Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 193
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Infect Dis ; 2024 May 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38768246

RESUMEN

The ongoing COVID-19 pandemic continues to present surges in infections demonstrating the persistent threat posed by COVID-19. Amidst efforts to develop effective treatments for acute COVID-19, there is a growing recognition of the need to address Long COVID. This manuscript reviews the current landscape of acute COVID-19 treatments and highlights the opportunity to incorporate Long COVID as a key outcome measure in clinical trials. Our analysis includes a review of current US clinical trials investigating acute COVID-19 treatments. Of the 19 studies that met our inclusion criteria, only one study currently incorporates Long COVID measurements. In our review, we argue there are 7 compelling reasons to include Long Covid measurements, including: (1) Long COVID is not rare (2) Long COVID is debilitating to individuals and has a high societal cost (3) Those at high risk of severe COVID-19 are also at higher risk of developing Long COVID if they are infected with COVID-19 (4) Treatments for acute COVID-19 may reduce the risk of Long COVID (5) Measures exist to track Long COVID (6) Long COVID considerations are potentially important for acute COVID-19 treatment decision-making (7) Deaths and hospitalizations due to COVID-19 are increasingly rare, creating an opportunity for other important clinical endpoints to be considered In conclusion, while not every trial needs to include assessments of Long COVID, it is worth the research burden to include assessments where possible as this could facilitate the uptake of acute COVID-19 treatments that lessen the societal burden of Long COVID.

2.
Clin Trials ; 18(4): 466-476, 2021 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33892597

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND/AIMS: Many investigators have tested interventions to improve research participant understanding of information shared during the informed consent process, using a variety of methods and with mixed results. A valid criticism of most consent research is that studies are often conducted in simulated research settings rather than ongoing clinical studies. The present study rigorously tested two simple and easily adoptable strategies for presenting key consent information to participants eligible to enroll in six actual clinical trials (i.e. six parent studies). METHODS: In collaboration with the study team from each parent study, we developed two consent interventions: a fact sheet and an interview-style video. The content of each of the intervention was based on the information shared in the consent form approved for each parent study. Participants were randomized to the standard consent process, or to one of the two interventions. Once exposed to the assigned consent mode, participants were asked to complete an assessment of understanding. The study was powered to determine whether those exposed to the fact sheet or video performed better on the consent assessment compared to those exposed to the standard consent. We also assessed participant satisfaction with the consent process. RESULTS: A total of 284 participants were randomized to one of the three consent arms. Assessments of understanding were completed with a total of 273 participants from July 2017 to April 2019. Participants exposed to the video had better understanding scores compared to those exposed to the standard consent form process (p value = 0.020). Participants were more satisfied with the video when compared to the standard consent. Participants who received the fact sheet did not achieve higher overall understanding or satisfaction scores when compared to the standard consent process. CONCLUSION: This randomized study of two novel consent interventions across six different clinical trials demonstrated a statistically significant difference in participant understanding based on overall scores among those exposed to the video intervention compared to those exposed to the standard consent.


Asunto(s)
Formularios de Consentimiento , Consentimiento Informado , Comprensión , Humanos , Grabación en Video
4.
Psychol Res ; 84(1): 177-191, 2020 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29318375

RESUMEN

Action-compatibility effects (ACEs) arise due to incongruity between perceptuo-motor traces stored in memory and the perceptuo-motor demands of a retrieval task. Recent research has suggested that ACEs arising during spatial memory retrieval are additionally modulated by individual differences in how experienced participants are with a college campus environment. However, the extent and nature of experience with a real-world environment is difficult to assess and control, and characteristics of the retrieval task itself might modulate ACEs during spatial memory retrieval. The present study provides a more controlled and in-depth examination of how individual differences and task-based factors interact to shape ACEs when participants retrieve spatial memories. In two experiments, participants with varied video game experience learned a virtual environment and then used the computer mouse to verify spatial relationships from different perspectives. Mouse trajectories demonstrated ACEs, differing by retrieval perspective and video game experience. Videogame experts demonstrated the ACE based on learned spatial relationships during egocentric retrieval only, whereas videogame novices showed the ACE based on semantic processing of directional terms only. Specifically, gaming experts invoke perspective-specific perceptuo-motor associations to retrieve spatial knowledge, whereas non-experts are influenced by semantically based associations specific to the retrieval task. Results are discussed in the context of action-compatibility effects, the intentional weighting hypothesis, and the flexible encoding and retrieval of spatial information.


Asunto(s)
Actividad Motora/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Aprendizaje Espacial/fisiología , Memoria Espacial/fisiología , Navegación Espacial/fisiología , Juegos de Video , Realidad Virtual , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estudiantes , Universidades , Adulto Joven
5.
Cogn Process ; 21(2): 287-302, 2020 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31974762

RESUMEN

Finding one's way to a destination is a common, everyday task that often relies on spatial information provided by humans and/or automatic devices. However, the information can be inaccurate. How we decide which route to take will depend on our thoughts about the available route information, including who or what provided it, and how these sources may be associated with differential accuracy and fallibility. In three experiments (previously reported in Brunyé et al. (Q J Exper Psychol 68(3):585-607, 2015)), we found that when route directions conflicted with the perceived environment, people trusted the landmark information other humans provided, but relied on the turn direction information from an automatic device. But what guides these behavioral results? Here we present a systematic linguistic analysis of retrospective reports that sheds some light on how information about the direction source affects cognitive focus. A focus on direction sources in the instruction triggered a cognitive focus on the direction source throughout. Participants who systematically switched strategies focused more on features of the scenario than those who did not. Non-switching strategies were associated with a higher focus on the participants' own reasoning processes, in particular when relying on turn information. These results highlight how cognitive focus is guided by scenario factors and individual preferences, triggering inferences that influence decisions.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones , Navegación Espacial , Adulto , Cognición , Femenino , Humanos , Solución de Problemas , Estudios Retrospectivos , Incertidumbre
7.
Behav Res Methods ; 51(2): 602-638, 2019 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30788797

RESUMEN

Individuals with better spatial thinking have increased interest and greater achievement in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) disciplines (Wai, Lubinski, & Benbow in Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 817-835, 2009). This relationship means that STEM education may benefit from leveraging spatial thinking, but measures of spatial thinking as they relate to specific STEM disciplines are needed. The present work presents an assessment of spatial and mathematical reasoning, called Make-A-Dice. In Make-A-Dice, individuals are presented with a cube net (i.e., a flattened cube) with numbers on two sides. Their goal is to "make a dice" by filling in the blank sides using two rules: opposite sides add to 7, and the numbers 1 through 6 should be used once each. Make-A-Dice was given to adults (Study 1) and elementary students (Studies 2 and 3) along with math, spatial, and other measures, across two sessions in all studies. Make-A-Dice had both internal and test-retest reliability, with items ordered by difficulty. Furthermore, performance was related to spatial and mathematical reasoning. In Study 1, adults reported a range of strategies used to complete Make-A-Dice, and one strategy predicted performance. Studies 2 and 3 showed that Make-A-Dice is age-appropriate for elementary students. Make-A-Dice shows promise as an individual-difference measure linking spatial and mathematical thinking and has the potential to identify elementary-aged children who may benefit from spatial training.


Asunto(s)
Matemática , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Percepción Espacial , Pensamiento , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Solución de Problemas , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Adulto Joven
9.
Am J Public Health ; 108(10): 1341-1344, 2018 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30138065

RESUMEN

Although advances in treatment and diagnosis have transformed HIV into a chronic disease in high-income countries, a spectrum of structural, political, sociocultural, and health system barriers hamper early diagnosis and timely treatment of HIV in many middle- and low-income countries. In most Latin American countries, in spite of the great improvement in access to antiretroviral therapy, a large proportion of individuals infected with HIV do not know their status. In Colombia, the Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS currently estimates a much larger number of HIV cases than the number reported by Colombian authorities. Potential reasons for underdiagnosis and underreporting include sociocultural factors such as social stigma, restrictions in access to health care, a lack of public health research and robust surveillance systems, and the particular recent history and social situation related to the armed conflict the country has suffered through for several decades. Lessons from Colombia may be helpful in monitoring, understanding, and tackling the HIV epidemic in countries with long-term armed conflicts.


Asunto(s)
Conflictos Armados , Recolección de Datos/métodos , Infecciones por VIH/epidemiología , Disparidades en Atención de Salud , Adulto , Colombia/epidemiología , Países en Desarrollo , Femenino , Accesibilidad a los Servicios de Salud , Humanos , Masculino , Vigilancia de la Población , Estigma Social , Naciones Unidas
10.
Mem Cognit ; 46(5): 809-825, 2018 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29383599

RESUMEN

Research examining object identity and location processing in visuo-spatial working memory (VSWM) has yielded inconsistent results on whether age differences exist in VSWM. The present study investigated whether these inconsistencies may stem from age-related differences in VSWM sub-processes, and whether processing of component VSWM information can be facilitated. In two experiments, younger and older adults studied 5 × 5 grids containing five objects in separate locations. In a continuous recognition paradigm, participants were tested on memory for object identity, location, or identity and location information combined. Spatial and categorical relationships were manipulated within grids to provide trial-level facilitation. In Experiment 1, randomizing trial types (location, identity, combination) assured that participants could not predict the information that would be queried. In Experiment 2, blocking trials by type encouraged strategic processing. Thus, we manipulated the nature of the task through object categorical relationship and spatial organization, and trial blocking. Our findings support age-related declines in VSWM. Additionally, grid organizations (categorical and spatial relationships), and trial blocking differentially affected younger and older adults. Younger adults used spatial organizations more effectively whereas older adults demonstrated an association bias. Our finding also suggests that older adults may be less efficient than younger adults in strategically engaging information processing.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
11.
Am J Bioeth ; 23(6): 118-119, 2023 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37220358
12.
Am J Bioeth ; 18(1): 39-45, 2018 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29313771

RESUMEN

The increasing complexity of human subjects research and its oversight has prompted researchers, as well as institutional review boards (IRBs), to have a forum in which to discuss challenging or novel ethical issues not fully addressed by regulations. Research ethics consultation (REC) services provide such a forum. In this article, we rely on the experiences of a national Research Ethics Consultation Collaborative that collected more than 350 research ethics consultations in a repository and published 18 challenging cases with accompanying ethical commentaries to highlight four contexts in which REC can be a valuable resource. REC assists: 1) investigators before and after the regulatory review; 2) investigators, IRBs, and other research administrators facing challenging and novel ethical issues; 3) IRBs and investigators with the increasing challenges of informed consent and risk/benefit analysis; and 4) in providing flexible and collaborative assistance to overcome study hurdles, mediate conflicts within a team, or directly engage with research participants. Institutions that have established, or plan to establish, REC services should work to raise the visibility of their service and engage in open communication with existing clinical ethics consult services as well as the IRB. While the IRB system remains the foundation for the ethical review of research, REC can be a valuable service for investigators, regulators, and research participants aligned with the goal of supporting ethical research.


Asunto(s)
Investigación Biomédica/ética , Consultoría Ética , Comités de Ética en Investigación , Consentimiento Informado , Sujetos de Investigación , Estados Unidos
13.
JAMA ; 330(3): 221-222, 2023 07 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37351871

RESUMEN

This Viewpoint discusses the difficult task of creating a stakeholder-driven, evidence-based approach to assessing institutional review board effectiveness beyond regulatory compliance.


Asunto(s)
Investigación Biomédica , Comités de Ética en Investigación , Investigación Biomédica/ética , Investigación Biomédica/normas , Comités de Ética en Investigación/normas , Ética Clínica
14.
J Clin Ethics ; 29(2): 124-138, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29916829

RESUMEN

This article proposes an action guide to making decisions regarding the ethical allocation of resources that affect access to healthcare services offered by community-based healthcare organizations. Using the filter of empirical data from a study of decision making in two community-based healthcare organizations, we identify potentially relevant conceptual guidance from a review of frameworks and action guides in the public health, health policy, and organizational ethics literature. We describe the development of this action guide. We used data from a prior empirical study of the values that influence decision making about the allocation of resources in particular types of community-based healthcare organizations. We evaluated, organized, and specified the conceptual guidance we found in 14 frameworks for ethical decision making. The result is an action guide that includes four domains that are relevant to the context of the decision to be made, eight domains that are relevant to the process of the decision to be made, and 15 domains that are relevant to the criteria of the decision to be made. We demonstrate the potential use of this action guide by walking through an illustrative resource allocation decision. The action guide provides community-based healthcare organizations with a conceptually grounded, empirically informed framework for ethical decision making.


Asunto(s)
Servicios de Salud Comunitaria/ética , Toma de Decisiones en la Organización , Ética Institucional , Asignación de Recursos/ética , Humanos
16.
Brain Cogn ; 118: 1-18, 2017 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28734164

RESUMEN

The classic mental rotation task (MRT; Shepard & Metzler, 1971) is commonly thought to measure mental rotation, a cognitive process involving covert simulation of motor rotation. Yet much research suggests that the MRT recruits both motor simulation and other analytic cognitive strategies that depend on visuospatial representation and visual working memory (WM). In the present study, we investigated cognitive strategies in the MRT using time-frequency analysis of EEG and independent component analysis. We scrutinized sensorimotor mu (µ) power reduction, associated with motor simulation, parietal alpha (pα) power reduction, associated with visuospatial representation, and frontal midline theta (fmθ) power enhancement, associated with WM maintenance and manipulation. µ power increased concomitant with increasing task difficulty, suggesting reduced use of motor simulation, while pα decreased and fmθ power increased, suggesting heightened use of visuospatial representation processing and WM, respectively. These findings suggest that MRT performance involves flexibly trading off between cognitive strategies, namely a motor simulation-based mental rotation strategy and WM-intensive analytic strategies based on task difficulty. Flexible cognitive strategy use may be a domain-general cognitive principle that underlies aptitude and spatial intelligence in a variety of cognitive domains. We close with discussion of the present study's implications as well as future directions.


Asunto(s)
Ondas Encefálicas/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Imaginación/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Rotación , Adulto Joven
17.
Brain Cogn ; 113: 32-39, 2017 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28107684

RESUMEN

The human extrastriate cortex contains a region critically involved in face detection and memory, the right fusiform gyrus. The present study evaluated whether transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) targeting this anatomical region would selectively influence memory for faces versus non-face objects (houses). Anodal tDCS targeted the right fusiform gyrus (Brodmann's Area 37), with the anode at electrode site PO10, and cathode at FP2. Two stimulation conditions were compared in a repeated-measures design: 0.5mA versus 1.5mA intensity; a separate control group received no stimulation. Participants completed a working memory task for face and house stimuli, varying in memory load from 1 to 4 items. Individual differences measures assessed trait-based differences in facial recognition skills. Results showed 1.5mA intensity stimulation (versus 0.5mA and control) increased performance at high memory loads, but only with faces. Lower overall working memory capacity predicted a positive impact of tDCS. Results provide support for the notion of functional specialization of the right fusiform regions for maintaining face (but not non-face object) stimuli in working memory, and further suggest that low intensity electrical stimulation of this region may enhance demanding face working memory performance particularly in those with relatively poor baseline working memory skills.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Estimulación Transcraneal de Corriente Directa/métodos , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
18.
Can J Physiol Pharmacol ; 95(1): 93-100, 2017 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28044450

RESUMEN

Tea is perceived as more relaxing than coffee, even though both contain caffeine. L-theanine in tea may account for the difference. Consumed together, caffeine and theanine exert similar cognitive effects to that of caffeine alone, but exert opposite effects on arousal, in that caffeine accentuates and theanine mitigates physiological and felt stress responses. We evaluated whether caffeine and theanine influenced cognition under emotional arousal. Using a double-blind, repeated-measures design, 36 participants received 4 treatments (200 mg caffeine + 0 mg theanine, 0 mg caffeine + 200 mg theanine, 200 mg caffeine + 200 mg theanine, 0 mg caffeine + 0 mg theanine) on separate days. Emotional arousal was induced by highly arousing negative film clips and pictures. Mood, salivary cortisol, and visual attention were evaluated. Caffeine accentuated global processing of visual attention on the hierarchical shape task (p < 0.05), theanine accentuated local processing (p < 0.05), and the combination did not differ from placebo. Caffeine reduced flanker conflict difference scores on the Attention Network Test (p < 0.05), theanine increased difference scores (p < 0.05), and the combination did not differ from placebo. Thus, under emotional arousal, caffeine and theanine exert opposite effects on certain attentional processes, but when consumed together, they counteract the effects of each other.


Asunto(s)
Nivel de Alerta , Atención/efectos de los fármacos , Cafeína/farmacología , Glutamatos/farmacología , Adolescente , Afecto/efectos de los fármacos , Cognición/efectos de los fármacos , Método Doble Ciego , Interacciones Farmacológicas , Femenino , Humanos , Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Masculino , Saliva/metabolismo , Adulto Joven
19.
Mem Cognit ; 45(1): 63-80, 2017 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27600699

RESUMEN

The mental rotation literature commonly reports a sex difference, almost always favoring men. Two strategies have been proposed in the literature to account for this difference: holistic and piecemeal. However, there is great variability in rotation performance suggesting other possible contributing factors. This study investigated the effects of stimuli characteristics and habitual spatial thinking on mental rotation performance. In three experiments, participants completed a mental rotation task with two modifications: (1) 3-D figures were presented with their cut versions to promote piecemeal strategy, and (2) block figures were either presented within a frame or none, in light of reference framework model or perceptual grouping model. Overall, whole figures generated faster responses than cut figures and this was more pronounced with greater angular disparity. Shape or the presence of a frame affected rotation performance. Having a frame seemed to impede overall rotation, especially for cut figures, supporting an object-based reference frame. However, a 3-D frame did not have the same effect, possibly suggesting the unitary configuration idea. Men rotated more accurately than women, but still sex was not as robust as it was suggested in the literature. Interestingly, there were similarities between possible strategy use and habitual spatial thinking. People who were categorized as having landmark-centered representations and who preferred verbal directions showed a pattern suggesting the use of a piecemeal strategy, and survey-centered representations and map preference seemed to reflect a holistic strategy.


Asunto(s)
Imaginación/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Pensamiento/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Rotación , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA