Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 24
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Mem Cognit ; 43(7): 973-89, 2015 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25962602

RESUMEN

We examined the role of dual-task interference in working memory using a novel dual two-back task that requires a redundant-target response (i.e., a response that neither the auditory nor the visual stimulus occurred two back versus a response that one or both occurred two back) on every trial. Comparisons with performance on single two-back trials (i.e., with only auditory or only visual stimuli) showed that dual-task demands reduced both speed and accuracy. Our task design enabled a novel application of Townsend and Nozawa's (Journal of Mathematical Psychology 39: 321-359, 1995) workload capacity measure, which revealed that the decrement in dual two-back performance was mediated by the sharing of a limited amount of processing capacity. Relative to most other single and dual n-back tasks, performance measures for our task were more reliable, due to the use of a small stimulus set that induced a high and constant level of proactive interference. For a version of our dual two-back task that minimized response bias, accuracy was also more strongly correlated with complex span than has been found for most other single and dual n-back tasks.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
2.
Front Psychol ; 12: 759616, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34955980

RESUMEN

Nature exposure can provide benefits on stress, health and cognitive performance. According to Attention Restoration Theory (ART), the positive impact of nature on cognition is mainly driven by fascination. Fascinating properties of nature such as water or a winding hiking trail may capture involuntary attention, allowing the directed form of attention to rest and to recover. This claim has been supported by studies relying on eye-tracking measures of attention deployment, comparing exposure to urban and nature settings. Yet, recent studies have shown that promoting higher engagement with a nature setting can improve restorative benefits, hence challenging ART's view that voluntary attention is resting. Besides, recent evidence published by Szolosi et al. (2014) suggests that voluntary attention may be involved during exposure to high-mystery nature images which they showed as having greater potential for attention restoration. The current study explored how exposure to nature images of different scenic qualities in mystery (and restoration potential) could impact the engagement of attention. To do so, participants were shown nature images characterized by either low or high mystery properties (with allegedly low or high restoration potential, respectively) and were asked to evaluate their fascination and aesthetic levels. Concurrently, an eye tracker collected measures of pupil size, fixations and spontaneous blinks as indices of attentional engagement. Results showed that high-mystery nature images had higher engagement than low-mystery images as supported by the larger pupil dilations, the higher number of fixations and the reduced number of blinks and durations of fixations. Taken together, these results challenge ART's view that directed attention is merely resting during exposure to restorative nature and offer new hypotheses on potential mechanisms underlying attention restoration.

3.
Cereb Cortex ; 19(7): 1557-66, 2009 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18980948

RESUMEN

Previous neuroimaging studies have implicated the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and nearby brain regions in deception. This is consistent with the hypothesis that lying involves the executive control system. To date, the nature of the contribution of different aspects of executive control to deception, however, remains unclear. In the present study, we utilized an activation likelihood estimate (ALE) method of meta-analysis to quantitatively identify brain regions that are consistently more active for deceptive responses relative to truthful responses across past studies. We then contrasted the results with additional ALE maps generated for 3 different aspects of executive control: working memory, inhibitory control, and task switching. Deception-related regions in dorsolateral PFC and posterior parietal cortex were selectively associated with working memory. Additional deception regions in ventrolateral PFC, anterior insula, and anterior cingulate cortex were associated with multiple aspects of executive control. In contrast, deception-related regions in bilateral inferior parietal lobule were not associated with any of the 3 executive control constructs. Our findings support the notion that executive control processes, particularly working memory, and their associated neural substrates play an integral role in deception. This work provides a foundation for future research on the neurocognitive basis of deception.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Decepción , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Detección de Mentiras , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Retroalimentación/fisiología , Humanos , Funciones de Verosimilitud
4.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 46(2): 280-295, 2020 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31219303

RESUMEN

We investigated participants' task set preparation by measuring changes in pupil diameter during a blank interval as they prepared for an easy (i.e., prosaccade) or difficult (i.e., antisaccade) trial. We used occasional thought probes to gauge "on-task" thoughts versus mind wandering. In both studies, participants' pupil diameters were larger when anticipating an antisaccade, relative to a prosaccade, trial. In contrast, their self-reported mind wandering depended upon whether the thought probes occurred after their target detection response (Experiment 1) or occurred in lieu of target detection (Experiment 2). In the latter case, self-reported mind wandering echoed the pupil diameter changes in demonstrating greater off-task behavior when preparing for a prosaccade trial. More important, trial type effects in pupil diameter emerged only when participants reported being "on-task," but disappeared during periods of mind wandering. These results demonstrate that changes in pupil diameter reflect the degree of preparatory control exerted for an upcoming trial, but only when attention is actively focused on the upcoming task. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Disposición en Psicología , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Pensamiento/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pupila/fisiología , Adulto Joven
6.
Psychophysiology ; 55(3)2018 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28295409

RESUMEN

The present study investigated individual differences in information processing following errant behavior. Participants were initially classified as high or as low working memory capacity using the Operation Span Task. In a subsequent session, they then performed a high congruency version of the flanker task under both speed and accuracy stress. We recorded ERPs and behavioral measures of accuracy and response time in the flanker task with a primary focus on processing following an error. The error-related negativity was larger for the high working memory capacity group than for the low working memory capacity group. The positivity following an error (Pe) was modulated to a greater extent by speed-accuracy instruction for the high working memory capacity group than for the low working memory capacity group. These data help to explicate the neural bases of individual differences in working memory capacity and cognitive control.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Objetivos , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto Joven
7.
Cortex ; 87: 96-107, 2017 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27745847

RESUMEN

Neuroimaging investigations of human memory encoding and retrieval have revealed that multiple regions of parietal cortex contribute to memory. Recently, a sparse network of regions within parietal cortex has been identified using resting state functional connectivity (MRI techniques). The regions within this network exhibit consistent task-related responses during memory formation and retrieval, leading to its being called the parietal memory network (PMN). Among its signature patterns are: deactivation during initial experience with an item (e.g., encoding); activation during subsequent repetitions (e.g., at retrieval); greater activation for successfully retrieved familiar words than novel words (e.g., hits relative to correctly-rejected lures). The question of interest here is whether novel words that are subjectively experienced as having been recently studied would elicit PMN activation similar to that of hits. That is, we compared old items correctly recognized to two types of novel items on a recognition test: those correctly identified as new and those incorrectly labeled as old due to their strong associative relation to the studied words (in the DRM false memory protocol). Subjective oldness plays a strong role in driving activation, as hits and false alarms activated similarly (and greater than correctly-rejected lures).


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Memoria/fisiopatología , Memoria/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiopatología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Asociación , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Trastornos de la Memoria/diagnóstico por imagen , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Red Nerviosa/diagnóstico por imagen , Lóbulo Parietal/diagnóstico por imagen , Adulto Joven
8.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 32(6): 1324-44, 2006 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17154775

RESUMEN

This article evaluates 2 competing models that address the decision-making processes mediating word recognition and lexical decision performance: a hybrid 2-stage model of lexical decision performance and a random-walk model. In 2 experiments, nonword type and word frequency were manipulated across 2 contrasts (pseudohomophone-legal nonword and legal-illegal nonword). When nonwords became more wordlike (i.e., BRNTA vs. BRANT vs. BRANE), response latencies to nonwords were slowed and the word frequency effect increased. More important, distributional analyses revealed that the Nonword Type = Word Frequency interaction was modulated by different components of the response time distribution, depending on the specific nonword contrast. A single-process random-walk model was able to account for this particular set of findings more successfully than the hybrid 2-stage model.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Lectura , Adulto , Humanos , Memoria , Modelos Psicológicos , Distribución Normal , Psicolingüística , Detección de Señal Psicológica
9.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 13(3): 446-51, 2006 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17048729

RESUMEN

In three experiments,we examined the relationship between orthographic andphonological distinctiveness and incidental recall. In each experiment, participants were given a surprise free recalltest after they read words aloud as quickly and accurately as possible. The pattern of results replicated those reported in Cortese, Watson, Wang, and Fugett (2004) for intentional and explicit free recall and recognition memory tasks in which items were read silently. Specifically, we found that phonological-to-orthographic neighborhood size influenced recall performance,whereas orthographic-to-phonologicalconsistency and phonological-to-orthographic consistency did not Also, we failed to replicate the orthographic-to-phonological consistency effect reported by Hirshman and Jackson (1997), and argue that their results were due to a confounding of consistency with phonological neighborhood size. Our results suggest that the processing of words sharing both orthography and phonology with a large number of words produces interference that reduces one's ability to remember them.


Asunto(s)
Memoria , Fonética , Vocabulario , Humanos , Lingüística/estadística & datos numéricos , Tiempo de Reacción , Lectura
10.
Accid Anal Prev ; 92: 22-33, 2016 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27035396

RESUMEN

The use of cell phones while driving is ubiquitous, particularly in countries where the practice is legal. However, surveys indicate that most drivers favor legislation to limit the use of mobile devices during the operation of a vehicle. A study was conducted to understand this inconsistency between what drivers do and what they advocate for others. Participants completed a survey about their driving attitudes, abilities, and behaviors. Following previous research, drivers reported using cell phones for benefits such as getting work done. The hypocrisy of using cell phones while advocating restrictions appears to stem from differences in the perceived safety risks of self vs. others' use of cell phones. Many if not most drivers believe they can drive safely while using mobile devices. However, they lack confidence in others' ability to drive safely while distracted and believe that others' use of cell phones is dangerous. The threat to public safety of others' usage of mobile devices was one of the strongest independent predictors of support for legislation to restrict cell phone use.


Asunto(s)
Actitud , Conducción de Automóvil/psicología , Teléfono Celular/legislación & jurisprudencia , Conducción Distraída/psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Conducción de Automóvil/legislación & jurisprudencia , Conducción Distraída/legislación & jurisprudencia , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Riesgo , Seguridad , Adulto Joven
11.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 31(1): 76-85, 2005 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15641906

RESUMEN

The authors addressed whether individual differences in the working memory capacity (WMC) of young adults influence susceptibility to false memories for nonpresented critical words in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott associative list paradigm. The results of 2 experiments indicated that individuals with greater WMC recalled fewer critical words than individuals with reduced WMC when participants were forewarned about the tendency of associative lists (e.g., bed, rest, . . .) to elicit illusory memories for critical words (e.g., sleep). In contrast, both high and low WMC participants used repeated study-test trials to reduce recall of critical words. These findings suggest that individual differences in WMC influence cognitive control and the ability to actively maintain task goals in the face of interfering information or habit.


Asunto(s)
Represión Psicológica , Adulto , Asociación , Hábitos , Humanos , Matemática , Recuerdo Mental , Semántica , Vocabulario
12.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 22(3): 876-83, 2015 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25223371

RESUMEN

The present study used brain imaging to determine the neural basis of individual differences in multitasking, the ability to successfully perform at least two attention-demanding tasks at once. Multitasking is mentally taxing and, therefore, should recruit the prefrontal cortex to maintain task goals when coordinating attentional control and managing the cognitive load. To investigate this possibility, we used functional neuroimaging to assess neural activity in both extraordinary multitaskers (Supertaskers) and control subjects who were matched on working memory capacity. Participants performed a challenging dual N-back task in which auditory and visual stimuli were presented simultaneously, requiring independent and continuous maintenance, updating, and verification of the contents of verbal and spatial working memory. With the task requirements and considerable cognitive load that accompanied increasing N-back, relative to the controls, the multitasking of Supertaskers was characterized by more efficient recruitment of anterior cingulate and posterior frontopolar prefrontal cortices. Results are interpreted using neuropsychological and evolutionary perspectives on individual differences in multitasking ability and the neural correlates of attentional control.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Giro del Cíngulo/fisiología , Individualidad , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Neuroimagen Funcional , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Adulto Joven
13.
Neuropsychologia ; 41(3): 293-303, 2003.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12457755

RESUMEN

A procedure is introduced for using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) techniques to identify neural regions associated with attention to semantic and phonological aspects of written words within a single group of subjects. Short lists (16 words/list), consisting of visually-presented semantically-related words (bed, rest) or rhyming words (weep, beep) were presented rapidly to subjects, who were asked to attend to the relations among the words. Regions preferentially involved in attention to semantic relations appeared within left anterior/ventral inferior frontal gyrus (IFG, approximate Brodmann Area, BA47), left posterior/dorsal IFG (BA44/45), left superior/middle temporal cortex (BA22/21), left fusiform gyrus (BA37), and right cerebellum. Regions preferentially involved in attention to phonological relations appeared within left inferior frontal cortex (near BA6/44, posterior to the semantic regions within IFG described above) and within bilateral inferior parietal cortex (BA40) and precuneus (BA7). This method is notable in that a comparison of the two tasks within some of the individual subjects revealed activation patterns similar to the group average, especially within left inferior frontal and left superior/middle parietal cortices. This fact combined with the efficiency with which the data can be obtained (here, in about an hour of functional scanning) and the adaptability of the task for many different subject populations suggests a wide range of possibilities for this technique: it could be used to track language development (e.g. in children), compare language organization across subject populations (e.g. for dyslexic or blind subjects), and identify language regions within individuals (e.g. potentially to aid in surgical planning).


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Fonética , Semántica , Adolescente , Adulto , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Mapeo Encefálico , Cognición/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Lenguaje , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Lectura
14.
Neuropsychology ; 18(2): 276-83, 2004 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15099150

RESUMEN

In prior studies, it was observed that patients with schizophrenia show abnormally high knowledge corruption (i.e., high-confident errors expressed as a percentage of all high-confident responses were increased for schizophrenic patients relative to controls). The authors examined the conditions under which excessive knowledge corruption occurred using the Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm. Whereas knowledge corruption in schizophrenia was significantly greater for false-negative errors relative to controls, no group difference occurred for false-positive errors. The groups showed a comparable high degree of confidence for false-positive recognition of critical lure items. Similar to findings collected in elderly participants, patients, but not controls, showed a strong positive correlation between the number of recognized studied items and false-positive recognition of the critical lure.


Asunto(s)
Memoria a Corto Plazo , Represión Psicológica , Esquizofrenia/diagnóstico , Psicología del Esquizofrénico , Percepción del Habla , Aprendizaje Verbal , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Aprendizaje por Asociación de Pares , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Semántica
15.
Front Psychol ; 5: 1360, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25505441

RESUMEN

Although research has provided prodigious evidence in support of the cognitive benefits that natural settings have over urban settings, all nature is not equal. Within nature, natural settings that contain mystery are often among the most preferred nature scenes. With the prospect of acquiring new information, scenes of this type could more effectively elicit a person's sense of fascination, enabling that person to rest the more effortful forms of attention. The present study examined the direct cognitive benefits that mystery in nature has on attention. Settings of this sort presumably evoke a form of attention that is undemanding or effortless. In order to investigate that notion, participants (n = 144) completed a Recognition Memory Task (RMT) that evaluated recognition performance based on the presence of mystery and presentation duration (300 ms, 1 s, 5 s, and 10 s). Results revealed that with additional viewing time, images perceived high in mystery achieved greater improvements in recognition performance when compared to those images perceived low in mystery. Tests for mediation showed that the effect mystery had on recognition performance occurred through perceptions of fascination. Implications of these and other findings are discussed in the context of Attention Restoration Theory.

16.
PLoS One ; 8(1): e54402, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23372720

RESUMEN

The present study examined the relationship between personality and individual differences in multi-tasking ability. Participants enrolled at the University of Utah completed measures of multi-tasking activity, perceived multi-tasking ability, impulsivity, and sensation seeking. In addition, they performed the Operation Span in order to assess their executive control and actual multi-tasking ability. The findings indicate that the persons who are most capable of multi-tasking effectively are not the persons who are most likely to engage in multiple tasks simultaneously. To the contrary, multi-tasking activity as measured by the Media Multitasking Inventory and self-reported cell phone usage while driving were negatively correlated with actual multi-tasking ability. Multi-tasking was positively correlated with participants' perceived ability to multi-task ability which was found to be significantly inflated. Participants with a strong approach orientation and a weak avoidance orientation--high levels of impulsivity and sensation seeking--reported greater multi-tasking behavior. Finally, the findings suggest that people often engage in multi-tasking because they are less able to block out distractions and focus on a singular task. Participants with less executive control--low scorers on the Operation Span task and persons high in impulsivity--tended to report higher levels of multi-tasking activity.


Asunto(s)
Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Conducta Impulsiva/psicología , Autoeficacia , Sensación/fisiología , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Masculino , Inventario de Personalidad , Asunción de Riesgos , Autoinforme
17.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 38(3): 757-763, 2012 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22250911

RESUMEN

Neuroscience suggests that the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is responsible for conflict monitoring and the detection of errors in cognitive tasks, thereby contributing to the implementation of attentional control. Though individual differences in frontally mediated goal maintenance have clearly been shown to influence outward behavior in interference-rich contexts, it is unclear whether corresponding differences exist in neural responses that arise out of the ACC. To investigate this possibility, we conducted an electrophysiological study using a variant of the Simon Task, recording event-related potentials (ERPs) in healthy normal individuals with varying working memory capacity (high vs. low spans; a behavioral proxy for variability in goal maintenance). Primary analyses focused on the magnitude of the error-related negativity (ERN), a response-locked ERP component associated with the commission of errors thought to arise because of action monitoring in the ACC. Our results revealed that frontally mediated working memory capacity may alter error monitoring by the ACC, with high spans showing a greater ERN than low spans. These individual differences were also observed in the posterror positivity, a response-locked ERP component associated with updating cognitive strategies, suggesting greater awareness of errors with increased working memory capacity. These results are interpreted within 2-process models of attentional control, suggesting individuals with greater working memory capacity may better maintain task goals by more strongly biasing neural activity in frontal-executive networks.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Individualidad , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Monitoreo Fisiológico , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
18.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 37(3): 785-91, 2011 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21299325

RESUMEN

Inattentional blindness refers to the finding that people do not always see what appears in their gaze. Though inattentional blindness affects large percentages of people, it is unclear if there are individual differences in susceptibility. The present study addressed whether individual differences in attentional control, as reflected by variability in working memory capacity, modulate susceptibility to inattentional blindness. Participants watched a classic inattentional blindness video (Simons & Chabris, 1999) and were instructed to count passes among basketball players, wherein 58% noticed the unexpected: a person wearing a gorilla suit. When participants were accurate with their pass counts, individuals with higher working memory capacity were more likely to report seeing the gorilla (67%) than those with lesser working memory capacity (36%). These results suggest that variability in attentional control is a potential mechanism underlying the apparent modulation of inattentional blindness across individuals.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Ceguera/fisiopatología , Individualidad , Adulto , Ceguera/diagnóstico , Susceptibilidad a Enfermedades , Femenino , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Luminosa , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
19.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 17(4): 479-85, 2010 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20702865

RESUMEN

Theory suggests that driving should be impaired for any motorist who is concurrently talking on a cell phone. But is everybody impaired by this dual-task combination? We tested 200 participants in a high-fidelity driving simulator in both single- and dual-task conditions. The dual task involved driving while performing a demanding auditory version of the operation span (OSPAN) task. Whereas the vast majority of participants showed significant performance decrements in dual-task conditions (compared with single-task conditions for either driving or OSPAN tasks), 2.5% of the sample showed absolutely no performance decrements with respect to performing single and dual tasks. In single-task conditions, these "supertaskers" scored in the top quartile on all dependent measures associated with driving and OSPAN tasks, and Monte Carlo simulations indicated that the frequency of supertaskers was significantly greater than chance. These individual differences help to sharpen our theoretical understanding of attention and cognitive control in naturalistic settings.


Asunto(s)
Aptitud , Atención , Conducción de Automóvil/psicología , Teléfono Celular , Simulación por Computador , Función Ejecutiva , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental , Solución de Problemas , Aprendizaje Seriado , Adulto Joven
20.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 61(7): 1036-66, 2008 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17853227

RESUMEN

The current study explores a set of variables that have the potential to predict semantic priming effects for 300 prime-target associates at the item level. Young and older adults performed either lexical decision (LDT) or naming tasks. A multiple regression procedure was used to predict priming based upon prime characteristics, target characteristics, and prime-target semantic similarity. Results indicate that semantic priming (a) can be reliably predicted at an item level; (b) is equivalent in magnitude across standardized measures of priming in LDTs and naming tasks; (c) is greater following quickly recognized primes; (d) is greater in LDTs for targets that produce slow lexical decision latencies; (e) is greater for pairs high in forward associative strength across tasks and across stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs); (f) is greater for pairs high in backward associative strength in both tasks, but only at a long SOA; and (g) does not vary as a function of estimates from latent semantic analysis (LSA). Based upon these results, it is suggested that researchers take extreme caution in comparing priming effects across different item sets. Moreover, the current findings lend support to spreading activation and feature overlap theories of priming, but do not support priming based upon contextual similarity as captured by LSA.


Asunto(s)
Tiempo de Reacción , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Semántica , Adulto , Anciano , Predicción , Humanos , Estudios Prospectivos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA