Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 56
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
BMC Public Health ; 24(1): 1102, 2024 Apr 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38649854

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: To determine the prevalence, risk factors; and impact on patient health and economic outcomes across the laterality spectrum of multiple sensory impairment (MSI) in a multi-ethnic older Asian population. METHODS: In this population-based study of Singaporeans aged ≥ 60 years, MSI was defined as concomitant vision (visual acuity > 0.3 logMAR), hearing (pure-tone air conduction average > 25 dB), and olfactory (score < 12 on the Sniffin' Sticks test) impairments across the spectrum of laterality (any, unilateral, combination [of unilateral and bilateral], and bilateral). RESULTS: Among 2,057 participants (mean ± SD 72.2 ± 0.2 years; 53.1% female), the national census-adjusted prevalence rates of any, unilateral, combination, and bilateral MSI were 20.6%, 1.2%, 12.2%, and 7.2%, respectively. Older age, male gender, low socioeconomic status (SES), and smoking (all p < 0.05) were independently associated with higher likelihood of any MSI. Compared to those with no sensory loss, those with MSI had significantly decreased mobility (range 5.4%-9.2%), had poor functioning (OR range 3.25-3.45) and increased healthcare costs (range 4-6 folds) across the laterality spectrum. Additionally, bilateral MSI had a significant decrease in HRQoL (5.5%, p = 0.012). CONCLUSIONS: MSI is a highly prevalent medical condition, with 1 in 5; and almost 1 in 10 community-dwelling older Asians having any and bilateral MSI, respectively, with a higher likelihood in men, smokers, and those with low SES. Critically, MSI has a substantial negative impact on patient health and economic outcomes across the laterality spectrum. Sensory testing is critical to detect and refer individuals with MSI for management to improve their functional independence and QoL.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Sensación , Humanos , Singapur/epidemiología , Femenino , Masculino , Anciano , Factores de Riesgo , Prevalencia , Persona de Mediana Edad , Trastornos de la Sensación/epidemiología , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Etnicidad/estadística & datos numéricos
2.
J Am Pharm Assoc (2003) ; 64(3): 102065, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38432477

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Financial, operational, and clinical workflow impacts of deploying an automated dispensing cabinet (ADC) in long-term care (LTC) facilities based on actual observations have not been documented in peer-reviewed literature. OBJECTIVES: To evaluate the impact of a closed-door pharmacy (CDP) implementing an ADC with unique secure, removable, and transportable locked pockets in an unstudied setting (LTC facilities) for management of first and emergency dose medications. PRACTICE DESCRIPTION: This study was conducted in 1 CDP and 2 LTC facilities. PRACTICE INNOVATION: Enhancing emergency medication management and inventory tracking in an unstudied setting through implementation of an ADC system featuring unique electronically encoded medication storage pockets that can be prepared in the CDP, locked and securely transported to the LTC, and when inserted into ADC it informs staff of its presence, position, and contents. EVALUATION METHODS: Mixed methods, pre- and poststudy to assess the impact of replacing manual emergency medication kits with an ADC. Outcomes were evaluated using rapid ethnography with workflow modeling; inventory and delivery reports; a nursing perception survey; and transactional data from the ADC during postimplementation phase. RESULTS: Pharmacy technician preparation time and pharmacist checking time decreased by 59% and 80%, respectively, and standing inventory was reduced by more than $10,000 combined for the CDP and 2 LTCs by replacing emergency medication kits with the ADC. In the LTCs, this change led to a 71% reduction in emergency medication retrieval time, an increase in emergency medication utilization, and a 96% reduction in the cost of unscheduled deliveries. Over 70% of the nurses surveyed favored replacement of the emergency medication kits with the ADC system. CONCLUSION: Replacing manual emergency medication kit with the described ADC system improved workflow efficiency in the CDP and LTC. It also significantly reduced unscheduled (STAT) deliveries and standing inventory and increased the availability of medications commonly used.


Asunto(s)
Cuidados a Largo Plazo , Farmacias , Flujo de Trabajo , Humanos , Farmacias/organización & administración , Administración del Tratamiento Farmacológico/organización & administración , Automatización , Servicios Farmacéuticos/organización & administración , Farmacéuticos/organización & administración
3.
Am J Obstet Gynecol ; 228(5S): S965-S976, 2023 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37164501

RESUMEN

In the United States, 98.3% of patients give birth in hospitals, 1.1% give birth at home, and 0.5% give birth in freestanding birth centers. This review investigated the impact of birth settings on birth outcomes in the United States. Presently, there are insufficient data to evaluate levels of maternal mortality and severe morbidity according to place of birth. Out-of-hospital births are associated with fewer interventions such as episiotomies, epidural anesthesia, operative deliveries, and cesarean deliveries. When compared with hospital births, there are increased rates of avoidable adverse perinatal outcomes in out-of-hospital births in the United States, both for those with and without risk factors. In one recent study, the neonatal mortality rates were significantly elevated for all planned home births: 13.66 per 10,000 live births (242/177,156; odds ratio, 4.19; 95% confidence interval, 3.62-4.84; P<.0001) vs 3.27 per 10,000 live births for in-hospital Certified Nurse-Midwife-attended births (745/2,280,044; odds ratio, 1). These differences increased further when patients were stratified by recognized risk factors such as breech presentation, multiple gestations, nulliparity, advanced maternal age, and postterm pregnancy. Causes of the increased perinatal morbidity and mortality include deliveries of patients with increased risks, absence of standardized criteria to exclude high-risk deliveries, and that most midwives attending out-of-hospital births in the United States do not meet the gold standard for midwifery regulation, the International Confederation of Midwives' Global Standards for Midwifery Education. As part of the informed consent process, pregnant patients interested in out-of-hospital births should be informed of its increased perinatal risks. Hospital births should be supported for all patients, especially those with increased risks.


Asunto(s)
Parto Domiciliario , Partería , Embarazo , Recién Nacido , Femenino , Humanos , Estados Unidos/epidemiología , Resultado del Embarazo/epidemiología , Entorno del Parto , Mortalidad Infantil
4.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 43(6): 1344-1355, 2023 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37392062

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To investigate the effect of low luminance on face recognition, specifically facial identity discrimination (FID) and facial expression recognition (FER), in adults with central vision loss (CVL) and peripheral vision loss (PVL) and to explore the association between clinical vision measures and low luminance FID and FER. METHODS: Participants included 33 adults with CVL, 17 with PVL and 20 controls. FID and FER were assessed under photopic and low luminance conditions. For the FID task, 12 sets of three faces with neutral expressions were presented and participants asked to indicate the odd-face-out. For FER, 12 single faces were presented and participants asked to name the expression (neutral, happy or angry). Photopic and low luminance visual acuity (VA) and contrast sensitivity (CS) were recorded for all participants and for the PVL group, Humphrey Field Analyzer (HFA) 24-2 mean deviation (MD). RESULTS: FID accuracy in CVL, and to a lesser extent PVL, was reduced under low compared with photopic luminance (mean reduction 20% and 8% respectively; p < 0.001). FER accuracy was reduced only in CVL (mean reduction 25%; p < 0.001). For both CVL and PVL, low luminance and photopic VA and CS were moderately to strongly correlated with low luminance FID (ρ = 0.61-0.77, p < 0.05). For PVL, better eye HFA 24-2 MD was moderately correlated with low luminance FID (ρ = 0.54, p = 0.02). Results were similar for low luminance FER. Together, photopic VA and CS explained 75% of the variance in low luminance FID, and photopic VA explained 61% of the variance in low luminance FER. Low luminance vision measures explained little additional variance. CONCLUSION: Low luminance significantly reduced face recognition, particularly for adults with CVL. Worse VA and CS were associated with reduced face recognition. Clinically, photopic VA is a good predictor of face recognition under low luminance conditions.

5.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 42(4): 872-878, 2022 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35366354

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Red signals signify danger in a range of situations, including train operations. Importantly, misperception of a red signal as yellow can have serious safety implications. This study investigated the effects of lens blur on incorrect colour perception of red signals, which has been implicated in previous train crashes. METHODS: Participants included 15 young (26.6 ± 4.6 years) and 15 older (55.8 ± 3.1 years) visually normal adults. Red and yellow wayside train signals were simulated for two brightness levels (dim, bright) using a custom-built projection system. The effect of blur (best-corrected refraction [No Blur], +0.25 DS, +0.50 DS, +0.75 DS, +1.00 DS, +1.25 DS) on the number of incorrect colour perception responses of the signals was recorded. The order of conditions was randomised between participants. RESULTS: For incorrect responses to the red signal, there were significant main effects of blur (p < 0.001) and signal brightness (p < 0.001) and a significant interaction between blur and brightness (p < 0.001). The effects of blur were greater for the dim compared to the bright signals, with significantly higher colour misperceptions for the dim signal for +0.50 DS blur and higher, compared with No Blur. Colour misperceptions of the yellow signals were low compared with that of the red signals, with only +1.25 DS blur resulting in a significantly higher number of incorrect responses than No Blur (p < 0.001). There were no effects of age for the red or yellow colour misperceptions (p > 0.19). CONCLUSIONS: Low levels of blur (+0.50 DS to +1.25 DS) resulted in a significant misperception of the red signals as orange-yellow, particularly for dim signals. The findings have implications for vision testing and refractive correction of train drivers to minimise the possibility of colour misperception of red train signals.


Asunto(s)
Errores de Refracción , Color , Humanos , Refracción Ocular , Pruebas de Visión , Agudeza Visual
6.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 41(2): 245-254, 2021 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33368495

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To investigate the prevalence and level of concern about falling (CF) among older people with vision impairment due to age-related macular degeneration (AMD) compared to a visually normal control group, and to identify determinants of CF for the AMD group. METHODS: Participants included 133 older people: 77 with AMD (mean age = 80.5 ± 6.2 years), and 56 controls (mean age = 75.4 ± 5.3 years). Binocular visual acuity, contrast sensitivity and visual fields were measured, and CF was assessed using the Falls Efficacy Scale - International (FES-I). Data were also collected for sensorimotor function (postural sway, sit-to-stand, knee extensions, walking speed, proprioception), and neuropsychological function (reaction time, symptoms of anxiety and depression) using validated tests and scales. RESULTS: Concern about falling scores were higher for AMD participants compared to control participants (mean ± S.D. 24.6 ± 8.0 vs 21.6 ± 5.7, p = 0.02, respectively), although these findings failed to reach significance when adjusted for age (p = 0.16). Among AMD participants, multivariable models showed that greater CF was associated with reduced contrast sensitivity (p = 0.02), slower sit-to-stand times (p < 0.001) and higher anxiety scores (p < 0.001); these factors explained 40% of the variance in CF (p < 0.01). CONCLUSION: Levels of CF in older people with AMD were not found to be elevated by their disease status alone, but rather by the extent of vision loss. Levels of CF in those with AMD were associated with various visual, sensorimotor and neuropsychological factors. These findings will assist clinicians in identifying those at greatest risk of developing high CF and inform the design of future intervention programmes for this population.


Asunto(s)
Accidentes por Caídas/estadística & datos numéricos , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Degeneración Macular/complicaciones , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Baja Visión/etiología , Agudeza Visual , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Cognición/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Incidencia , Degeneración Macular/diagnóstico , Degeneración Macular/fisiopatología , Masculino , Nueva Gales del Sur/epidemiología , Baja Visión/diagnóstico , Baja Visión/epidemiología , Caminata
7.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 41(4): 853-863, 2021 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33878195

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To investigate the effect of low light levels on postural stability in older adults with and without age-related macular degeneration (AMD). METHODS: Participants included 28 older adults [14 with AMD (mean age ± S.D., 83.4 ± 6.7 years) and 14 controls with normal vision (74.6 ± 3.3 years)]. Postural stability was assessed with eyes open on both a firm and foam surface under four lighting conditions in a randomised order: photopic (~436 lux, vertically at the eye), sudden reduction to mesopic (~436 to ~1 lux), adapted mesopic (~1 lux) and adapted mesopic with a light emitting diode (LED) door frame lighting system (~1.3 lux), using the root mean square (RMS) of the centre of pressure measures derived from an electronic force plate in the anterior-posterior (AP) and medio-lateral (ML) directions. Visual function was assessed binocularly (visual acuity, contrast sensitivity and visual fields), physical function was assessed using standardised measures (sit-to-stand, grip strength and the timed walk test) and self-reported difficulties under low light levels were recorded using the Low Luminance Questionnaire. Data were analysed using linear mixed models. RESULTS: For all participants, low light levels significantly increased postural sway on the foam surface in the AP (p = 0.01) but not ML (p = 0.80) direction, but had no effect on postural stability on the firm surface. On the foam surface, while AP-RMS sway was significantly greater in the sudden (p < 0.001) and adapted (p = 0.02) mesopic compared to the photopic condition, sway for the adapted mesopic with the LED lighting system was not significantly different to the photopic condition (p = 0.20). On the foam surface, AP-RMS (p = 0.02) and ML-RMS (p < 0.001) sway were significantly greater in the AMD compared to the control group. None of the measures of visual function was significantly associated with AP- or ML-RMS sway. CONCLUSIONS: On the foam surface, low light levels significantly reduced postural stability in older adults with and without AMD, and postural stability was significantly reduced for the AMD group compared to controls, regardless of light level. Importantly, the LED lighting system reduced sway under mesopic conditions, which was not significantly greater than that measured under photopic conditions in either group. These findings have important implications for enhancing the visual environment for older adults with and without AMD to improve postural stability and reduce the risk of falls in low lighting environments.


Asunto(s)
Visión de Colores , Degeneración Macular , Anciano , Sensibilidad de Contraste , Humanos , Degeneración Macular/diagnóstico , Equilibrio Postural , Agudeza Visual , Campos Visuales
8.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 41(5): 1134-1143, 2021 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34431543

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Eye movements are integral to the reading process. This study characterised the eye movement patterns of children differentiated by their reading ability, while completing a saccadic eye movement test with irregular target spacing (Development Eye Movement (DEM) test) using a novel eye movement classification algorithm. METHODS: Participants included 196 Grade 2 Australian schoolchildren (mean age: 7.9 ± 0.3 years) who completed a computerised version of the DEM test, while their eye movements were recorded (Tobii TX300 eye-tracker). Children also completed a standardised reading comprehension test, which categorised them into below average and average or above reading ability groups. A novel eye movement classification algorithm was developed that considered the vertical and horizontal eye movements of each child. RESULTS: Compared to children with average or above reading ability, the below average reading group displayed poor vertical eye movement control, demonstrated by a significantly greater proportion of interline eye movements (vertical eye movements away from the current line) (p < 0.001). Differences in horizontal eye movements were also observed, with below average readers demonstrating a smaller proportion of expected forward saccades (p < 0.001) (within-line forward saccades with horizontal amplitude between the minimum and maximum horizontal spacing between digits), while this group also displayed longer fixation durations (p = 0.001). The below average reading group demonstrated significantly poorer results on all standard DEM metrics than the average or above reading ability group: horizontal subtest time (p < 0.001), vertical subtest time (p = 0.004) and ratio (p = 0.004). CONCLUSIONS: Children exhibiting below average reading ability were poorer at maintaining control of vertical (interline), as well as horizontal, eye movements compared to children with average or above reading ability. Future studies should explore the mechanisms underlying these differences, particularly in vertical eye movements, given that reading paragraphs (involving multiple lines of text) requires accurate eye movements in both the vertical and horizontal direction.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Lectura , Australia , Niño , Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Movimientos Sacádicos
9.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 41(4): 798-807, 2021 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33877691

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To investigate the prevalence of mental health conditions and burnout among practising optometrists in Australia. METHODS: A cross-sectional survey of registered practising Australian optometrists was undertaken over a three-week period from mid-November 2019. The survey comprised three well-established mental health scales (Kessler Psychological Distress Scale [K10], Depression Anxiety Stress Scales [DASS-21] and Maslach Burnout Inventory [MBI]) and an open-ended question inviting comments. RESULTS: Five hundred and five respondents completed the K10, representing 8.8% of registered optometrists in Australia; 466 completed all three scales. Prevalence of moderate to severe psychological distress (K10 ≥ 25) was 30.7% (95% CI 26.7%-34.7%), with similar findings for depression and anxiety (DASS-21). Prevalence of high burnout, as indicated by MBI-GS exhaustion was 56.1% (95% CI 51.7%-60.4%), cynicism 57.1% (95% CI 52.7%-61.5%) and professional efficacy 23.1% (95% CI 19.4%-26.8%). Optometrists aged ≤ 30 years were 3.5 times more likely to report moderate to severe psychological distress compared to optometrists aged >30 years (OR = 3.54, P < 0.001, 95% CI 2.38-5.25). The most frequently mentioned work-related issues concerned retail pressures, workload and career dissatisfaction. CONCLUSIONS: The rates of mental health conditions and burnout reported by practising Australian optometrists were high compared with the general population and other health professionals. Younger age and burnout were significant risk factors for psychological distress. Interventions are required to address these issues, particularly for younger optometrists, and could include workplace modifications and building resilience to improve personal mental wellbeing and ensure patient safety.


Asunto(s)
Salud Mental , Optometristas , Australia/epidemiología , Estudios Transversales , Humanos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
10.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 41(3): 582-590, 2021 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33772849

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Many individuals drive with uncorrected refractive errors, which has implications for night driving, where poor visibility contributes to the increased crash risk relative to daytime. This study explored how small amounts of refractive blur affects the judgment of the walking direction of night-time pedestrians and whether different types of retro-reflective clothing influence this effect. METHODS: Judgement of the walking direction of night-time pedestrians was investigated for 20 young participants with normal vision (mean age, 21.8 ± 1.6 years) for two levels of binocular blur (+0.50DS, +1.00DS) compared to baseline (best-corrected refractive correction). Participants seated in a stationary car with low beam headlamps observed a pedestrian wearing three clothing conditions: retro-reflective vest (1) and retro-reflective biomotion clothing (incorporating thin (2) or thick (3) retro-reflective strips), who walked across the road in three directions (straight across, away or towards the car). The order of conditions was randomised among participants. Participants reported the perceived pedestrian walking direction and how confident they rated their response. Outcome measures included the proportion of correct responses (response accuracy) and confidence ratings. RESULTS: Blur had a significant effect on accuracy in judging pedestrian walking direction; accuracy decreased significantly with increasing blur (p < 0.001), with all blur levels being significantly different from one another. The effect of pedestrian clothing was also significant (p < 0.001); direction judgements were least accurate for vest, followed by both thin and thick biomotion, where accuracy judgements were similar. There was also a significant interaction between blur and clothing (p < 0.05). Similar trends were found for confidence ratings across the blur and clothing conditions. Greater confidence was significantly associated with increased response accuracy for the biomotion clothing, but not the vest. CONCLUSION: Findings highlight that even small amounts of blur (+0.50DS), that do not reduce visual acuity below the legal driving limits, reduce the ability to accurately judge pedestrian walking direction at night. Retro-reflective clothing in a biomotion configuration facilitated the highest accuracy and confidence in judgment of pedestrian walking direction, for both thin and thick strips compared to vest, for all blur levels.


Asunto(s)
Accidentes de Tránsito/prevención & control , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Juicio , Visión Nocturna/fisiología , Peatones , Errores de Refracción/fisiopatología , Caminata/fisiología , Adulto , Conducción de Automóvil , Femenino , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Ropa de Protección/provisión & distribución , Tiempo de Reacción , Pruebas de Visión , Agudeza Visual , Adulto Joven
11.
Optom Vis Sci ; 96(7): 484-491, 2019 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31274736

RESUMEN

SIGNIFICANCE: Glaucoma has been shown to impair hazard detection ability and increase crash risk compared to controls. Differences in visual search behavior of the driving scene may explain these differences; however, there has been limited investigation of this issue with inconsistent findings. PURPOSE: Through eye movement tracking of older drivers with glaucoma, we explored their visual search behavior in comparison with controls while performing the DriveSafe, a slide recognition test purported to predict fitness to drive. METHODS: Thirty-one drivers with glaucoma (mean age, 71.7 ± 6.3 years; average better-eye mean defect,-3 dB; average worse-eye mean defect,-12 dB) and 25 age-matched controls underwent measurements of their visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, visual fields, and useful field of view (visual processing speeds). Participants' eye movements were recorded while they completed the DriveSafe test, which consists of brief presentations of static, real-world driving scenes containing various road users (pedestrians, bicycles, vehicles). Participants reported the types, positions, and direction of travel of road users in each image; the score was the total number of correctly reported items (maximum, 128). RESULTS: Drivers with glaucoma had significantly worse DriveSafe scores (P = .03), fixated on road users for shorter durations (P < .001), and exhibited smaller saccades (P = .02) compared with controls. For all participants, longer fixation times on road users (P < .001) was the eye movement measure most strongly associated with better DriveSafe scores; this relationship was not significantly different between groups. Useful field-of-view divided attention was the strongest visual predictor of DriveSafe scores. CONCLUSIONS: Eye movement changes in the glaucoma group may reflect increased difficulty in identifying relevant objects in the visual scene, which may be related to their lower DriveSafe scores. Given the DriveSafe's potential utility in assessing drivers with visual impairment before on-road testing, further investigations on how DriveSafe performance and eye movement patterns compare to those during on-road driving are warranted.


Asunto(s)
Conducción de Automóvil , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Glaucoma/fisiopatología , Procesamiento Espacial/fisiología , Anciano , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Pruebas de Visión , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Pruebas del Campo Visual , Campos Visuales/fisiología
12.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 39(3): 141-147, 2019 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30994200

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Screening for uncorrected hyperopia in school children is important given its association with poorer visual function and academic performance. However, standard distance visual acuity screening may not detect low to moderate hyperopia. The plus lens test is used to screen for hyperopia in many school screening protocols, but has not been well validated. The current study investigated the effectiveness of the plus lens test to identify hyperopia in school children. METHODS: Participants included Grade 2 school children. Monocular distance visual acuity (logMAR letter chart) was measured unaided, and then through a +1.50D lens, known as the plus lens test. Cycloplegic refraction was undertaken to classify moderate hyperopia (≥+2.00D). Sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive values (PPV) and negative predictive values (NPV) were calculated for commonly used cut-offs for the plus lens test: 6/6, 6/9 and less than two lines difference between unaided acuity and acuity through the plus lens test. RESULTS: The sample included 59 children (mean age 7.2 ± 0.4 years). Fourteen (24%) children were classified as having uncorrected hyperopia. The sensitivity and specificity of the +1.50 plus lens test for identifying hyperopia were 0% and 98% respectively for a 6/6 cut-off, 29% and 91% for 6/9 cut-off, and 50% and 76% for a <2 line reduction between unaided acuity and acuity through the plus lens test. Receiver Operating Curve (ROC) analysis revealed area under curves of 0.69 based on acuity through the plus lens test, and 0.65 for a reduction in acuity through the plus lens test. CONCLUSIONS: The plus lens test has low sensitivity for detecting uncorrected hyperopia using traditional cut-offs of 6/9 or better. This raises questions about the role of the plus lens test in school screening batteries.


Asunto(s)
Hiperopía/diagnóstico , Lentes , Óptica y Fotónica/métodos , Refracción Ocular/fisiología , Selección Visual/métodos , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
14.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 39(5): 350-357, 2019 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31378990

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To investigate the effect of uncorrected astigmatism on night driving performance on a closed-road circuit. METHODS: Participants included 10 drivers (mean age 24.4 ± 7.0 years), with low to moderate bilateral astigmatism (0.75-1.50 DC), who were regular contact lens (CL) wearers. Vision and night driving performance were assessed in a cross-over design with a toric CL and a best-sphere spherical CL. Binocular visual function measures included photopic and mesopic visual acuity (VA), contrast sensitivity (CS), mesopic motion sensitivity and glare tests (Mesotest® II and Halometer). Night-time driving performance was assessed on a closed-road circuit, which included measures of sign recognition, hazard detection and avoidance, pedestrian recognition distances, lane keeping, speed and overall driving score. RESULTS: Correction of astigmatism with toric CL significantly improved mesopic VA, photopic and mesopic CS, mesopic motion sensitivity, and reduced glare (p < 0.05), compared to the spherical CL; there were no significant effects of visual correction type on photopic VA. Correction of astigmatism using toric CL resulted in significant improvements in night driving performance, compared to driving with spherical CL, particularly for sign recognition, avoidance of low contrast hazards, increased pedestrian recognition distances and overall driving score (p < 0.05). CONCLUSIONS: Correction of low to moderate levels of astigmatism had significant positive effects on night-time driving performance and related tests of visual performance. This has important implications for optical corrections to improve night road safety of drivers with astigmatism.


Asunto(s)
Astigmatismo/fisiopatología , Conducción de Automóvil/estadística & datos numéricos , Adulto , Estudios Cruzados , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas de Visión , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
15.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 38(5): 516-524, 2018 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30221376

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Vision is considered important for academic performance in children; however, the evidence in this area tends to be inconsistent and inconclusive. This study explored the association between vision function and visual information processing measures and standardised academic achievement scores in Grade 3 Australian children. METHODS: Participants included 108 Grade 3 primary school children (M = 8.82 ± 0.32 years) from three state primary schools in South-East Queensland. All participants underwent a standard vision screening, including distance visual acuity (VA), binocular vision testing and stereoacuity (SA). A computer-based battery of visual information processing tests including the Development Eye Movement (DEM) test, Visual Sequential Memory (VSM) and Symbol Search (SS) was also administered. Australian National Assessment Program for Literacy and Numeracy (NAPLAN) scores across five subtests of academic performance were obtained for each child: Reading, Writing, Spelling, Grammar/Punctuation and Numeracy. RESULTS: The DEM adjusted horizontal and vertical times were most strongly associated with all of the NAPLAN subtest scores (p < 0.01), adjusted for age and the socio-economic status of the school; the DEM ratio was not significantly associated with any of the NAPLAN subtests. VSM and SS scores were significantly associated with one or more NAPLAN subtests, as were worse and better eye VA; SA showed no significant association with any of the NAPLAN subtests. CONCLUSIONS: Performance on the horizontal and vertical DEM subtests was most strongly associated with academic performance. These data, in conjunction with other clinical data, can provide useful information to clinicians regarding their prescribing and management philosophy for children with lower levels of uncorrected refractive error and binocular vision anomalies.


Asunto(s)
Rendimiento Académico/normas , Lectura , Errores de Refracción/fisiopatología , Instituciones Académicas , Selección Visual/métodos , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Incidencia , Masculino , Queensland/epidemiología , Errores de Refracción/diagnóstico , Errores de Refracción/epidemiología
16.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 37(2): 184-190, 2017 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28211184

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Drivers' responses and eye movements were assessed as they approached pedestrians at night in order to explore the relative conspicuity benefits of different configurations of retroreflective markings. METHODS: Eye movements were recorded using an ASL Mobile Eye (Applied Science Technologies, www.asleyetracking.com) from 14 young licensed drivers (M = 24.1 ± 6.4 years) as they drove along a closed-road circuit at night. At two locations, pedestrians walked in place facing either towards or away from the road. Pedestrians wore black clothing with a standard retroreflective vest either alone or with additional retroreflective markers positioned in a configuration conveying biological motion (biomotion). Drivers responded when they recognised that a pedestrian was present and again when they identified the direction the pedestrian was facing. RESULTS: Drivers recognised pedestrians from a significantly greater distance (p < 0.05) when the pedestrian's clothing included the biomotion configuration (319.1 m) than when the biomotion markings were absent (184.5 m). Further, the drivers recognised the direction that the pedestrian faced from a longer distance when biomotion markings were present (215.4 m vs 95.6 m). Eye movement data suggested that the biomotion configuration attracted drivers' attention significantly sooner than the vest (time to first fixation: 1.1 vs 3.5 s), that drivers fixated on pedestrians wearing biomotion for significantly less time prior to responding (3.4 s vs 6.1 s), and the time to first recognise a pedestrian was approximately half that for biomotion compared to vest (6.4 vs 13.9 s). CONCLUSION: Adding biomotion reflectors to the vest facilitated earlier recognition of pedestrians and faster identification of the direction that the pedestrian faced. These findings confirm that the conspicuity advantages of biomotion configurations on pedestrians at night result in part from drivers fixating pedestrians earlier and more efficiently.


Asunto(s)
Conducción de Automóvil , Oscuridad , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Visión Nocturna/fisiología , Ropa de Protección , Adolescente , Adulto , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Adulto Joven
17.
Optom Vis Sci ; 93(9): 1137-46, 2016 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27281681

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To examine the effects of optical blur, auditory distractors, and age on eye movement patterns while performing a driving hazard perception test (HPT). METHODS: Twenty young (mean age 27.1 ± 4.6 years) and 20 older (73.3 ± 5.7 years) drivers with normal vision completed a HPT in a repeated-measures counterbalanced design while their eye movements were recorded. Testing was performed under two visual (best-corrected vision and with +2.00DS blur) and two distractor (with and without auditory distraction) conditions. Participants were required to respond to road hazards appearing in the HPT videos of real-world driving scenes and their hazard response times were recorded. RESULTS: Blur and distractors each significantly delayed hazard response time by 0.42 and 0.76 s, respectively (p < 0.05). A significant interaction between age and distractors indicated that older drivers were more affected by distractors than young drivers (response with distractors delayed by 0.96 and 0.60 s, respectively). There were no other two- or three-way interaction effects on response time. With blur, for example, both groups fixated significantly longer on hazards before responding compared to best-corrected vision. In the presence of distractors, both groups exhibited delayed first fixation on the hazards and spent less time fixating on the hazards. There were also significant differences in eye movement characteristics between groups, where older drivers exhibited smaller saccades, delayed first fixation on hazards, and shorter fixation duration on hazards compared to the young drivers. CONCLUSIONS: Collectively, the findings of delayed hazard response times and alterations in eye movement patterns with blur and distractors provide further evidence that visual impairment and distractors are independently detrimental to driving safety given that delayed hazard response times are linked to increased crash risk.


Asunto(s)
Conducción de Automóvil , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Seguridad , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad
18.
Optom Vis Sci ; 93(6): 560-6, 2016 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26945174

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To examine the effects of gaze position and optical blur, similar to that used in multifocal corrections, on stepping accuracy for a precision stepping task among older adults. METHODS: Nineteen healthy older adults (mean age, 71.6 ± 8.8 years) with normal vision performed a series of precision stepping tasks onto a fixed target. The stepping tasks were performed using a repeated-measures design for three gaze positions (fixating on the stepping target as well as 30 and 60 cm farther forward of the stepping target) and two visual conditions (best-corrected vision and with +2.50DS blur). Participants' gaze position was tracked using a head-mounted eye tracker. Absolute, anteroposterior, and mediolateral foot placement errors and within-subject foot placement variability were calculated from the locations of foot and floor-mounted retroreflective markers captured by flash photography of the final foot position. RESULTS: Participants made significantly larger absolute and anteroposterior foot placement errors and exhibited greater foot placement variability when their gaze was directed farther forward of the stepping target. Blur led to significantly increased absolute and anteroposterior foot placement errors and increased foot placement variability. Furthermore, blur differentially increased the absolute and anteroposterior foot placement errors and variability when gaze was directed 60 cm farther forward of the stepping target. CONCLUSIONS: Increasing gaze position farther ahead from stepping locations and the presence of blur negatively impact the stepping accuracy of older adults. These findings indicate that blur, similar to that used in multifocal corrections, has the potential to increase the risk of trips and falls among older populations when negotiating challenging environments where precision stepping is required, particularly as gaze is directed farther ahead from stepping locations when walking.


Asunto(s)
Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Errores de Refracción/fisiopatología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Caminata/fisiología , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Agudeza Visual/fisiología
19.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 36(4): 465-76, 2016 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27350185

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Night-time driving difficulties are a common concern of older drivers and those with eye disease. This study aimed to develop and validate a questionnaire for assessing vision-related night driving difficulties in older drivers. METHODS: Items from existing vision-related quality of life questionnaires and driving studies were used to develop a questionnaire that was completed by 283 participants who reported visual difficulties for night driving (65% female, 50 to >80 years). The questionnaire included items relating to demographic and night driving characteristics (seven items), general vision ratings (eight items), vision-related night driving difficulties (11 items), and a single open question about specific night driving difficulties. The vision-related night driving difficulty items were analysed separately using Rasch analysis to form the vision and night driving questionnaire (VND-Q). Rasch analysis assessed validity and psychometric properties of the scale. Generalised linear regression models examined associations between VND-Q scores and age, gender, amount of night driving, self-rated vision, and eye conditions. Test-retest repeatability was assessed using intra-class correlation analysis and Bland-Altman methods of agreement for a subset of 30 participants. RESULTS: Rasch analysis indicated that a nine-item VND-Q scale was unidimensional, valid and reliable, and showed excellent discriminant ability (person separation index 3.04; person reliability 0.90). Targeting was better for those with greater self-reported night driving difficulties. Participants with self-reported bilateral eye conditions and worse self-reported general vision ratings had significantly more night driving difficulties with the VND-Q scale than individuals without eye conditions (p = 0.03) and with better general vision ratings (p < 0.001). Females reported more difficulties than males (p < 0.001) and drove shorter distances at night per week which was also associated with greater difficulties (p < 0.001). A repeatability coefficient (Rc ) of 2.07 demonstrated excellent test-retest repeatability. CONCLUSIONS: The nine-item VND-Q is a unidimensional and reliable questionnaire allowing quantification of the level of visual difficulties that older drivers report at night. The development of this questionnaire is an important step in providing a reliable and validated instrument for use to guide appropriate investigations, referrals, or interventions in clinical and research settings.


Asunto(s)
Conducción de Automóvil/psicología , Visión Nocturna/fisiología , Psicometría/métodos , Calidad de Vida , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Agudeza Visual , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
20.
Optom Vis Sci ; 92(6): 730-5, 2015 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25930978

RESUMEN

Falls are the leading cause of injury-related morbidity and mortality among older adults. In addition to the resulting physical injury and potential disability after a fall, there are also important psychological consequences, including depression, anxiety, activity restriction, and fear of falling. Fear of falling affects 20 to 43% of community-dwelling older adults and is not limited to those who have previously experienced a fall. About half of older adults who experience fear of falling subsequently restrict their physical and everyday activities, which can lead to functional decline, depression, increased falls risk, and reduced quality of life. Although there is clear evidence that older adults with visual impairment have higher falls risk, only a limited number of studies have investigated fear of falling in older adults with visual impairment and the findings have been mixed. Recent studies suggest increased levels of fear of falling among older adults with various eye conditions, including glaucoma and age-related macular degeneration, whereas other studies have failed to find differences. Interventions, which are still in their infancy in the general population, are also largely unexplored in those with visual impairment. The major aims of this review were to provide an overview of the literature on fear of falling, its measurement, and risk factors among older populations, with specific focus on older adults with visual impairment, and to identify directions for future research in this area.


Asunto(s)
Accidentes por Caídas/estadística & datos numéricos , Trastornos de la Visión/psicología , Personas con Daño Visual/psicología , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Calidad de Vida/psicología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA