Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 9 de 9
Filtrar
1.
J Sex Res ; 52(9): 1017-27, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25211014

RESUMEN

Sexual assertiveness has been defined in a number of ways by many researchers, with different aspects of sexual assertiveness emphasized in different measures. Most previous measures have included condom insistence as an important aspect of sexual assertiveness, but this may not translate well to women at all life stages or in varied types of relationships. The goal of the current study was to develop a comprehensive measure of sexual assertiveness that encompasses the aspects of sexual assertiveness that have been emphasized by previous researchers, with the exception of condom insistence. Items were generated based on previous measures and definitions, and an exploratory factor analysis was conducted (Study 1) to better understand the dimensions of sexual assertiveness. The proposed scale was revised and further refined using both exploratory factor analysis and confirmatory factor analysis in Study 2. The final scale consisted of 18 items that seem to capture three dimensions of sexual assertiveness: the ability to initiate and communicate about desired sex, the ability to refuse unwanted sex, and the ability to communicate about sexual history and risk. Model fit indices indicate that this three-factor solution fits the data well. Implications and suggestions for future research are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Asertividad , Conducta Sexual/psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Condones/estadística & datos numéricos , Análisis Factorial , Femenino , Humanos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
2.
Am J Psychol ; 127(2): 215-31, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24934012

RESUMEN

The present studies used exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses to explore the degree to which probability discounting processes are similar to delay discounting processes. To determine whether these processes are similar, 2 questions were addressed: the degree to which probability discounting outcomes can be categorized into multiple domains (as demonstrated for delay discounting) and whether the inverse magnitude effect would be observed for nonmonetary outcomes. An exploratory factor analysis was conducted using data from the fill-in-the-blank method (Study 1), followed by a confirmatory factor analysis using data from a multiple-choice method (Study 2) as a replication. These studies provide support for the idea that outcomes can be subdivided into multiple domains. Generally, the discounting rates were steeper for tangible outcomes than nontangible outcomes, and a magnitude effect was observed that was consistent with, rather than the inverse of, that observed for delay discounting tasks. Complexities related to the relationship between probability discounting processes and delay discounting processing are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Anticipación Psicológica , Negación en Psicología , Análisis Factorial , Motivación , Aprendizaje por Probabilidad , Psicometría/estadística & datos numéricos , Incertidumbre , Adolescente , Cultura , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
3.
J Gambl Stud ; 30(2): 335-47, 2014 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23283635

RESUMEN

The gambling functional assessment-revised (GFA-R) was designed to assess whether the respondent's gambling was maintained by positive reinforcement or escape. The present study attempted to validate the GFA-R's psychometric properties using United Kingdom (UK) university students and to compare the results to those from a sample of American university students. Two hundred seventy four UK students completed the GFA-R, and 153 also completed the South Oaks gambling screen (SOGS). Two hundred one United States (US) university students completed both measures. A confirmatory factor analysis indicated that the original model of the GFA-R provided an excellent fit for the UK data and internal consistency was high. These outcomes were similar, if not superior, to those from the US sample. SOGS scores strongly correlated with GFA-R escape subscale scores in both samples, replicating previous results. These findings indicate that the GFA-R is a valid measure for use in the UK, which is potentially useful to both practitioners and researchers. They also suggest that the strong relationship between endorsing gambling as an escape and measures of disordered gambling may be ubiquitous.


Asunto(s)
Juego de Azar/psicología , Estudiantes/psicología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Psicometría , Refuerzo en Psicología , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Estudiantes/estadística & datos numéricos , Reino Unido , Estados Unidos , Universidades , Adulto Joven
4.
Behav Modif ; 35(6): 553-69, 2011 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21885468

RESUMEN

An attempt was made to modify the Gambling Functional Assessment (GFA), which was proposed to identify four possible contingencies maintaining the respondent's gambling behavior. However, previous research found that it only identified two contingencies (i.e., positive vs. negative reinforcement), with some items cross-loading on both contingencies and one not loading at all. A total of 1,060 undergraduate students completed a revised version of the GFA containing 22 items. Exploratory factor analyses conducted on a random selection of half of the participants led to a two-factor solution (positive and negative reinforcement) for 16 of the items that strongly loaded on the two factors. Confirmatory factor analyses conducted using structural equation modeling on the data from the other half of the sample confirmed the two-factor model. The GFA-Revised consists of 16 items, 8 each measuring positive and negative reinforcement contingencies. Although this revised measure cleanly parses the two contingencies, the data indicate that gambling maintained by positive reinforcement is more frequent than gambling maintained by negative reinforcement. This outcome will make directly comparing the two contingencies difficult, especially given that evidence suggests that gambling maintained by negative reinforcement is more strongly associated with pathology than gambling maintained by positive reinforcement.


Asunto(s)
Juego de Azar/diagnóstico , Juego de Azar/psicología , Psicometría/instrumentación , Encuestas y Cuestionarios/normas , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicometría/estadística & datos numéricos , Refuerzo en Psicología , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Adulto Joven
5.
J Couns Psychol ; 58(4): 481-93, 2011 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21728412

RESUMEN

This article describes the development and validation of the Internalized Racial Oppression Scale (IROS) for Black individuals in 2 studies using a total sample of 468 Black college students. The IROS is intended to measure the degree to which racial oppression is internalized and replicated by Black individuals in the United States. An exploratory factor analysis suggested a five-factor solution: Belief in the Biased Representation of History (BRH), Devaluation of the African Worldview and Motifs (DAW), Alteration of Physical Appearance (APA), Internalization of Negative Stereotypes (INS), and Hair Change (HC). Confirmatory factory analysis supported an adequate model fit of a four-factor model: BRH, APA, INS, and HC. All factors of the IROS were positively correlated with the Pre-Encounter subscale of the Racial Identity Attitude Scale (RIAS-B; J. E. Helms & T. A. Parham, 1996), and 4 of the factors were negatively correlated with the Immersion/Emersion subscale of the RIAS-B. Four factors of the IROS were negatively correlated with all subscales and total scores of the African Self-Consciousness Scale (J. A. Baldwin & Y. R. Bell, 1985). These results provide some support of the validity of the IROS.


Asunto(s)
Negro o Afroamericano/psicología , Prejuicio , Identificación Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Negro o Afroamericano/estadística & datos numéricos , Anciano , Análisis de Varianza , Análisis Factorial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Psicometría , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Autoimagen , Deseabilidad Social , Sudeste de Estados Unidos , Estereotipo , Estudiantes/psicología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
6.
J Gen Psychol ; 138(1): 35-48, 2011.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21404948

RESUMEN

Delay discounting occurs when individuals are increasingly willing to accept less than the full amount of something to receive it immediately as the delay to the full amount increases. Previous research has reported different "domains" of commodities, with discounting in one domain not being informative about discounting in another. In the present study, 283 participants completed a delay-discounting task with 1 of 2 sets of 5 commodities identical to those used in previous research (Weatherly, Terrell, & Derenne, 2010). Confirmatory factor analyses determined that the observed discounting conformed to previously reported domains. Also replicated was finding that discounting differs both as a function of commodity and how the commodity is framed. These results provide strong support for the idea that temporal decisions made about certain consequences may not necessarily be informative about how an individual will make decisions about other consequences. Also, the framing of the decision can significantly influence observed levels of discounting, potentially clouding interpretation.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones , Inhibición Psicológica , Motivación , Psicometría/estadística & datos numéricos , Esquema de Refuerzo , Percepción del Tiempo , Adolescente , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Adulto Joven
7.
J Gen Psychol ; 137(3): 273-86, 2010.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20718227

RESUMEN

When outcomes are delayed, their value is decreased. Delay discounting is a much-studied topic because it is correlated with certain disorders (e.g., pathological gambling). The present study attempts to determine how people would delay discount a number of different commodities, ranging from money to dating partners to federal education legislation. Participants completed delay discounting tasks pertaining to 5 different commodities, with a different set of 5 commodities for 2 groups. Results showed that different commodities were often discounted differently. Both data sets were also subjected to factor analysis. A 2-factor solution was found for both, suggesting that there are multiple "domains" of commodities. This finding is of interest because it suggests that measuring delay discounting for one commodity within a particular domain of commodities will be predictive of how people discount other commodities within that domain but will not be predictive of how they discount commodities within another domain.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Adolescente , Área Bajo la Curva , Análisis Factorial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Adulto Joven
8.
Aggress Behav ; 35(3): 259-73, 2009.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19291759

RESUMEN

The effect of sex, status, and mating cues on expected aggression was examined via three scenario-based studies in which participants imagined themselves in a situation with a same-sex instigator of a provocation. Participants were randomly assigned to receive a scenario, which included one of two levels of status of instigator (high, low), one of two levels of attractiveness of the instigator (unattractive, attractive), and one of two levels of provocation (apology, insult). Sex and dispositional aggressivity were also included in a full factorial design. Based on evolutionary psychology ideas, we anticipated that status and attractiveness would differentially influence expected aggression for men vs. women. Participants in Experiment 1 were instructed to imagine that they were alone, whereas participants in Experiments 2 and 3 imagined themselves in a situation that included mating-related primes. In general expected aggression was greater for aggression-prone participants and under conditions of provocation and/or a high-status instigator. Experiments 2 and 3 found that, in the context of mate competition, sex differences in the effects of instigator provocation, status, and attractiveness emerged: greater aggressivity now only predicted more aggression for males but not females who were insulted; aggression was highest for females confronting an unattractive, high-status instigator and for males confronting an attractive, high-status instigator; females were more likely to aggress against a high-status instigator, regardless of being in a steady relationship or a first date situation, but males were only more likely to aggress against a high-status instigator in a first date situation.


Asunto(s)
Agresión/psicología , Jerarquia Social , Conducta Sexual/psicología , Percepción Social , Adolescente , Análisis de Varianza , Belleza , Evolución Biológica , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Factores Sexuales , Adulto Joven
9.
Br J Psychol ; 100(Pt 4): 675-98, 2009 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19236793

RESUMEN

Two studies examined correlates of the Narrative Emplotment Scale (NES), which measures the extent to which individuals perceive chance events and unchosen experiences as meaningfully connected. In Study 1 (N=99), the NES demonstrated adequate test-retest stability and good internal reliability. The scale was positively related to paranormal beliefs, mystical experiences, and absorption. In Study 2 (N=342), personality measures indicative of external locus of control, intrinsic religiosity, well-being, satisfaction with life, and a measure of frequency of coincidence experience were all positively correlated with narrative emplotment, providing further support for the construct validity of the scale. In terms of the question of whether meaning making is predictive of better or worse psychological adjustment, analyses indicated that the relationship between narrative emplotment and psychological adjustment was moderated by individual differences in coping strategies. Path analysis indicated that emplotment was a mediator of the pathway between religiosity and well-being. Emplotment had a negative effect on well-being through chance locus of control. These analyses suggest that this type of meaning-making is an important variable for understanding religious/spiritual beliefs and their influence on psychological adjustment.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Psicológica , Asociación , Cultura , Misticismo , Narración , Parapsicología , Inventario de Personalidad/estadística & datos numéricos , Calidad de Vida/psicología , Comprensión , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Control Interno-Externo , Masculino , Psicometría/estadística & datos numéricos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Espiritualidad , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA