study investigated whether autistic adults report different levels of mental imagery vividness than non-autistic adults, and, moreover, if autism is associated with aphantasia which is defined as a condition of reduced or absent voluntary imagery. DESIGN AND METHODS: Clinically diagnosed and self-identifying autistic participants were compared with non-autistic participants in their mental imagery vividness (vision, sound, smell, taste, touch, bodily sensation and emotional feeling) and autistic traits using an online survey (N = 121). RESULTS: The autistic group scored significantly lower than the non-autistic group on imagery vividness (d = -0.44), in addition to having a higher proportion of participants scoring at cut-off for aphantasia. Moreover, a similar difference was observed for the emotional feel (η2 = 0.11). CONCLUSION: The vividness of visual and emotional mental imagery was on average lower for autistic individuals, with a higher proportion presenting at cut-off to be considered an aphantasic.

14.
Conscious Cogn ; 125: 103766, 2024 Oct 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39383563

RESUMEN

Contradictory findings suggest mental imagery may both exacerbate and protect against negative affect. We aimed to reconcile these contradictory findings by considering individual differences (N=989) in imagery vividness, attention control, resilience, emotion regulation strategy, and negative affect (depressive, anxious, and posttraumatic stress symptomology). We hypothesized that attention control would mediate relationships between imagery vividness and emotion regulation strategy use, and psychopathology symptomology. Results revealed that imagery vividness, as mediated by attention control, predicted greater levels of healthy reappraisal and deleterious rumination. Attention control also mediated negative relationships between imagery vividness and catastrophizing, self-blame, and psychopathology symptomology. An exploratory latent structural equation model revealed that imagery vividness and attention control aggregated positively with reappraisal and resilience scores. The present investigation suggests an adaptive function of imagery vividness via the indirect effects of attention control, facilitating adaptive emotion regulation and limiting maladaptive strategy use, thereby protecting against negative affect.

15.
Conscious Cogn ; 121: 103694, 2024 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38657474

RESUMEN

Mental rotation tasks are frequently used as standard measures of mental imagery. However, aphantasia research has brought such use into question. Here, we assessed a large group of individuals who lack visual imagery (aphantasia) on two mental rotation tasks: a three-dimensional block-shape, and a human manikin rotation task. In both tasks, those with aphantasia had slower, but more accurate responses than controls. Both groups demonstrated classic linear increases in response time and error-rate as functions of angular disparity. In the three-dimensional block-shape rotation task, a within-group speed-accuracy trade-off was found in controls, whereas faster individuals in the aphantasia group were also more accurate. Control participants generally favoured using object-based mental rotation strategies, whereas those with aphantasia favoured analytic strategies. These results suggest that visual imagery is not crucial for successful performance in classical mental rotation tasks, as alternative strategies can be effectively utilised in the absence of holistic mental representations.


Asunto(s)
Imaginación , Humanos , Imaginación/fisiología , Masculino , Adulto , Femenino , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Rotación , Persona de Mediana Edad , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
16.
Appetite ; 199: 107507, 2024 08 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38768925

RESUMEN

Previous research has demonstrated that music can impact people's food choices by triggering emotional states. We reported two virtual reality (VR) experiments designed to examine how Chinese folk music influences people's food choices by inducing mental imagery of different scenes. In both experiments, young healthy Chinese participants were asked to select three dishes from an assortment of two meat and two vegetable dishes while listening to Chinese folk music that could elicit mental imagery of nature or urban scenes. The results of Experiment 1 revealed that they chose vegetable-forward meals more frequently while listening to Chinese folk music eliciting mental imagery of nature versus urban scenes. In Experiment 2, the participants were randomly divided into three groups, in which the prevalence of their mental imagery was enhanced, moderately suppressed, or strongly suppressed by performing different tasks while listening to the music pieces. We replicated the results of Experiment 1 when the participants' mental imagery was enhanced, whereas no such effect was observed when the participants' mental imagery was moderately or strongly suppressed. Collectively, these findings suggest that music may influence the food choices people make in virtual food choice tasks by inducing mental imagery, which provides insights into utilizing environmental cues to promote healthier food choices.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Pueblos del Este de Asia , Preferencias Alimentarias , Imaginación , Música , Naturaleza , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven , Pueblo Asiatico/psicología , Belleza , China , Señales (Psicología) , Emociones , Preferencias Alimentarias/psicología , Música/psicología , Realidad Virtual , Ciudades
17.
Memory ; : 1-14, 2024 Sep 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39288236

RESUMEN

Most people experience positive involuntary mental imagery (IMI) frequently in daily life; however, evidence for the importance and effects of positive IMI is largely indirect. The current study adapted a paradigm to experimentally induce positive IMI in participants' daily lives. This could in turn provide a means to directly test positive IMI's effects. In a within-subjects design, participants (N = 41) generated positive mental images (imagery condition) and sentences (verbal condition) from photo cues, half of which participants provided from their own living environment. Participants then recorded involuntary memories of the previously generated images or sentences in a seven-day diary, before returning to the lab and completing some measures including an involuntary memory task. In the diary, participants reported more involuntary memories from the imagery condition than from the verbal condition, and more involuntary memories from their own photos compared to the other photos. A more mixed pattern of findings was found across other tasks in the lab. The study indicates that the paradigm can be used as a means to induce positive IMI and that using photos as the basis for generating positive imagery increases the amount of IMI in daily life. Theoretical and potential clinical implications are discussed.

18.
Memory ; 32(4): 502-514, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38557551

RESUMEN

Mounting evidence supports the efficacy of mental imagery for verbal information retention. Motor imagery, imagining oneself interacting physically with the object to be learned, emerges as an optimal form compared to less physically engaging imagery. Yet, when engaging in mental imagery, it occurs within a specific context that may affect imagined actions and consequently impact the mnemonic benefits of mental imagery. In a first study, participants were given instructions for incidental learning: mental rehearsal, visual imagery, motor imagery or situated motor imagery. The latter, which involved imagining physical interaction with an item within a coherent situation, produced the highest proportion of correct recalls. This highlights memory's role in supporting situated actions and offers the possibility for further developing the mnemonic potential of embodied mental imagery. Furthermore, item-level analysis showed that individuals who engaged in situated motor imagery remembered words primarily due to the sensorimotor characteristics of the words' referent. A second study investigating the role of inter-item distinctiveness in this effect failed to determine the extent to which the situational and motor elements need to be distinctive in order to be considered useful retrieval cues and produce an optimal memory performance.


Asunto(s)
Imaginación , Aprendizaje , Recuerdo Mental , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Adulto Joven , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Adulto , Adolescente , Memoria/fisiología , Señales (Psicología)
19.
Cogn Emot ; : 1-19, 2024 Sep 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39330940

RESUMEN

Anhedonia, or a deficit in the liking, wanting, and seeking of rewards, is typically assessed via self-reported "in-the-moment" emotional and motivational responses to reward stimuli and activities. Given that mental imagery is known to evoke emotion and motivational responses, we conducted two studies to investigate the relationship between mental imagery use and self-reported anhedonia. Using a novel Reward Response Scale (adapted from the Dimensional Anhedonia Rating Scale, DARS; Rizvi et al., 2015) modified to assess deliberate and spontaneous mental imagery use, Study 1 (N = 394) compared uninstructed and instructed mental imagery use, and Study 2 (N = 586) conducted a test of replication of uninstructed mental imagery use. Results showed that greater mental imagery use was associated with higher reward response scores (Study 1 & 2), and this relationship was not moderated by whether imagery use was uninstructed or instructed (Study 1). Importantly, mental imagery use moderated the convergence between reward response and depression scale measures of anhedonia, with lower convergence for those reporting higher mental imagery use (Study 1 & 2). Results suggest that higher spontaneous mental imagery use may increase self-reported reward response and reduce the convergence between reward response scale and depression questionnaire measures of anhedonia. [199 / 200 words].

20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38704800

RESUMEN

Adolescent depression is associated with unhelpful emotional mental imagery. Here, we investigated whether vividness of negative and positive prospective mental imagery predict negative affect and anhedonia in adolescents. 111 people from Israel completed measures of prospective mental imagery, negative affect, and anhedonia at two time-points approximately three months apart. Using three cross-lagged panel models, we showed once 'concurrent' (across-variable, within-time) and 'stability' paths (across-time, within-variable) were estimated, there were no significant cross-lag paths between: i) T1 prospective negative mental imagery and T8 negative affect (i.e. increased vividness of negative future imagery at Time 1 did not predict increased negative affect at Time 8); ii) T1 prospective positive mental imagery and T8 negative affect (i.e. reduced vividness of positive future imagery at Time 1 did not predict increased negative affect at Time 8); and iii) T1 prospective positive mental imagery and T8 anhedonia (i.e. reduced vividness of positive future imagery at Time 1 did not predict increased anhedonia at Time 8). Given high levels of attrition, future research should aim to explore these associations in a larger, more diverse population, as such data could inform on whether modifying earlier prospective mental imagery may influence later time/context-specific effects of prospective mental imagery on negative affect and anhedonia.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA