RESUMEN
Distinguishing sensory stimuli caused by changes in the environment from those caused by an animal's own actions is a hallmark of sensory processing1. Saccades are rapid eye movements that shift the image on the retina. How visual systems differentiate motion of the image induced by saccades from actual motion in the environment is not fully understood2. Here we discovered that in mouse primary visual cortex (V1) the two types of motion evoke distinct activity patterns. This is because, during saccades, V1 combines the visual input with a strong non-visual input arriving from the thalamic pulvinar nucleus. The non-visual input triggers responses that are specific to the direction of the saccade and the visual input triggers responses that are specific to the direction of the shift of the stimulus on the retina, yet the preferred directions of these two responses are uncorrelated. Thus, the pulvinar input ensures differential V1 responses to external and self-generated motion. Integration of external sensory information with information about body movement may be a general mechanism for sensory cortices to distinguish between self-generated and external stimuli.
Asunto(s)
Movimiento , Movimientos Sacádicos , Corteza Visual , Animales , Ratones , Movimiento/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Retina/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Núcleos Talámicos/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiologíaRESUMEN
Most animals have compound eyes, with tens to thousands of lenses attached rigidly to the exoskeleton. A natural assumption is that all of these species must resort to moving either their head or their body to actively change their visual input. However, classic anatomy has revealed that flies have muscles poised to move their retinas under the stable lenses of each compound eye1-3. Here we show that Drosophila use their retinal muscles to smoothly track visual motion, which helps to stabilize the retinal image, and also to perform small saccades when viewing a stationary scene. We show that when the retina moves, visual receptive fields shift accordingly, and that even the smallest retinal saccades activate visual neurons. Using a head-fixed behavioural paradigm, we find that Drosophila perform binocular, vergence movements of their retinas-which could enhance depth perception-when crossing gaps, and impairing the physiology of retinal motor neurons alters gap-crossing trajectories during free behaviour. That flies evolved an ability to actuate their retinas suggests that moving the eye independently of the head is broadly paramount for animals. The similarities of smooth and saccadic movements of the Drosophila retina and the vertebrate eye highlight a notable example of convergent evolution.
Asunto(s)
Drosophila , Movimientos Oculares , Músculos , Retina , Visión Ocular , Animales , Drosophila/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Músculos/fisiología , Retina/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Visión Ocular/fisiología , Visión Binocular , Percepción de Profundidad , Neuronas Motoras , Cabeza/fisiología , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Evolución BiológicaRESUMEN
Coordination of goal-directed behavior depends on the brain's ability to recover the locations of relevant objects in the world. In humans, the visual system encodes the spatial organization of sensory inputs, but neurons in early visual areas map objects according to their retinal positions, rather than where they are in the world. How the brain computes world-referenced spatial information across eye movements has been widely researched and debated. Here, we tested whether shifts of covert attention are sufficiently precise in space and time to track an object's real-world location across eye movements. We found that observers' attentional selectivity is remarkably precise and is barely perturbed by the execution of saccades. Inspired by recent neurophysiological discoveries, we developed an observer model that rapidly estimates the real-world locations of objects and allocates attention within this reference frame. The model recapitulates the human data and provides a parsimonious explanation for previously reported phenomena in which observers allocate attention to task-irrelevant locations across eye movements. Our findings reveal that visual attention operates in real-world coordinates, which can be computed rapidly at the earliest stages of cortical processing.
Asunto(s)
Atención , Movimientos Sacádicos , Humanos , Atención/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Adulto , Masculino , Femenino , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodosRESUMEN
Humans explore visual scenes by alternating short fixations with saccades directing the fovea to points of interest. During fixation, the visual system not only examines the foveal stimulus at high resolution, but it also processes the extrafoveal input to plan the next saccade. Although foveal analysis and peripheral selection occur in parallel, little is known about the temporal dynamics of foveal and peripheral processing upon saccade landing, during fixation. Here we investigate whether the ability to localize changes across the visual field differs depending on when the change occurs during fixation, and on whether the change localization involves foveal, extrafoveal processing, or both. Our findings reveal that the ability to localize changes in peripheral areas of the visual field improves as a function of time after fixation onset, whereas localization accuracy for foveal stimuli remains approximately constant. Importantly, this pattern holds regardless of whether individuals monitor only foveal or peripheral stimuli, or both simultaneously. Altogether, these results show that the visual system is more attuned to the foveal input early on during fixation, whereas change localization for peripheral stimuli progressively improves throughout fixation, possibly as a consequence of an increased readiness to plan the next saccade.
Asunto(s)
Fijación Ocular , Fóvea Central , Movimientos Sacádicos , Campos Visuales , Humanos , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Fóvea Central/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Percepción Visual/fisiologíaRESUMEN
The presaccadic preview of a peripheral target enhances the efficiency of its postsaccadic processing, termed the extrafoveal preview effect. Peripheral visual performance-and thus the quality of the preview-varies around the visual field, even at isoeccentric locations: It is better along the horizontal than vertical meridian and along the lower than upper vertical meridian. To investigate whether these polar angle asymmetries influence the preview effect, we asked human participants to preview four tilted gratings at the cardinals, until a central cue indicated which one to saccade to. During the saccade, the target orientation either remained or slightly changed (valid/invalid preview). After saccade landing, participants discriminated the orientation of the (briefly presented) second grating. Stimulus contrast was titrated with adaptive staircases to assess visual performance. Expectedly, valid previews increased participants' postsaccadic contrast sensitivity. This preview benefit, however, was inversely related to polar angle perceptual asymmetries; largest at the upper, and smallest at the horizontal meridian. This finding reveals that the visual system compensates for peripheral asymmetries when integrating information across saccades, by selectively assigning higher weights to the less-well perceived preview information. Our study supports the recent line of evidence showing that perceptual dynamics around saccades vary with eye movement direction.
Asunto(s)
Movimientos Sacádicos , Campos Visuales , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Adulto , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Femenino , Masculino , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Adulto Joven , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiologíaRESUMEN
It has been suggested that, prior to a saccade, visual neurons predictively respond to stimuli that will fall in their receptive fields after completion of the saccade. This saccadic remapping process is thought to compensate for the shift of the visual world across the retina caused by eye movements. To map the timing of this predictive process in the brain, we recorded neural activity using electroencephalography during a saccade task. Human participants (male and female) made saccades between two fixation points while covertly attending to oriented gratings briefly presented at various locations on the screen. Data recorded during trials in which participants maintained fixation were used to train classifiers on stimuli in different positions. Subsequently, data collected during saccade trials were used to test for the presence of remapped stimulus information at the post-saccadic retinotopic location in the peri-saccadic period, providing unique insight into when remapped information becomes available. We found that the stimulus could be decoded at the remapped location â¼180â ms post-stimulus onset, but only when the stimulus was presented 100-200â ms before saccade onset. Within this range, we found that the timing of remapping was dictated by stimulus onset rather than saccade onset. We conclude that presenting the stimulus immediately before the saccade allows for optimal integration of the corollary discharge signal with the incoming peripheral visual information, resulting in a remapping of activation to the relevant post-saccadic retinotopic neurons.
Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Estimulación Luminosa , Movimientos Sacádicos , Humanos , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Adulto Joven , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiologíaRESUMEN
Attentional reorienting is dysfunctional not only in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), but also in infants who will develop ASD, thus constituting a potential causal factor of future social interaction and communication abilities. Following the research domain criteria framework, we hypothesized that the presence of subclinical autistic traits in parents should lead to atypical infants' attentional reorienting, which in turn should impact on their future socio-communication behavior in toddlerhood. During an attentional cueing task, we measured the saccadic latencies in a large sample (total enrolled n = 89; final sample n = 71) of 8-month-old infants from the general population as a proxy for their stimulus-driven attention. Infants were grouped in a high parental traits (HPT; n = 23) or in a low parental traits (LPT; n = 48) group, according to the degree of autistic traits self-reported by their parents. Infants (n = 33) were then longitudinally followed to test their socio-communicative behaviors at 21 months. Results show a sluggish reorienting system, which was a longitudinal predictor of future socio-communicative skills at 21 months. Our combined transgenerational and longitudinal findings suggest that the early functionality of the stimulus-driven attentional network-redirecting attention from one event to another-could be directly connected to future social and communication development.
Asunto(s)
Atención , Padres , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Lactante , Atención/fisiología , Padres/psicología , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/fisiopatología , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Conducta Social , Comunicación , Estudios Longitudinales , Trastorno Autístico/psicología , Trastorno Autístico/fisiopatología , Señales (Psicología) , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , AdultoRESUMEN
A prominent aspect of primate lateral prefrontal cortex organization is its division into several cytoarchitecturally distinct subregions. Neurophysiological investigations in macaques have provided evidence for the functional specialization of these subregions, but an understanding of the relative representational topography of sensory, social, and cognitive processes within them remains elusive. One explanatory factor is that evidence for functional specialization has been compiled largely from a patchwork of findings across studies, in many animals, and with considerable variation in stimulus sets and tasks. Here, we addressed this by leveraging the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) to carry out large-scale neurophysiological mapping of the lateral prefrontal cortex using high-density microelectrode arrays, and a diverse suite of test stimuli including faces, marmoset calls, and spatial working memory task. Task-modulated units and units responsive to visual and auditory stimuli were distributed throughout the lateral prefrontal cortex, while those with saccade-related activity or face-selective responses were restricted to 8aV, 8aD, 10, 46 V, and 47. Neurons with contralateral visual receptive fields were limited to areas 8aV and 8aD. These data reveal a mixed pattern of functional specialization in the lateral prefrontal cortex, in which responses to some stimuli and tasks are distributed broadly across lateral prefrontal cortex subregions, while others are more limited in their representation.
Asunto(s)
Callithrix , Corteza Prefrontal , Animales , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Mapeo Encefálico , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Neuronas/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Vocalización Animal/fisiologíaRESUMEN
OBJECTIVE: To date the vast majority of research in the visual neurosciences have been forced to adopt a highly constrained perspective of the vision system in which stimuli are processed in an open-loop reactive fashion (i.e., abrupt stimulus presentation followed by an evoked neural response). While such constraints enable high construct validity for neuroscientific investigation, the primary outcomes have been a reductionistic approach to isolate the component processes of visual perception. In electrophysiology, of the many neural processes studied under this rubric, the most well-known is, arguably, the P300 evoked response. There is, however, relatively little known about the real-world corollary of this component in free-viewing paradigms where visual stimuli are connected to neural function in a closed-loop. While growing evidence suggests that neural activity analogous to the P300 does occur in such paradigms, it is an open question when this response occurs and what behavioral or environmental factors could be used to isolate this component. APPROACH: The current work uses convolutional networks to decode neural signals during a free-viewing visual search task in a closed-loop paradigm within an open-world virtual environment. From the decoded activity we construct fixation-locked response profiles that enable estimations of the variable latency of any P300 analogue around the moment of fixation. We then use these estimates to investigate which factors best reduce variable latency and, thus, predict the onset time of the response. We consider measurable, search-related factors encompassing top-down (i.e., goal driven) and bottom-up (i.e., stimulus driven) processes, such as fixation duration and salience. We also consider saccade size as an intermediate factor reflecting the integration of these two systems. MAIN RESULTS: The results show that of these factors only saccade size reliably determines the onset time of P300 analogous activity for this task. Specifically, we find that for large saccades the variability in response onset is small enough to enable analysis using traditional ensemble averaging methods. SIGNIFICANCE: The results show that P300 analogous activity does occur during closed-loop, free-viewing visual search while highlighting distinct differences between the open-loop version of this response and its real-world analogue. The results also further establish saccades, and saccade size, as a key factor in real-world visual processing.
Asunto(s)
Movimientos Sacádicos , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Realidad Virtual , Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300/fisiología , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodosRESUMEN
Adaptation of reactive saccades (RS), made toward the sudden appearance of stimuli in our environment, is a plastic mechanism thought to occur at the motor level of saccade generation. As saccadic oculomotor commands integrate multisensory information in the parietal cortex and superior colliculus, adaptation of RS should occur not only toward visual but also tactile targets. In addition, saccadic adaptation in one modality (vision or touch) should transfer cross-modally. To test these predictions, we used the double-step target paradigm to adapt rightward saccades made at two different eccentricities toward the participants' index and middle fingers, identified either visually (experiment 1) or tactually (experiment 2). In each experiment, the rate of adaptation induced for the adapted modality and the rate of adaptation transfer to the nonadapted modality were compared with that measured in a control (no adaptation) session. Results revealed that touch-triggered RS can be adapted as well as visually triggered ones. Moreover, the transfer pattern was asymmetric: visual saccadic adaptation transferred fully to tactile saccades, whereas tactile saccadic adaptation, despite full generalization to nonadapted fingers, transferred only partially to visual saccades. These findings disclose that in the case of tactile saccades, adaptation can be elicited in the absence of postsaccadic visual feedback. In addition, the asymmetric adaptation transfer across sensory modalities suggests that the adaptation locus for tactile saccades may occur in part upstream of the final motor pathway common to all saccades. These findings bring new insights both on the functional loci(us) and on the error signals of RS adaptation. NEW & NOTEWORTHY The present study revealed that, as predicted from a large literature, adaptation of visual reactive saccades transfers to tactile saccades of the same as well as neighboring amplitudes. Furthermore, in a modified double-step target paradigm, tactile saccades exposed to repeated errors adapt with a similar rate and spatial generalization as visual saccades, but this adaptation only slightly transfers to visual saccades. These findings bring new information on saccadic adaptation processes.
Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica , Movimientos Sacádicos , Percepción Visual , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Humanos , Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Percepción del Tacto/fisiologíaRESUMEN
Temporal intervals appear compressed at the time of saccades. Here, I asked if saccadic compression of time is related to motor planning or to saccade execution. To dissociate saccade motor planning from its execution, I used the double-step paradigm, in which subjects have to perform two horizontal saccades successively. At various times around the saccade sequence, I presented two large horizontal bars, which marked an interval lasting 100 ms. After 700 ms, a second temporal interval was presented, varying in duration across trials. Subjects were required to judge which interval appeared shorter. I found that during the first saccades in the double-step paradigm, temporal intervals were compressed. Maximum temporal compression coincided with saccade onset. Around the time of the second saccade, I found temporal compression as well, however, the time of maximum compression preceded saccade onset by about 70 ms. I compared the magnitude and time of temporal compression between double-step saccades and amplitude-matched single saccades, which I measured separately. Although I found no difference in time compression magnitude, the time when maximum compression occurred differed significantly. I conclude that the temporal shift of time compression in double-step saccades demonstrates the influence of saccade motor planning on time perception.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Visually defined temporal intervals appear compressed at the time of saccades. Here, I tested time perception during double-step saccades dissociating saccade planning from execution. Although around the time of the first saccade, peak compression was found at saccade onset, compression around the time of the second saccade peaked 70 ms before saccade onset. The results suggest that saccade motor planning influences time perception.
Asunto(s)
Movimientos Sacádicos , Percepción del Tiempo , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto , Femenino , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiologíaRESUMEN
Saccade adaptation plays a crucial role in maintaining saccade accuracy. The behavioral characteristics and neural mechanisms of saccade adaptation for an externally cued movement, such as visually guided saccades (VGS), are well studied in nonhuman primates. In contrast, little is known about the saccade adaptation of an internally driven movement, such as memory-guided saccades (MGS), which are guided by visuospatial working memory. As the oculomotor plant changes because of growth, aging, or skeletomuscular problems, both types of saccades need to be adapted. Do both saccade types engage a common adaptation mechanism? In this study, we compared the characteristics of amplitude decrease adaptation in MGS with VGS in nonhuman primates. We found that the adaptation speed was faster for MGS than for VGS. Saccade duration changed during MGS adaptation, whereas saccade peak velocity changed during VGS adaptation. We also compared the adaptation field, that is, the gain change for saccade amplitudes other than the adapted. The gain change for MGS declines on both smaller and larger sides of adapted amplitude, more rapidly for larger than smaller amplitudes, whereas the decline in VGS was reversed. Thus, the differences between VGS and MGS adaptation characteristics support the previously suggested hypothesis that the adaptation mechanisms of VGS and MGS are distinct. Furthermore, the result suggests that the MGS adaptation site is a brain structure that influences saccade duration, whereas the VGS adaptation site influences saccade peak velocity. These results should be beneficial for future neurophysiological experiments.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Plasticity helps to overcome persistent motor errors. Such motor plasticity or adaptation can be investigated with saccades. Thus far our knowledge is primarily about visually guided saccades, an externally cued movement, which we can make only when the object is visible at the time of saccade. However, as the world is complex, we can make saccades even when the object is not visible. Here, we investigate the adaptation of an internally driven movement: the memory-guided saccade.
Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica , Macaca mulatta , Movimientos Sacádicos , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Animales , Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Masculino , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Femenino , Memoria/fisiologíaRESUMEN
Previous research has shown that action effects of self-generated movements are internally predicted before outcome feedback becomes available. To test whether these sensorimotor predictions are used to facilitate visual information uptake for feedback processing, we measured eye movements during the execution of a goal-directed throwing task. Participants could fully observe the effects of their throwing actions (ball trajectory and either hitting or missing a target) in most of the trials. In a portion of the trials, the ball trajectory was not visible, and participants only received static information about the outcome. We observed a large proportion of predictive saccades, shifting gaze toward the goal region before the ball arrived and outcome feedback became available. Fixation locations after predictive saccades systematically covaried with future ball positions in trials with continuous ball flight information, but notably also in trials with static outcome feedback and only efferent and proprioceptive information about the movement that could be used for predictions. Fixation durations at the chosen positions after feedback onset were modulated by action outcome (longer durations for misses than for hits) and outcome uncertainty (longer durations for narrow vs. clear outcomes). Combining both effects, durations were longest for narrow errors and shortest for clear hits, indicating that the chosen locations offer informational value for feedback processing. Thus, humans are able to use sensorimotor predictions to direct their gaze toward task-relevant feedback locations. Outcome-dependent saccade latency differences (miss vs. hit) indicate that also predictive valuation processes are involved in planning predictive saccades.NEW & NOTEWORTHY We elucidate the potential benefits of sensorimotor predictions, focusing on how the system actually uses this information to optimize feedback processing in goal-directed actions. Sensorimotor information is used to predict spatial parameters of movement outcomes, guiding predictive saccades toward future action effects. Saccade latencies and fixation durations are modulated by outcome quality, indicating that predictive valuation processes are considered and that the locations chosen are of high informational value for feedback processing.
Asunto(s)
Fijación Ocular , Objetivos , Desempeño Psicomotor , Movimientos Sacádicos , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Retroalimentación Sensorial/fisiologíaRESUMEN
Our eyes execute rapid, directional movements known as saccades, occurring several times per second, to focus on objects of interest in our environment. During these movements, visual sensitivity is temporarily reduced. Despite numerous studies on this topic, the underlying mechanism remains elusive, including a lingering debate on whether saccadic suppression affects the parvocellular visual pathway. To address this issue, we conducted a study employing steady-state visual evoked potentials (SSVEPs) elicited by chromatic and luminance stimuli while observers performed saccadic eye movements. We also employed an innovative analysis pipeline to enhance the signal-to-noise ratio, yielding superior results compared to the previous method. Our findings revealed a clear suppression effect on SSVEP signals during saccades compared to fixation periods. Notably, this suppression effect was comparable for both chromatic and luminance stimuli. We went further to measure the suppression effect across various contrast levels, which enabled us to model SSVEP responses with contrast response functions. The results suggest that saccades primarily reduce response gain without significantly affecting contrast gain and that this reduction applies uniformly to both chromatic and luminance pathways. In summary, our study provides robust evidence that saccades similarly suppress visual processing in both the parvocellular and magnocellular pathways within the human early visual cortex, as indicated by SSVEP responses. The observation that saccadic eye movements impact response gain rather than contrast gain implies that they influence visual processing through a multiplicative mechanism.NEW & NOTEWORTHY The present study demonstrates that saccadic eye movements reduce the processing of both luminance and chromatic stimuli in the early visual cortex of humans. By modeling the contrast response function, the study further shows that saccades affect visual processing by reducing the response gain rather than altering the contrast gain, suggesting that a multiplicative mechanism of visual attenuation affects both parvocellular and magnocellular pathways.
Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados Visuales , Movimientos Sacádicos , Corteza Visual , Humanos , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Masculino , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Estimulación LuminosaRESUMEN
People usually reach for objects to place them in some position and orientation, but the placement component of this sequence is often ignored. For example, reaches are influenced by gaze position, visual feedback, and memory delays, but their influence on object placement is unclear. Here, we tested these factors in a task where participants placed and oriented a trapezoidal block against two-dimensional (2-D) visual templates displayed on a frontally located computer screen. In experiment 1, participants matched the block to three possible orientations: 0° (horizontal), +45° and -45°, with gaze fixated 10° to the left/right. The hand and template either remained illuminated (closed-loop), or visual feedback was removed (open-loop). Here, hand location consistently overshot the template relative to gaze, especially in the open-loop task; likewise, orientation was influenced by gaze position (depending on template orientation and visual feedback). In experiment 2, a memory delay was added, and participants sometimes performed saccades (toward, away from, or across the template). In this task, the influence of gaze on orientation vanished, but location errors were influenced by both template orientation and final gaze position. Contrary to our expectations, the previous saccade metrics also impacted placement overshoot. Overall, hand orientation was influenced by template orientation in a nonlinear fashion. These results demonstrate interactions between gaze and orientation signals in the planning and execution of hand placement and suggest different neural mechanisms for closed-loop, open-loop, and memory delay placement.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Eye-hand coordination studies usually focus on object acquisition, but placement is equally important. We investigated how gaze position influences object placement toward a 2-D template with different levels of visual feedback. Like reach, placement overestimated goal location relative to gaze and was influenced by previous saccade metrics. Gaze also modulated hand orientation, depending on template orientation and level of visual feedback. Gaze influence was feedback-dependent, with location errors having no significant effect after a memory delay.
Asunto(s)
Retroalimentación Sensorial , Fijación Ocular , Mano , Memoria , Desempeño Psicomotor , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Mano/fisiología , Adulto , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Fenómenos Biomecánicos/fisiología , Retroalimentación Sensorial/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiologíaRESUMEN
Humans have the remarkable ability to integrate information from different senses, which greatly facilitates the detection, localization and identification of events in the environment. About 466 million people worldwide suffer from hearing loss. Yet, the impact of hearing loss on how the senses work together is rarely investigated. Here, we investigate how a common sensory impairment, asymmetric conductive hearing loss (AHL), alters the way our senses interact by examining human orienting behaviour with normal hearing (NH) and acute AHL. This type of hearing loss disrupts auditory localization. We hypothesized that this creates a conflict between auditory and visual spatial estimates and alters how auditory and visual inputs are integrated to facilitate multisensory spatial perception. We analysed the spatial and temporal properties of saccades to auditory, visual and audiovisual stimuli before and after plugging the right ear of participants. Both spatial and temporal aspects of multisensory integration were affected by AHL. Compared with NH, AHL caused participants to make slow, inaccurate and unprecise saccades towards auditory targets. Surprisingly, increased weight on visual input resulted in accurate audiovisual localization with AHL. This came at a cost: saccade latencies for audiovisual targets increased significantly. The larger the auditory localization errors, the less participants were able to benefit from audiovisual integration in terms of saccade latency. Our results indicate that observers immediately change sensory weights to effectively deal with acute AHL and preserve audiovisual accuracy in a way that cannot be fully explained by statistical models of optimal cue integration.
Asunto(s)
Localización de Sonidos , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Femenino , Adulto , Masculino , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Localización de Sonidos/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Pérdida Auditiva/fisiopatología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Percepción Espacial/fisiologíaRESUMEN
The regulation of emotions is a crucial facet of well-being and social adaptability, with explicit strategies receiving primary attention in prior research. Recent studies, however, emphasize the role of implicit emotion regulation, particularly implicating the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) in association with its implementation. This study delves into the nuanced role of the VMPFC through focality-optimized multichannel transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), shedding light on its causal involvement in implicit reappraisal. The primary goal was to evaluate the effectiveness of VMFPC-targeted tDCS and elucidate its role in individuals with high trait anxiety. Participants engaged in implicit and explicit emotion regulation tasks during multichannel tDCS targeting the VMPFC. The outcome measures encompassed negative emotion ratings, pupillary diameter, and saccade count, providing a comprehensive evaluation of emotion regulation efficiency. The intervention exhibited a notable impact, resulting in significant reductions in negative emotion ratings and pupillary reactions during implicit reappraisal, highlighting the indispensable role of the VMPFC in modulating emotional responses. Notably, these effects demonstrated sustained efficacy up to 1 day postintervention. This study underscores the potency of VMPFC-targeted multichannel tDCS in augmenting implicit emotion regulation. This not only contributes insights into the neural mechanisms of emotion regulation but also suggests innovative therapeutic avenues for anxiety disorders. The findings present a promising trajectory for future mood disorder interventions, bridging the gap between implicit emotion regulation and neural stimulation techniques.
Asunto(s)
Regulación Emocional , Corteza Prefrontal , Estimulación Transcraneal de Corriente Directa , Humanos , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/diagnóstico por imagen , Regulación Emocional/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Ansiedad/terapia , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Emociones/fisiologíaRESUMEN
Dysregulation of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) is commonly observed in various mental disorders, particularly when individuals engage in prolonged cognitive-emotional tasks that require ANS adjustment to workload. Although the understanding of the temporal dynamics of sympathetic and parasympathetic tones in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is limited, analyzing ANS reactions to cognitive-emotional workload could provide valuable insights into one of the underlying causes of OCD. This study investigated the temporal dynamics of heart rate (HR) and pupil area (PA) while participants with OCD and healthy volunteers solved antisaccade tasks, with affective pictures serving as central fixation stimuli. The data of 31 individuals with OCD and 30 healthy volunteers were included in the study, comprising three separate blocks, each lasting approximately 8 min. The results revealed an increase in sympathetic tone in the OCD group, with the most noticeable rise occurring during the middle part of each block, particularly during the presentation of negative stimuli. Healthy volunteers demonstrated adaptive temporal dynamics of HR and PA from the first block to the last block of tasks, whereas individuals with OCD exhibited fewer changes over time, suggesting a reduced adaptation of the ANS sympathetic tone to cognitive-emotional workload in OCD.
Asunto(s)
Sistema Nervioso Autónomo , Emociones , Frecuencia Cardíaca , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo , Pupila , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/fisiopatología , Adulto Joven , Emociones/fisiología , Sistema Nervioso Autónomo/fisiopatología , Pupila/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiologíaRESUMEN
Noise is a ubiquitous component of motor systems that leads to behavioral variability of all types of movements. Nonetheless, systems-based models investigating human movements are generally deterministic and explain only the central tendencies like mean trajectories. In this paper, a novel approach to modeling kinematic variability of movements is presented and tested on the oculomotor system. This approach reconciles the two prominent philosophies of saccade control: displacement-based control versus velocity-based control. This was achieved by quantifying the variability in saccadic eye movements and developing a stochastic model of its control. The proposed stochastic dual model generated significantly better fits of inter-trial variances of the saccade trajectories compared to existing models. These results suggest that the saccadic system can flexibly use the information of both desired displacement and velocity for its control. This study presents a potential framework for investigating computational principles of motor control in the presence of noise utilizing stochastic modeling of kinematic variability.
Asunto(s)
Movimientos Sacádicos , Humanos , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Fenómenos Biomecánicos/fisiología , Procesos Estocásticos , Adulto , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto Joven , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiologíaRESUMEN
People with multiple sclerosis (PwMS) who report dizziness often have gaze instability due to vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) deficiencies and compensatory saccade (CS) abnormalities. Herein, we aimed to describe and compare the gaze stabilization mechanisms for yaw and pitch head movements in PwMS. Thirty-seven PwMS (27 female, mean ± SD age = 53.4 ± 12.4 years old, median [IQR] Expanded Disability Status Scale Score = 3.5, [1.0]. We analyzed video head impulse test results for VOR gain, CS frequency, CS latency, gaze position error (GPE) at impulse end, and GPE at 400 ms after impulse start. Discrepancies were found for median [IQR] VOR gain in yaw (0.92 [0.14]) versus pitch-up (0.71 [0.44], p < 0.001) and pitch-down (0.81 [0.44], p = 0.014]), CS latency in yaw (258.13 [76.8]) ms versus pitch-up (208.78 [65.97]) ms, p = 0.001] and pitch-down (132.17 [97.56] ms, p = 0.006), GPE at impulse end in yaw (1.15 [1.85] degs versus pitch-up (2.71 [3.9] degs, p < 0.001), and GPE at 400 ms in yaw (-0.25 [0.98] degs) versus pitch-up (1.53 [1.07] degs, p < 0.001) and pitch-down (1.12 [1.82] degs, p = 0.001). Compared with yaw (0.91 [0.75]), CS frequency was similar for pitch-up (1.03 [0.93], p = 0.999) but lower for pitch-down (0.65 [0.64], p = 0.023). GPE at 400 ms was similar for yaw and pitch-down (1.88 [2.76] degs, p = 0.400). We postulate that MS may have preferentially damaged the vertical VOR and saccade pathways in this cohort.