Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 2 de 2
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo de estudio
Tipo del documento
Asunto de la revista
País de afiliación
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Clin Cancer Res ; 4(3): 763-71, 1998 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9533546

RESUMEN

Recent clinical observations indicate that ibuprofen may alleviate the radiation-induced dysuria that almost invariably occurs during radiation therapy for prostate cancer. Because the use of ibuprofen could consequently become common during radiation therapy for prostate cancer, we have been interested in the potential interactions between ibuprofen and ionizing radiation on prostate tumor cells. The effects of gamma-irradiation and/or ibuprofen on PC3 and DU-145 human prostate carcinoma cells were evaluated in vitro using three model systems. Clonogenic survival was determined by plating cells 24 h after treatment of nearly confluent monolayers. Analysis of cell growth, cell detachment, and apoptotic cell death was carried out over a period of up to 9 days after treatment of PC3 and DU-145 monolayers. The effect of ibuprofen and/or radiation was also probed by observing the inhibition of growth of established PC3 and DU-145 colonies that were treated on the 14th day of colony growth. Ibuprofen enhanced the radiation response of prostate cancer cells in all three in vitro models. Both the cytotoxic and radiosensitizing effects of ibuprofen seem to require concentrations that are higher than those reported to inhibit prostaglandin synthesis, suggesting that other molecular mechanisms may be responsible for ibuprofen cytotoxicity.


Asunto(s)
Antineoplásicos/toxicidad , Supervivencia Celular/efectos de los fármacos , Supervivencia Celular/efectos de la radiación , Ibuprofeno/toxicidad , Apoptosis/efectos de los fármacos , Apoptosis/efectos de la radiación , Adhesión Celular/efectos de los fármacos , Adhesión Celular/efectos de la radiación , División Celular/efectos de los fármacos , División Celular/efectos de la radiación , Fragmentación del ADN , Rayos gamma , Humanos , Masculino , Neoplasias de la Próstata/tratamiento farmacológico , Neoplasias de la Próstata/radioterapia , Factores de Tiempo , Células Tumorales Cultivadas , Ensayo de Tumor de Célula Madre
2.
Brain ; 105 (Pt 4): 735-54, 1982 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7139253

RESUMEN

This article extends our previous reports that multiple sclerosis can cause a visual dysfunction better described as a distortion than as a blurring of vision. An earlier paper reported that multiple sclerosis spares visual acuity in some patients while reducing visual sensitivity for less fine detail. Specifically, these patients experience a loss of contrast sensitivity for low and/or intermediate spatial frequencies, while contrast sensitivity for high spatial frequencies is unimpaired. We report here that some patients also lose spatial frequency discrimination, so that these patients cannot tell which of two clearly visible gratings has the higher spatial frequency even though control subjects accurately report which grating has the higher spatial frequency. One way of regarding this discrimination loss is in terms of a deterioration of the ability to discriminate size. Contrast sensitivity was measured over the spatial frequency range 1 to 20 cycles/deg using the von Békésy tracking method for 10 patients. (20 eyes) and 16 control subjects (32 eyes). The limit of normality was taken as 2.5 standard deviations from the control mean (99 per cent confidence). Spatial frequency discrimination was measured using the criterion-free method of temporal two-alternative forced choice over the spatial frequency range 2 to 16 cycles/deg for 10 patients (20 eyes), and for 14 to 26 control eyes at each spatial frequency. Three control subjects were studied more extensively over the range 1 to 20 cycles/deg. Control subjects could discriminate two spatial frequencies that differed by more than about 5 per cent. This held for all spatial frequencies tested. Grating contrast had little effect on discrimination, provided that all test gratings were clearly visible. The normal limit for discrimination threshold was set at 2.5 standard deviations from the control mean. Seven of 10 patients have abnormal contrast sensitivity at one or more spatial frequencies. Six of 10 patients had abnormal discrimination at one or more spatial frequencies. At any given spatial frequency the correlation between the magnitudes of sensitivity loss and discrimination loss was weak, though an eye that was less sensitive than its fellow also tended to have poorer discrimination. A more subtle relationship between sensitivity loss and discrimination loss was clearly shown by one patient. Sensitivity loss was restricted to spatial frequencies below 8 cycles/deg, while discrimination loss in the same eye was restricted to spatial frequencies above 8 cycles/deg. We propose that this finding can be straightforwardly understood if discrimination is determined by the relative activities of different spatial frequency channels analogously, to the way opponent-colour mechanisms determine colour discrimination.


Asunto(s)
Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Esclerosis Múltiple/fisiopatología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Ojo/fisiopatología , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Modelos Neurológicos , Fenómenos Fisiológicos Oculares , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Percepción del Tamaño/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Pruebas de Visión , Agudeza Visual , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiopatología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA