Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 23
Filtrar
1.
Eur J Neurosci ; 59(12): 3151-3161, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38752321

RESUMEN

Regarding the stage of arousal level required for working memory to function properly, limited studies have been conducted on changes in working memory performance when the arousal level of consciousness decreases. This study aimed to experimentally clarify the stages of consciousness necessary for optimal working memory function. In this experiment, the sedation levels were changed step-by-step using anaesthesia, and the performance accuracy during the execution of working memory was assessed using a dual-task paradigm. Participants were required to categorize and remember words in a specific target category. Categorization performance was measured across four different sedative phases: before anaesthesia (baseline), and deep, moderate and light stages of sedation. Short-delay recognition tasks were performed under these four sedative stages, followed by long-delay recognition tasks after participants recovered from sedation. The results of the short-delay recognition task showed that the performance was lowest at the deep stage. The performance of the moderate stage was lower than the baseline. In the long-delay recognition task, the performance under moderate sedation was lower than that under baseline and light sedation. In addition, the performance under light sedation was lower than that under baseline. These results suggest that task performance becomes difficult under half sedation and that transferring information to long-term memory is difficult even under one-quarter sedation.


Asunto(s)
Nivel de Alerta , Estado de Conciencia , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Humanos , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/efectos de los fármacos , Masculino , Femenino , Estado de Conciencia/fisiología , Estado de Conciencia/efectos de los fármacos , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Hipnóticos y Sedantes/farmacología , Hipnóticos y Sedantes/administración & dosificación , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología
2.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 18: 1337798, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38545513

RESUMEN

Introduction: Hypoxia has been reported to impair psychological functions, such as working memory and decision-making. However, few studies have examined hypoxia's effect on social cognition. Methods: Using a self-referential task, the present study investigated normobaric hypoxia's effect on the self-referential process. Additionally, we measured brain activity during the task with fNIRS and performed conventional univariate analysis with the general linear model (GLM) as well as homologous cortical functional connectivity analysis. Results: The results revealed that normobaric hypoxia impaired recognition of adjectives in the other-reference condition but not in the self-reference. The GLM analysis did not detect differences in brain activity between the self- and other-reference conditions, suggesting that GLM analysis may not be suitable for examining self- and other-reference conditions' neural correlates. The homologous cortical connectivity analysis revealed that the connectivity's magnitude was greater in the self-reference than in the other-reference conditions in the normoxic group. However, such a decrease in connectivity in the other-reference conditions was not observed in the hypoxic group, possibly to compensate for cognitive decline induced by the hypoxia. Conclusion: Considering that homologous connectivity reflects the default mode network, which is supposedly linked to continuous self-reference, stable strength of the connectivity in the self-reference condition under the hypoxia may suggest robust nature of the self-reference process under normobaric hypoxia.

3.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 49(10): 1539-1556, 2023 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37307321

RESUMEN

Although working memory (WM) is usually defined as a cognitive system coordinating processing and storage in the short term, in most WM models, memory aspects have been developed more fully than processing systems, and many studies of WM tasks have tended to focus on memory performance. The present study investigated WM functioning without focusing exclusively on short-term memory performance by presenting participants with an n-back task on letters, n varying from 0 to 2, each letter being followed by a tone discrimination task involving from one to three tones. Predictions regarding the reciprocal effects of these tasks on each other were motivated by the time-based resource-sharing (TBRS) theoretical framework for WM that assumes the temporal sharing of attention between processing and memory. Although, as predicted, increasing the n value had a detrimental effect on tone discrimination in terms of accuracy and response times, and increasing the number of tones disrupted speed and accuracy on n-back performance, the overall pattern of results did not perfectly fit the TBRS predictions. Nonetheless, the main alternative models of WM do not seem to offer a complete account. The present findings point toward the need to use a larger range of tasks and situations in designing and testing models of WM. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Humanos , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción
4.
Front Psychol ; 14: 1140399, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37275713

RESUMEN

Recent research has shown that the Default Mode Network (DMN) typically exhibits increased activation during processing of social and personal information but shows deactivation during working memory (WM) tasks. Previously, we reported the Frontal Parietal Network (FPN) and DMN showed coactivation during task preparation whereas the DMN exhibited deactivation during task execution in working memory tasks. Aging research has shown that older adults exhibited decreased functional connectivity in the DMN relative to younger adults. Here, we investigated whether age-related cognitive decline is related to a reduced relationship between the FPN and DMN using a working memory task during the execution period. First, we replicated our previous finding that the FPN and DMN showed coactivation during the preparation period, whereas the DMN showed deactivation during the execution period. The older adults showed reduced DMN activity during task preparation and reduced deactivation during task execution; however, they exhibited a higher magnitude of activation in the FPN than the young individuals during task execution. Functional connectivity analyses showed that the elderly group, compared to the young group, showed weaker correlations within the FPN and the DMN, weaker positive correlations between the FPN and DMN during task preparation, and weaker negative correlations between the FPN and DMN during execution. The results suggest that cognitive decline in the older adults might be related to reduced connectivity within the DMN as well as between the FPN and DMN.

5.
Neuroscience ; 503: 28-44, 2022 11 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36087900

RESUMEN

Some studies have argued that the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) is generally activated in response to aversive information, including pain, negative affect, and cognitive conflict. Other studies have claimed that the dACC has subdivisions, and each division has a specific function. By manipulating emotionally and cognitively aversive cues, the present study determined whether the dACC is generally responsive to aversiveness or it has subdivisions for specific forms of aversiveness. Conjunction functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) analysis showed that emotionally and cognitively aversive cues activated the same portion of the dACC. When these cues were contiguously presented, the region demonstrated additive activity, further supporting the overlapping representation of the two different forms of aversiveness in the dACC. Additional effective connectivity analysis showed that the dACC was co-activated with different brain regions depending on the cue type, characterizing its behavioral control mechanism. Complementary multivariate analyses showed that the reaction time was negatively correlated with the activity of the dACC and that the activity of the dACC under the emotional cue was predicted by the individual state anxiety score but not under the cognitive cue. We also found that the superior part of the dACC was uniquely activated in response to cognitively aversive cues, partially supporting the functional segregation account. Collectively, our results provide evidence that the specific locus of the dACC is generally responsive to distinctive motivational information, whereas the other loci may have segregated functions. Discussion includes recent neurocomputational theories that seem to satisfactorily account for the present results.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Giro del Cíngulo , Giro del Cíngulo/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Emociones/fisiología , Afecto
6.
Nat Commun ; 10(1): 4287, 2019 09 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31537795

RESUMEN

Performance anxiety can profoundly affect motor performance, even in experts such as professional athletes and musicians. Previously, the neural mechanisms underlying anxiety-induced performance deterioration have predominantly been investigated for individual one-shot actions. Sports and music, however, are characterized by action sequences, where many individual actions are assembled to develop a performance. Here, utilizing a novel differential sequential motor learning paradigm, we first show that performance at the junctions between pre-learnt action sequences is particularly prone to anxiety. Next, utilizing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we reveal that performance deterioration at the junctions is parametrically correlated with activity in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC). Finally, we show that 1 Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the dACC attenuates the performance deterioration at the junctions. These results demonstrate causality between dACC activity and impairment of sequential motor performance due to anxiety, and suggest new intervention techniques against the deterioration.


Asunto(s)
Giro del Cíngulo/fisiología , Ansiedad de Desempeño/fisiopatología , Ansiedad de Desempeño/terapia , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Ansiedad de Desempeño/psicología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
7.
Neurosci Conscious ; 2018(1): niy002, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30042855

RESUMEN

Feature binding is considered to be the basis for conscious stimulus perception, while anaesthetics exert a gradient effect on the loss of consciousness (LOC). By integrating these two streams of research, the present study assessed the effect of two anaesthetic agents (i.e. propofol and midazolam) on audio-spatial feature binding. We also recorded the electrophysiological activity of the frontal channels. Using pharmacokinetic simulation, we determined the effect-site concentration (Ce) of the anaesthetics at loss of response to verbal command and eyelash reflex. We subsequently adjusted Ce to 75%, 50% and 25% of Ce-LOC to achieve deep, moderate and light sedation, respectively. Behavioural results showed that moderate sedation selectively disrupted feature binding. The frontal channels showed a P3 component (350-600 ms peristimulus period) following the presentation of audio-spatial stimuli at baseline and under moderate and light sedations. Critically, the late event-related potential component (600-1000 ms) returned to the pre-activated level (0-350 ms) at baseline and under light sedation but was sustained under moderate sedation. We propose that audio-spatial feature binding may require the presence of a P3 component and its subsequent and sufficient decline, as under anaesthetic-induced moderate sedation the P3 component was sustained and featured binding was impaired.

8.
PLoS One ; 12(9): e0183635, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28880899

RESUMEN

Rather than relying solely on subjective pain evaluation using means such as the visual analogue scale (VAS), in clinical situations it is possible to observe evoked responses of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) as objective indicators. Few studies, however, have reported these relationships under finely controlled sedation. 16 healthy male participants were administrated in intravenous sedation with either propofol or midazolam randomly. We initially determined, using pharmacokinetic simulation, the effect-site concentration (Ce) of anaesthetic at loss of response to verbal command and eyelash reflex (Ce-LOR). Then subsequently adjusted Ce to 75%, 50%, and 25% of Ce-LOR to achieve deep, moderate, and light sedation. At awake control state and each sedation level, a noxious electrical stimulation was applied three times at the right forearm, an average pain intensity of the three stimuli was rated on a VAS (0-10). Changes in the peripheral perfusion index measured by oximetry were used as an indicator of ANS response. We analyzed the influence of sedation level on VAS and ANS responses compared to the awake control state. While ANS responses were similar in all conditions, VAS was statistically significantly lower in moderate (5.6±0.6, p <0.005) or deep (5.3±0.6, p <0.001) sedation than in the awake state (7.2±0.4). This study revealed that even when the ANS responds similarly to the same stimulation, subjective pain perception is attenuated by sedation. A cerebral mechanism other than that of the brainstem might determine subjective pain intensity.


Asunto(s)
Sistema Nervioso Autónomo/efectos de los fármacos , Hipnóticos y Sedantes/uso terapéutico , Midazolam/uso terapéutico , Dolor/tratamiento farmacológico , Propofol/uso terapéutico , Adulto , Anestésicos Intravenosos , Sedación Consciente , Humanos , Hipnóticos y Sedantes/administración & dosificación , Masculino , Midazolam/administración & dosificación , Dimensión del Dolor , Propofol/administración & dosificación , Adulto Joven
9.
Front Psychol ; 6: 1811, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26635703

RESUMEN

One form of communication that is common in all cultures is people singing together. Singing together reflects an index of cognitive synchronization and cooperation of human brains. Little is known about the neural synchronization mechanism, however. Here, we examined how two brains make one synchronized behavior using cooperated singing/humming between two people and hyperscanning, a new brain scanning technique. Hyperscanning allowed us to observe dynamic cooperation between interacting participants. We used functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to simultaneously record the brain activity of two people while they cooperatively sang or hummed a song in face-to-face (FtF) or face-to-wall (FtW) conditions. By calculating the inter-brain wavelet transform coherence between two interacting brains, we found a significant increase in the neural synchronization of the left inferior frontal cortex (IFC) for cooperative singing or humming regardless of FtF or FtW compared with singing or humming alone. On the other hand, the right IFC showed an increase in neural synchronization for humming only, possibly due to more dependence on musical processing.

10.
Cortex ; 71: 277-90, 2015 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26280275

RESUMEN

Neuroimaging and behavioral evidence has suggested that the lateral prefrontal cortex is involved in individual differences in working memory capacity (WMC). However, few studies have localized the neural structures that differentiate high and low WMC individuals, considering the functional architecture of the prefrontal cortex. The present study aimed to identify a frontal region that underlies individual differences from the perspective of the hierarchical architecture of the frontal cortex. By manipulating an episodic factor of cognitive control (control in selecting an appropriate task set according to a temporal context) and using a parametric modulation analysis, we found that both high- and low- WMC individuals have similar activation patterns in the premotor cortex (BA6, 8), caudal prefrontal cortex (BA44, 45), and frontopolar cortex (BA10, 11), but differed in the rostral part of the prefrontal cortex (BA46/47); high WMC individuals showed greater activation in the higher episodic control condition, whereas low WMC individuals showed reduced activation when episodic control was required. Similar patterns of activation were found in the right inferior parietal and middle/inferior temporal cortices. These results indicate that the rostral prefrontal cortex, which supports episodic cognitive control, possibly by sending a weighting signal toward the inferior parietal and middle/inferior temporal cortices that modulate saliency and sensory processing, underlies individual differences in WMC. Episodic control account, which considers the organization of the prefrontal cortex, fits well with previous findings of individual differences in WMC.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Función Ejecutiva , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Neuroimagen , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Lectura , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Adulto Joven
11.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 77(5): 1659-73, 2015 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25813738

RESUMEN

Studies on visual cognitive load have reported inconsistent effects of distractor interference when distractors have visual characteristic that are similar to the cognitive load. Some studies have shown that the cognitive load enhances distractor interference, while others reported an attenuating effect. We attribute these inconsistencies to the amount of cognitive load that a person is required to maintain. Lower amounts of cognitive load increase distractor interference by orienting attention toward visually similar distractors. Higher amounts of cognitive load attenuate distractor interference by depleting attentional resources needed to process distractors. In the present study, cognitive load consisted of faces (Experiments 1-3) or scenes (Experiment 2). Participants performed a selective attention task in which they ignored face distractors while judging a color of a target dot presented nearby, under differing amounts of load. Across these experiments distractor interference was greater in the low-load condition and smaller in the high-load condition when the content of the cognitive load had similar visual characteristic to the distractors. We also found that when a series of judgments needed to be made, the effect was apparent for the first trial but not for the second. We further tested an involvement of working memory capacity (WMC) in the load effect (Experiment 3). Interestingly, both high and low WMC groups received an equivalent effect of the cognitive load in the first distractor, suggesting these effects are fairly automatic.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Análisis de Varianza , Cara , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio/fisiología , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Enmascaramiento Perceptual/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
12.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 8: 992, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25538609

RESUMEN

The posterior parietal cortex (PPC) has two attentional functions: top-down attentional control and stimulus-driven attentional processing. Using the focused version of the reading span test (RST), in which the target word to be remembered is the critical word for comprehending a sentence (focused word) or a non-focused word, we examined the effect of tDCS on resolution of distractor interference by the focused word in the non-focus condition (top-down attentional control) and on augmented/shrunk attentional capture by the focused word in both the focus and non-focus conditions (stimulus-driven attentional processing). Participants were divided into two groups: anodal tDCS (atDCS) and cathodal tDCS (ctDCS). Online stimulation was given while participants performed the RST. A post-hoc recognition task was also administered in which three kinds of words were presented: target words in the RST, distractor words in the RST, and novel words. atDCS augmented the effect of the focused word by increasing differences in performance between the focus and non-focus conditions. Such an effect was not observed in the ctDCS group. As for the recognition task, atDCS again produced the augmented effect of the focused words in the distractor recognition. On the other hand, ctDCS brought less recognition of non-focused target words in comparison to sham. The results indicate that atDCS promotes stimulus-driven attentional processing, possibly by affecting neural firing in the inferior parietal regions. In contrast, ctDCS appears to prevent retrieval of less important information from episodic memory, which may require top-down attentional processing.

13.
Front Psychol ; 5: 1285, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25426092

RESUMEN

The storage and processing of spatial information is done by spatial working memory. To measure spatial working memory, the Corsi blocks task, which separates the memory into two types, forward and backward, is often used. Although it had been thought that backward recall requires more of the executive function than forward recall, some studies have shown otherwise. Here, we focused on the spatial and sequential aspects of the Corsi blocks task to investigate cognitive processes by dissociating forward and backward recall. We used a dual task method (serial articulatory suppression or spatial tapping as the secondary task) and analyzed two kinds of errors (position error and order error) to investigate cognitive performance during the forward and backward recall. We ran two experiments: in experiment 1, we employed the standard Corsi blocks task, and in experiment 2, we employed the modified Corsi blocks task in order to prevent verbal strategies. We found that spatial tapping affected both forward and backward recall, while serial articulatory suppression increased the number of order errors in the backward condition. These results indicate that stronger order representation is required for backward recall in the Corsi blocks task.

14.
Springerplus ; 3: 546, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25279333

RESUMEN

A discrepancy exists among previous studies regarding whether priming and subsequent recognition memory are positively or negatively correlated. We consider that the difference in recognition memory measures used in these studies accounts for the discrepancy. To examine this, we introduced three different recognition measures and reexamined the relationship between priming and subsequent recognition. Participants learned stimulus words in the first encoding block while performing an abstract/concrete decision task. In the second encoding block, a priming test was conducted, followed by a surprise recognition memory test. Results showed that the hit rate and hit rate (pHit)-false-alarm rate (pFA) positively correlated with priming. However, the difference between hit rates for the twice- and once-encoded stimuli, which can reflect the representations acquired at the second exposure in particular, did not significantly correlate with priming. These results suggest that priming and subsequent recognition relate positively because of the common representations acquired at the initial encoding. Furthermore, the present results are consistent with a previous study that failed to reproduce the negative correlation between priming and subsequent recognition.

15.
Sci Rep ; 4: 5954, 2014 Aug 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25092432

RESUMEN

The Default Mode Network (DMN) regions exhibit deactivation during a wide variety of resource demanding tasks. However, recent brain imaging studies reported that they also show activation during various cognitive activities. In addition, studies have found a negative correlation between the DMN and the working memory network (WMN). Here, we investigated activity in the DMN and WMN regions during preparation and execution phases of a verbal working memory task. Results showed that the core DMN regions, including the medial prefrontal cortex and posterior cingulate cortex, and WMN regions were activated during preparation. During execution, however, the WMN regions were activated but the DMN regions were deactivated. The results suggest that activation of these network regions is affected by allocation of attentional resources to the task relevant regions due to task demands. This study extends our previous results by showing that the core DMN regions exhibit activation during task preparation and deactivation during task execution.


Asunto(s)
Giro del Cíngulo/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Adulto , Atención/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Giro del Cíngulo/anatomía & histología , Humanos , Masculino , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Corteza Prefrontal/anatomía & histología , Descanso/fisiología
16.
Sci Rep ; 4: 5828, 2014 Jul 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25059843

RESUMEN

Serial changes of humor comprehension evoked by a well organized four-frame comic Manga were investigated by fMRI in each step of humor comprehension. The neural substrates underlying the amusing effects in response to funny and mixed order manga were compared. In accordance with the time course of the four frames, fMRI activations changed serially. Beginning with the second frame (development scene), activation of the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) was observed, followed by activations in the temporal and frontal areas during viewing of the third frame (turn scene). For the fourth frame (punch line), strong increased activations were confirmed in the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) and cerebellum. Interestingly, distinguishable activation differences in the cerebellum between funny and non-funny conditions were also found for the fourth frame. These findings suggest that humor comprehension evokes activation that initiates in the TPJ and expands to the MPFC and cerebellum at the convergence level.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Comprensión/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Cerebelo/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Radiografía
17.
PLoS One ; 9(1): e86036, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24454951

RESUMEN

Different people make different responses when they face a frustrating situation: some punish others (extrapunitive), while others punish themselves (intropunitive). Few studies have investigated the neural structures that differentiate extrapunitive and intropunitive individuals. The present fMRI study explored these neural structures using two different frustrating situations: an ego-blocking situation which blocks a desire or goal, and a superego-blocking situation which blocks self-esteem. In the ego-blocking condition, the extrapunitive group (n = 9) showed greater activation in the bilateral ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, indicating that these individuals prefer emotional processing. On the other hand, the intropunitive group (n = 9) showed greater activation in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, possibly reflecting an effortful control for anger reduction. Such patterns were not observed in the superego-blocking condition. These results indicate that the prefrontal cortex is the source of individual differences in aggression direction in the ego-blocking situation.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Castigo/psicología , Ira , Mapeo Encefálico , Ego , Femenino , Frustación , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Autoimagen , Adulto Joven
18.
Sci Rep ; 3: 1375, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23459220

RESUMEN

The present study investigated when emotion modulates working memory from the perspective of neural activation. Using fMRI, we measured brain activity during the encoding and retrieval phases of a reading span test (RST) that used emotional contexts. The emotional RST required participants to read sentences that elicited negative, neural or positive emotional states while they were memorizing target words from the sentences. Compared with the neutral RST, the negative RST activated the right amygdala during the reading phase. Significant activation was also found in the parahippocampal gyrus, albeit only after activation of the amygdala became comparable to that in the neutral RST. In contrast, the positive RST activated the substantia nigra during the reading phase relative to the neutral RST. These findings suggest that negative and positive emotions modulate working memory through distinctive neural circuits. We also discuss possible relationships between emotional modulation and working memory capacity.


Asunto(s)
Emociones/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Adulto , Conducta/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Lenguaje , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Lectura , Adulto Joven
19.
Neuroreport ; 23(1): 26-9, 2012 Jan 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22113213

RESUMEN

Although the role of the amygdala in processing facial expressions of fear is well established, its role in the processing of other emotions, such as sadness, remains unclear. We hypothesized that the amygdala would respond to a negative emotion such as sadness, when sadness was represented by a theatrical mask. In the traditional Japanese Noh theater, performers use masks to indicate many of the mental states of the characters they portray. Here, we report a functional MRI study, in which participants' brains were scanned while viewing Noh masks, whose faces appeared delicately sad. Among seventy standard Noh masks previously rated by the individual participants, we chose six top-rated sad masks and six neutral masks to study the neural correlates of such delicate sadness. Results based on a region of interest analysis indicated the activation of the right amygdala while viewing sad masks. We suggest the fact that such delicate sad masks could activate the amygdala, and it could possibly be because of an underlying similarity to emotions such as fear and disgust.


Asunto(s)
Amígdala del Cerebelo/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Cara/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Amígdala del Cerebelo/anatomía & histología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Estética/psicología , Expresión Facial , Miedo/fisiología , Miedo/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Red Nerviosa/fisiología
20.
Brain Res ; 1429: 82-97, 2012 Jan 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22093949

RESUMEN

Previous studies have shown that goal-irrelevant distractors are incidentally encoded into long-term memory. Neuroimaging studies have suggested that the medial temporal and visual association regions are involved in incidental encoding of goal-irrelevant information. However, few studies have investigated prefrontal/parietal influence during the incidental encoding. The present study performed whole brain analysis to identify the brain regions involved in the incidental encoding of goal-irrelevant information. A face working memory (WM) task was administered with insertion of face distractors during the delay period. Following the WM task, a surprise recognition task was given in an MRI scanner. Recognition rate of distractors was higher than that of novel fillers. Recognition time was also faster in distractors than in novel fillers. Neuroimaging results showed less activation to distractors subsequently remembered than those forgotten in the middle and superior frontal regions and the lateral inferior parietal lobe including the angular gyrus and the temporoparietal regions. However, the left anterior hippocampus and the right fusiform gyrus showed greater activation to distractors subsequently remembered. Those findings suggest that insufficient engagement of the dorsal frontal cortex which regulates attentional control and the inferior parietal lobe which functions to reorient attention may allow goal-irrelevant information access to working memory and to be encoded into long-term memory.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Interpretación de Imagen Asistida por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA