Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 58
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Ann Neurol ; 95(5): 951-965, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38400792

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: A clock relating amyloid positron emission tomography (PET) to time was used to estimate the timing of biomarker changes in sporadic Alzheimer disease (AD). METHODS: Research participants were included who underwent cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) collection within 2 years of amyloid PET. The ages at amyloid onset and AD symptom onset were estimated for each individual. The timing of change for plasma, CSF, imaging, and cognitive measures was calculated by comparing restricted cubic splines of cross-sectional data from the amyloid PET positive and negative groups. RESULTS: The amyloid PET positive sub-cohort (n = 118) had an average age of 70.4 ± 7.4 years (mean ± standard deviation) and 16% were cognitively impaired. The amyloid PET negative sub-cohort (n = 277) included individuals with low levels of amyloid plaque burden at all scans who were cognitively unimpaired at the time of the scans. Biomarker changes were detected 15-19 years before estimated symptom onset for CSF Aß42/Aß40, plasma Aß42/Aß40, CSF pT217/T217, and amyloid PET; 12-14 years before estimated symptom onset for plasma pT217/T217, CSF neurogranin, CSF SNAP-25, CSF sTREM2, plasma GFAP, and plasma NfL; and 7-9 years before estimated symptom onset for CSF pT205/T205, CSF YKL-40, hippocampal volumes, and cognitive measures. INTERPRETATION: The use of an amyloid clock enabled visualization and analysis of biomarker changes as a function of estimated years from symptom onset in sporadic AD. This study demonstrates that estimated years from symptom onset based on an amyloid clock can be used as a continuous staging measure for sporadic AD and aligns with findings in autosomal dominant AD. ANN NEUROL 2024;95:951-965.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Péptidos beta-Amiloides , Biomarcadores , Tomografía de Emisión de Positrones , Humanos , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/sangre , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico por imagen , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Femenino , Masculino , Biomarcadores/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Biomarcadores/sangre , Anciano , Péptidos beta-Amiloides/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Péptidos beta-Amiloides/sangre , Persona de Mediana Edad , Fragmentos de Péptidos/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Fragmentos de Péptidos/sangre , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Estudios Transversales , Factores de Tiempo , Edad de Inicio , Estudios de Cohortes , Progresión de la Enfermedad , Proteína 1 Similar a Quitinasa-3/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Proteína 1 Similar a Quitinasa-3/sangre , Disfunción Cognitiva/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Disfunción Cognitiva/sangre , Placa Amiloide/diagnóstico por imagen , Placa Amiloide/patología
2.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 30(5): 428-438, 2024 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38282413

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Maintaining attention underlies many aspects of cognition and becomes compromised early in neurodegenerative diseases like Alzheimer's disease (AD). The consistency of maintaining attention can be measured with reaction time (RT) variability. Previous work has focused on measuring such fluctuations during in-clinic testing, but recent developments in remote, smartphone-based cognitive assessments can allow one to test if these fluctuations in attention are evident in naturalistic settings and if they are sensitive to traditional clinical and cognitive markers of AD. METHOD: Three hundred and seventy older adults (aged 75.8 +/- 5.8 years) completed a week of remote daily testing on the Ambulatory Research in Cognition (ARC) smartphone platform and also completed clinical, genetic, and conventional in-clinic cognitive assessments. RT variability was assessed in a brief (20-40 seconds) processing speed task using two different measures of variability, the Coefficient of Variation (CoV) and the Root Mean Squared Successive Difference (RMSSD) of RTs on correct trials. RESULTS: Symptomatic participants showed greater variability compared to cognitively normal participants. When restricted to cognitively normal participants, APOE ε4 carriers exhibited greater variability than noncarriers. Both CoV and RMSSD showed significant, and similar, correlations with several in-clinic cognitive composites. Finally, both RT variability measures significantly mediated the relationship between APOE ε4 status and several in-clinic cognition composites. CONCLUSIONS: Attentional fluctuations over 20-40 seconds assessed in daily life, are sensitive to clinical status and genetic risk for AD. RT variability appears to be an important predictor of cognitive deficits during the preclinical disease stage.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Tiempo de Reacción , Humanos , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/fisiopatología , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/genética , Anciano , Masculino , Femenino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Apolipoproteína E4/genética , Teléfono Inteligente , Atención/fisiología
3.
Alzheimers Dement ; 20(4): 2698-2706, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38400532

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Increasing evidence suggests that amyloid reduction could serve as a plausible surrogate endpoint for clinical and cognitive efficacy. The double-blind phase 3 DIAN-TU-001 trial tested clinical and cognitive declines with increasing doses of solanezumab or gantenerumab. METHODS: We used latent class (LC) analysis on data from the Dominantly Inherited Alzheimer Network Trials Unit 001 trial to test amyloid positron emission tomography (PET) reduction as a potential surrogate biomarker. RESULTS: LC analysis categorized participants into three classes: amyloid no change, amyloid reduction, and amyloid growth, based on longitudinal amyloid Pittsburgh compound B PET standardized uptake value ratio data. The amyloid-no-change class was at an earlier disease stage for amyloid amounts and dementia. Despite similar baseline characteristics, the amyloid-reduction class exhibited reductions in the annual decline rates compared to the amyloid-growth class across multiple biomarker, clinical, and cognitive outcomes. DISCUSSION: LC analysis indicates that amyloid reduction is associated with improved clinical outcomes and supports its use as a surrogate biomarker in clinical trials. HIGHLIGHTS: We used latent class (LC) analysis to test amyloid reduction as a surrogate biomarker. Despite similar baseline characteristics, the amyloid-reduction class exhibited remarkably better outcomes compared to the amyloid-growth class across multiple measures. LC analysis proves valuable in testing amyloid reduction as a surrogate biomarker in clinical trials lacking significant treatment effects.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Humanos , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico por imagen , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/tratamiento farmacológico , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/genética , Amiloide , Péptidos beta-Amiloides , Proteínas Amiloidogénicas , Biomarcadores , Método Doble Ciego , Análisis de Clases Latentes , Tomografía de Emisión de Positrones/métodos
4.
Alzheimers Dement ; 20(1): 47-62, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37740921

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Studies suggest distinct differences in the development, presentation, progression, and response to treatment of Alzheimer's disease (AD) between females and males. We investigated sex differences in cognition, neuroimaging, and fluid biomarkers in dominantly inherited AD (DIAD). METHODS: Three hundred twenty-five mutation carriers (55% female) and one hundred eighty-six non-carriers (58% female) of the Dominantly Inherited Alzheimer Network Observational Study were analyzed. Linear mixed models and Spearman's correlation explored cross-sectional sex differences in cognition, cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers, Pittsburgh compound B positron emission tomography (11 C-PiB PET) and structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). RESULTS: Female carriers performed better than males on delayed recall and processing speed despite similar hippocampal volumes. As the disease progressed, symptomatic females revealed higher increases in MRI markers of neurodegeneration and memory impairment. PiB PET and established CSF AD markers revealed no sex differences. DISCUSSION: Our findings suggest an initial cognitive reserve in female carriers followed by a pronounced increase in neurodegeneration coupled with worse performance on delayed recall at later stages of DIAD.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico por imagen , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/genética , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Péptidos beta-Amiloides/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Estudios Transversales , Caracteres Sexuales , Tomografía de Emisión de Positrones , Mutación/genética , Biomarcadores
5.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 29(5): 459-471, 2023 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36062528

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Smartphones have the potential for capturing subtle changes in cognition that characterize preclinical Alzheimer's disease (AD) in older adults. The Ambulatory Research in Cognition (ARC) smartphone application is based on principles from ecological momentary assessment (EMA) and administers brief tests of associative memory, processing speed, and working memory up to 4 times per day over 7 consecutive days. ARC was designed to be administered unsupervised using participants' personal devices in their everyday environments. METHODS: We evaluated the reliability and validity of ARC in a sample of 268 cognitively normal older adults (ages 65-97 years) and 22 individuals with very mild dementia (ages 61-88 years). Participants completed at least one 7-day cycle of ARC testing and conventional cognitive assessments; most also completed cerebrospinal fluid, amyloid and tau positron emission tomography, and structural magnetic resonance imaging studies. RESULTS: First, ARC tasks were reliable as between-person reliability across the 7-day cycle and test-retest reliabilities at 6-month and 1-year follow-ups all exceeded 0.85. Second, ARC demonstrated construct validity as evidenced by correlations with conventional cognitive measures (r = 0.53 between composite scores). Third, ARC measures correlated with AD biomarker burden at baseline to a similar degree as conventional cognitive measures. Finally, the intensive 7-day cycle indicated that ARC was feasible (86.50% approached chose to enroll), well tolerated (80.42% adherence, 4.83% dropout), and was rated favorably by older adult participants. CONCLUSIONS: Overall, the results suggest that ARC is reliable and valid and represents a feasible tool for assessing cognitive changes associated with the earliest stages of AD.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Disfunción Cognitiva , Humanos , Anciano , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/psicología , Teléfono Inteligente , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Cognición , Biomarcadores/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Tomografía de Emisión de Positrones , Disfunción Cognitiva/psicología , Péptidos beta-Amiloides/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Proteínas tau/líquido cefalorraquídeo
6.
Behav Res Methods ; 55(6): 2800-2812, 2023 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35953659

RESUMEN

Studies using remote cognitive testing must make a critical decision: whether to allow participants to use their own devices or to provide participants with a study-specific device. Bring-your-own-device (BYOD) studies have several advantages including increased accessibility, potential for larger sample sizes, and reduced participant burden. However, BYOD studies offer little control over device performance characteristics that could potentially influence results. In particular, response times measured by each device not only include the participant's true response time, but also latencies of the device itself. The present study investigated two prominent sources of device latencies that pose significant risks to data quality: device display output latency and touchscreen input latency. We comprehensively tested 26 popular smartphones ranging in price from < $100 to $1000+ running either Android or iOS to determine if hardware and operating system differences led to appreciable device latency variability. To accomplish this, a custom-built device called the Latency and Timing Assessment Robot (LaTARbot) measured device display output and capacitive touchscreen input latencies. We found considerable variability across smartphones in display and touch latencies which, if unaccounted for, could be misattributed as individual or group differences in response times. Specifically, total device (sum of display and touch) latencies ranged from 35 to 140 ms. We offer recommendations to researchers to increase the precision of data collection and analysis in the context of remote BYOD studies.


Asunto(s)
Computadoras de Mano , Teléfono Inteligente , Humanos , Recolección de Datos/métodos , Programas Informáticos
7.
Psychol Sci ; 32(6): 849-860, 2021 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34043464

RESUMEN

Neurodegenerative disease is highly prevalent among older adults and, if undetected, may obscure estimates of cognitive change among aging samples. Our aim in this study was to determine the nature and magnitude of cognitive change in the absence of common neuropathologic markers of neurodegenerative disease. Cognitively normal older adults (ages 65-89 years, N = 199) were classified as normal or abnormal using neuroimaging and cerebrospinal-fluid biomarkers of ß-amyloid, tau, and neurodegeneration. When cognitive change was modeled without accounting for biomarker status, significant decline was evident for semantic memory, processing speed, and working memory. However, after adjusting for biomarker status, we found that the rate of change was attenuated and that the biomarker-normal group demonstrated no decline for any cognitive domain. These results indicate that estimates of cognitive change in otherwise healthy older adults will be biased toward decline when the presence of early neurodegenerative disease is not accounted for.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico por imagen , Péptidos beta-Amiloides , Sesgo , Biomarcadores , Cognición , Humanos , Memoria , Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas/diagnóstico por imagen , Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas/epidemiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
8.
Alzheimers Dement ; 17(3): 534-542, 2021 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33215873

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) is widely used in Alzheimer's disease research studies and has well established reliability and validity. To facilitate the development of an online, electronic CDR (eCDR) for more efficient clinical applications, this study aims to produce a shortened version of the CDR, and to develop the statistical model for automatic scoring. METHODS: Item response theory (IRT) was used for item evaluation and model development. An automatic scoring algorithm was validated using existing CDR global and domain box scores as the reference standard. RESULTS: Most CDR items discriminate well at mild and very mild levels of cognitive impairment. The bi-factor IRT model fits best and the shortened CDR still demonstrates very high classification accuracy (81%∼92%). DISCUSSION: The shortened version of the CDR and the automatic scoring algorithm has established a good foundation for developing an eCDR and will ultimately improve the efficiency of cognitive assessment.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Disfunción Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Pruebas de Estado Mental y Demencia , Anciano , Algoritmos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas de Estado Mental y Demencia/normas , Pruebas de Estado Mental y Demencia/estadística & datos numéricos , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica/normas , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica/estadística & datos numéricos
9.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 26(6): 596-606, 2020 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31822309

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: The present study explored relationships among personality, Alzheimer's disease (AD) biomarkers, and dementia by addressing the following questions: (1) Does personality discriminate healthy aging and earliest detectable stage of AD? (2) Does personality predict conversion from healthy aging to early-stage AD? (3) Do AD biomarkers mediate any observed relationships between personality and dementia status/conversion? METHODS: Both self- and informant ratings of personality were obtained in a large well-characterized longitudinal sample of cognitively normal older adults (N = 436) and individuals with early-stage dementia (N = 74). Biomarkers included amyloid imaging, hippocampal volume, cerebral spinal fluid (CSF) Aß42, and CSF tau. RESULTS: Higher neuroticism, lower conscientiousness, along with all four biomarkers strongly discriminated cognitively normal controls from early-stage AD individuals. The direct effects of neuroticism and conscientiousness were only mediated by hippocampal volume. Conscientiousness along with all biomarkers predicted conversion from healthy aging to early-stage AD; however, none of the biomarkers mediated the relationship between conscientiousness and conversion. Conscientiousness predicted conversion as strongly as the biomarkers, with the exception of hippocampal volume. CONCLUSIONS: Conscientiousness and to a lesser extent neuroticism serve as important independent behavioral markers for AD risk.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/fisiopatología , Biomarcadores , Personalidad , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Péptidos beta-Amiloides , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Cognición , Femenino , Hipocampo/patología , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Missouri , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Neuroticismo , Sensibilidad y Especificidad , Proteínas tau
10.
Alzheimers Dement ; 16(1): 219-228, 2020 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31914221

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Although some members of families with autosomal dominant Alzheimer's disease mutations learn their mutation status, most do not. How knowledge of mutation status affects clinical disease progression is unknown. This study quantifies the influence of mutation awareness on clinical symptoms, cognition, and biomarkers. METHODS: Mutation carriers and non-carriers from the Dominantly Inherited Alzheimer Network (DIAN) were stratified based on knowledge of mutation status. Rates of change on standard clinical, cognitive, and neuroimaging outcomes were examined. RESULTS: Mutation knowledge had no associations with cognitive decline, clinical progression, amyloid deposition, hippocampal volume, or depression in either carriers or non-carriers. Carriers who learned their status mid-study had slightly higher levels of depression and lower cognitive scores. DISCUSSION: Knowledge of mutation status does not affect rates of change on any measured outcome. Learning of status mid-study may confer short-term changes in cognitive functioning, or changes in cognition may influence the determination of mutation status.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/genética , Concienciación , Conocimientos, Actitudes y Práctica en Salud , Mutación/genética , Neuroimagen , Adulto , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico por imagen , Amiloide , Biomarcadores , Cognición , Progresión de la Enfermedad , Femenino , Hipocampo/metabolismo , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Pruebas de Estado Mental y Demencia , Factores de Riesgo
11.
Mem Cognit ; 46(8): 1287-1301, 2018 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29959616

RESUMEN

Our study examined processing effects in improving memory accuracy in older and younger adults. Specifically, we evaluated the effectiveness of item-specific and relational processing instructions relative to a read-only control task on correct and false recognition in younger and older adults using a categorized-list paradigm. In both age groups, item-specific and relational processing improved correct recognition versus a read-only control task, and item-specific encoding decreased false recognition relative to both the relational and read-only groups. This pattern was found in older adults despite overall elevated rates of false recognition. We then applied signal-detection and diffusion-modeling analyses, which separately utilized recognition responses and the latencies to those responses to estimate contributions of encoding and monitoring processes on recognition decisions. Converging evidence from both analyses demonstrated that item-specific processing benefits to memory accuracy were due to improvements of both encoding (estimates of d' and drift rate) and monitoring (estimates of lambda and boundary separation) processes, and, importantly, occurred similarly in both younger and older adults. Thus, older and younger adults showed similar encoding-based and test-based benefits of item-specific processing to enhance memory accuracy.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Modelos Psicológicos , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Detección de Señal Psicológica/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Lectura , Adulto Joven
12.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 21(8): 573-83, 2015 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26416094

RESUMEN

Cognitive measures that are sensitive to biological markers of Alzheimer disease (AD) pathology are needed to (a) facilitate preclinical staging, (b) identify individuals who are at the highest risk for developing clinical symptoms, and (c) serve as endpoints for evaluating the efficacy of interventions. The present study assesses the utility of two cognitive composite scores of attentional control and episodic memory as markers for preclinical AD pathology in a group of cognitively normal older adults (N = 238), as part of the Adult Children Study. All participants were given a baseline cognitive assessment and follow-up assessments every 3 years over an 8-year period, as well as a lumbar puncture within 2 years of the initial assessment to collect cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) and amyloid tracer Pittsburgh compound-B scan for amyloid imaging. Results indicated that attentional control was correlated with levels of Aß42 at the initial assessment whereas episodic memory was not. Longitudinally, individuals with high CSF tau exhibited a decline in both attention and episodic memory over the course of the study. These results indicate that measures of attentional control and episodic memory can be used to evaluate cognitive decline in preclinical AD and provide support that CSF tau may be a key mechanism driving longitudinal cognitive change.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/complicaciones , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/etiología , Biomarcadores/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Memoria Episódica , Anciano , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico por imagen , Compuestos de Anilina/farmacocinética , Ensayo de Inmunoadsorción Enzimática , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tomografía de Emisión de Positrones , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Tiazoles/farmacocinética
13.
Mem Cognit ; 42(6): 886-97, 2014 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24643790

RESUMEN

Research on metacognitive judgment accuracy during retrieval practice has increased in recent years. However, prior work had not systematically evaluated item-level judgment accuracy and the underlying bases of judgment accuracy in a criterion-learning paradigm (in which items are practiced until correctly recalled during encoding). Understanding these relationships during criterion learning has important theoretical implications for self-regulated learning frameworks, and also has applied implications for student learning: If the factors that influence metacognitive judgments are not predictive of subsequent test performance, students may make poor decisions during self-regulated learning. In the present experiments, participants engaged in test-restudy practice until items were recalled correctly. Once a given item reached criterion, participants made an immediate or delayed judgment of learning (JOL) for the item. A final cued-recall test occurred 30 min later. We examined judgment accuracy (the relationship between JOLs and test performance) and the underlying bases of judgment accuracy by evaluating cue utilization (the relationship between cues and JOLs) and cue diagnosticity (the relationship between cues and test performance). Immediate JOLs were only modestly related to subsequent test performance, and further analyses revealed that the cues related to JOLs were only weakly predictive of test accuracy. However, delaying JOLs improved both the accuracy of the JOLs and the diagnosticity of the cues that influenced judgments.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Adulto , Humanos , Adulto Joven
14.
Behav Res Methods ; 46(4): 1052-67, 2014 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24488815

RESUMEN

Reading involves a process of matching an orthographic input with stored representations in lexical memory. The masked priming paradigm has become a standard tool for investigating this process. Use of existing results from this paradigm can be limited by the precision of the data and the need for cross-experiment comparisons that lack normal experimental controls. Here, we present a single, large, high-precision, multicondition experiment to address these problems. Over 1,000 participants from 14 sites responded to 840 trials involving 28 different types of orthographically related primes (e.g., castfe-CASTLE) in a lexical decision task, as well as completing measures of spelling and vocabulary. The data were indeed highly sensitive to differences between conditions: After correction for multiple comparisons, prime type condition differences of 2.90 ms and above reached significance at the 5% level. This article presents the method of data collection and preliminary findings from these data, which included replications of the most widely agreed-upon differences between prime types, further evidence for systematic individual differences in susceptibility to priming, and new evidence regarding lexical properties associated with a target word's susceptibility to priming. These analyses will form a basis for the use of these data in quantitative model fitting and evaluation and for future exploration of these data that will inform and motivate new experiments.


Asunto(s)
Bases de Datos Factuales , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Enmascaramiento Perceptual/fisiología , Lectura , Memoria Implícita/fisiología , Análisis de Varianza , Humanos , Individualidad , Lenguaje , Memoria , Tiempo de Reacción , Vocabulario
15.
Neuropsychology ; 38(5): 430-442, 2024 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38330359

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Mind wandering refers to periods of internally directed attention and comprises up to 30% or more of our waking thoughts. Frequent mind wandering can be detrimental to ongoing task performance. We aim to determine whether rates of mind wandering change in healthy aging and mild cognitive impairment and how differences in mind wandering contribute to differences in attention and working memory. METHOD: We administered a standard behavioral task, the Sustained Attention to Response Test, to measure mind wandering in healthy younger adults (N = 66), healthy older adults (N = 51), and adults with cognitive impairment (N = 38), that was completed daily for 3 weeks. The N-back test was also administered at a reduced frequency as a measure of working memory performance. RESULTS: Generally speaking, averaged across 3 weeks of testing, relative to healthy older adults, mind wandering was higher in younger adults and in cognitive impairment, although the specific patterns varied across mind wandering states. Multiple states of mind wandering also predicted working memory performance; however, reaction time variability tended to be the best predictor based on model comparisons. Each state was also modestly associated with different dispositional factors including mood and Agreeableness. CONCLUSIONS: Patterns of mind wandering change across healthy aging and cognitive impairment and are related to individual differences in multiple dispositional factors and also working memory performance. These results suggest that different states of mind wandering should be measured and accounted for when modeling cognitive change in healthy and pathological aging. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Atención , Disfunción Cognitiva , Envejecimiento Saludable , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Humanos , Disfunción Cognitiva/fisiopatología , Masculino , Anciano , Femenino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Persona de Mediana Edad , Envejecimiento Saludable/psicología , Envejecimiento Saludable/fisiología , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Pensamiento/fisiología , Envejecimiento/fisiología
16.
Neuropsychology ; 38(1): 69-80, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37079810

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Observational studies on aging and Alzheimer's disease (AD) typically focus on mean-level changes in cognitive performance over relatively long periods of time (years or decades). Additionally, some studies have examined how trial-level fluctuations in speeded reaction time are related to both age and AD. The aim of the current project was to describe patterns of variability across repeated days of testing as a function of AD risk in cognitively normal older adults. METHOD: The current project examined the performance of the Ambulatory Research in Cognition (ARC) smartphone application, a high-frequency remote cognitive assessment paradigm, that administers brief tests of episodic memory, spatial working memory, and processing speed. Bayesian mixed-effects location scale models were used to explore differences in mean cognitive performance and intraindividual variability across 28 repeated sessions over a 1-week assessment interval as function of age and genetic risk of AD, specifically the presence of at least one apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 allele. RESULTS: Mean performance on processing speed and working memory was negatively related to age and APOE status. More importantly, e4 carriers exhibited increased session-level variability on a test of processing speed compared to noncarriers. Age and education did not consistently relate to cognitive variability, contrary to expectations. CONCLUSION: Preclinical AD risk, defined as possessing at least one APOE ε4 allele, is not only associated with mean-level performance differences, but also with increases in variability across repeated testing occasions particularly on a test of processing speed. Thus, cognitive variability may serve as an additional and important indicator of AD risk. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Humanos , Anciano , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/genética , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/complicaciones , Teorema de Bayes , Apolipoproteína E4/genética , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Cognición , Apolipoproteínas E/genética , Genotipo
17.
Neuropsychology ; 38(5): 443-464, 2024 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38602816

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: We aimed to illustrate how complex cognitive data can be used to create domain-specific and general cognitive composites relevant to Alzheimer disease research. METHOD: Using equipercentile equating, we combined data from the Charles F. and Joanne Knight Alzheimer Disease Research Center that spanned multiple iterations of the Uniform Data Set. Exploratory factor analyses revealed four domain-specific composites representing episodic memory, semantic memory, working memory, and attention/processing speed. The previously defined preclinical Alzheimer disease cognitive composite (PACC) and a novel alternative, the Knight-PACC, were also computed alongside a global composite comprising all available tests. These three composites allowed us to compare the usefulness of domain and general composites in the context of predicting common Alzheimer disease biomarkers. RESULTS: General composites slightly outperformed domain-specific metrics in predicting imaging-derived amyloid, tau, and neurodegeneration burden. Power analyses revealed that the global, Knight-PACC, and attention and processing speed composites would require the smallest sample sizes to detect cognitive change in a clinical trial, while the Alzheimer Disease Cooperative Study-PACC required two to three times as many participants. CONCLUSIONS: Analyses of cognition with the Knight-PACC and our domain-specific composites offer researchers flexibility by providing validated outcome assessments that can equate across test versions to answer a wide range of questions regarding cognitive decline in normal aging and neurodegenerative disease. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Humanos , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/psicología , Femenino , Masculino , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Atención/fisiología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Memoria Episódica , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Disfunción Cognitiva/diagnóstico
18.
Nat Aging ; 4(5): 694-708, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38514824

RESUMEN

Biological staging of individuals with Alzheimer's disease (AD) may improve diagnostic and prognostic workup of dementia in clinical practice and the design of clinical trials. In this study, we used the Subtype and Stage Inference (SuStaIn) algorithm to establish a robust biological staging model for AD using cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers. Our analysis involved 426 participants from BioFINDER-2 and was validated in 222 participants from the Knight Alzheimer Disease Research Center cohort. SuStaIn identified a singular biomarker sequence and revealed that five CSF biomarkers effectively constituted a reliable staging model (ordered: Aß42/40, pT217/T217, pT205/T205, MTBR-tau243 and non-phosphorylated mid-region tau). The CSF stages (0-5) demonstrated a correlation with increased abnormalities in other AD-related biomarkers, such as Aß-PET and tau-PET, and aligned with longitudinal biomarker changes reflective of AD progression. Higher CSF stages at baseline were associated with an elevated hazard ratio of clinical decline. This study highlights a common molecular pathway underlying AD pathophysiology across all patients, suggesting that a single CSF collection can accurately indicate the presence of AD pathologies and characterize the stage of disease progression. The proposed staging model has implications for enhancing diagnostic and prognostic assessments in both clinical practice and the design of clinical trials.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Péptidos beta-Amiloides , Biomarcadores , Proteínas tau , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/patología , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Humanos , Biomarcadores/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Proteínas tau/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Femenino , Masculino , Péptidos beta-Amiloides/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Anciano , Progresión de la Enfermedad , Fragmentos de Péptidos/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Algoritmos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Tomografía de Emisión de Positrones
19.
Neuroimage Clin ; 41: 103551, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38150745

RESUMEN

The use of biomarkers for the early detection of Alzheimer's disease (AD) is crucial for developing potential therapeutic treatments. Positron Emission Tomography (PET) is a well-established tool used to detect ß-amyloid (Aß) plaques in the brain. Previous studies have shown that cross-sectional biomarkers can predict cognitive decline (Schindler et al.,2021). However, it is still unclear whether longitudinal Aß-PET may have additional value for predicting time to cognitive impairment in AD. The current study aims to evaluate the ability of baseline- versus longitudinal rate of change in-11C-Pittsburgh compound B (PiB) Aß-PET to predict cognitive decline. A cohort of 153 participants who previously underwent PiB-PET scans and comprehensive clinical assessments were used in this study. Our analyses revealed that baseline Aß is significantly associated with the rate of change in cognitive composite scores, with cognition declining more rapidly when baseline PiB Aß levels were higher. In contrast, no signification association was identified between the rate of change in PiB-PET Aß and cognitive decline. Additionally, the ability of the rate of change in the PiB-PET measures to predict cognitive decline was significantly influenced by APOE ε4 carrier status. These results suggest that a single PiB-PET scan is sufficient to predict cognitive decline and that longitudinal measures of Aß accumulation do not improve the prediction of cognitive decline once someone is amyloid positive.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Disfunción Cognitiva , Humanos , Estudios Transversales , Péptidos beta-Amiloides/metabolismo , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico por imagen , Amiloide/metabolismo , Disfunción Cognitiva/diagnóstico por imagen , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Biomarcadores , Tomografía de Emisión de Positrones/métodos , Estudios Longitudinales
20.
EBioMedicine ; 103: 105080, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38552342

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Neuroimaging studies often quantify tau burden in standardized brain regions to assess Alzheimer disease (AD) progression. However, this method ignores another key biological process in which tau spreads to additional brain regions. We have developed a metric for calculating the extent tau pathology has spread throughout the brain and evaluate the relationship between this metric and tau burden across early stages of AD. METHODS: 445 cross-sectional participants (aged ≥ 50) who had MRI, amyloid PET, tau PET, and clinical testing were separated into disease-stage groups based on amyloid positivity and cognitive status (older cognitively normal control, preclinical AD, and symptomatic AD). Tau burden and tau spatial spread were calculated for all participants. FINDINGS: We found both tau metrics significantly elevated across increasing disease stages (p < 0.0001) and as a function of increasing amyloid burden for participants with preclinical (p < 0.0001, p = 0.0056) and symptomatic (p = 0.010, p = 0.0021) AD. An interaction was found between tau burden and tau spatial spread when predicting amyloid burden (p = 0.00013). Analyses of slope between tau metrics demonstrated more spread than burden in preclinical AD (ß = 0.59), but then tau burden elevated relative to spread (ß = 0.42) once participants had symptomatic AD, when the tau metrics became highly correlated (R = 0.83). INTERPRETATION: Tau burden and tau spatial spread are both strong biomarkers for early AD but provide unique information, particularly at the preclinical stage. Tau spatial spread may demonstrate earlier changes than tau burden which could have broad impact in clinical trial design. FUNDING: This research was supported by the Knight Alzheimer Disease Research Center (Knight ADRC, NIH grants P30AG066444, P01AG026276, P01AG003991), Dominantly Inherited Alzheimer Network (DIAN, NIH grants U01AG042791, U19AG03243808, R01AG052550-01A1, R01AG05255003), and the Barnes-Jewish Hospital Foundation Willman Scholar Fund.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Encéfalo , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Neuroimagen , Proteínas tau , Humanos , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/metabolismo , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico por imagen , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/patología , Proteínas tau/metabolismo , Femenino , Masculino , Anciano , Neuroimagen/métodos , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Encéfalo/patología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Tomografía de Emisión de Positrones/métodos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estudios Transversales , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Progresión de la Enfermedad , Biomarcadores
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA