Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 56
Filtrar
1.
Infant Ment Health J ; 45(3): 276-285, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38343150

RESUMO

In this cross-sectional study performed in Canada, we evaluated the frustration levels of prepartum and postpartum mother and father couple-pairs. Our goal was to determine if there were differences in frustration levels between mothers and fathers while listening to prolonged infant crying, and further, how frustration levels might differ between prepartum and postpartum samples. Using two discrete groups, prepartum (Sample 1; N = 48) and postpartum (Sample 2; N = 44) mother and father couple-pairs completed 600 s of listening to audio-recorded infant cry sounds. Participants continuously reported their subjective frustration using a computerized Continuous Visual Analog Scale (CVAS). There was no significant difference in frustration responses between mothers and fathers across both prepartum and postpartum samples. Postpartum mothers and fathers experienced greater frustration than their prepartum counterparts, and frustration increased faster in postpartum couples compared to prepartum couples. Informing first-time parents of the universal experiences of frustration to prolonged crying bouts that are characteristic of their infant's early weeks of life may lead to greater understanding towards their infant, and perhaps decreased instances of harmful responses.


En este estudio transeccional, evaluamos los niveles de frustración de las parejas de mamás y papás antes y después del parto. Nuestro propósito fue determinar si hay diferencias entre mamás y papás en cuanto a los niveles de frustración mientras escuchan el prolongado llanto del infante, y cómo los niveles de frustración pudieran diferir entre grupos­muestra antes y después del parto. Usando dos grupos discretos, antes del parto (grupo­muestra 1; N = 48) y después del parto (grupo­muestra 2; N = 44), las parejas de mamás y papás completaron 600 segundos escuchando sonidos grabados en audio de llanto de infante. Los participantes continuamente reportaron su frustración subjetiva usando una escala análoga visual continua computarizada (CVAS). No hubo diferencia significativa en las respuestas de frustración entre mamás y papás a lo largo de los grupos­muestra tanto antes del parto como después del parto. Las mamás y papás en el grupo­muestra después del parto experimentaron mayor frustración que sus homólogos en el grupo­muestra antes del parto, y la frustración aumentó más rápido en las parejas del grupo­muestra después del parto tal como se les comparó con las parejas del grupo­muestra antes del parto. Estos resultados sugieren que las parejas primíparas posterior al parto están más propensas a experimentar considerables cantidades de frustración como respuesta al llanto del infante después que el bebé ha nacido. Informarles a los progenitores primerizos acerca de las experiencias generales de la frustración a los prolongados ataques de llanto que son característicos de las primeras semanas de vida de su infante pudiera llevar a una mayor comprensión hacia su infante y quizás disminuir las instancias de respuestas dañinas.


Dans cette étude transversale nous avons évalué les niveaux de frustration des couples­paires mère et père avant et après la naissance. Notre but était de déterminer s'il existe des différences entres les mères et les pères dans leurs niveaux de frustration en entendant des pleurs de bébé prolongés et de quelle manière les niveaux pourraient différer entre les échantillons avant la naissance et après la naissance. En utilisant deux groupes discrets, avant la naissance (Echantillon 1; N = 48) et après la naissance (Echantillon 2; N = 44) les couples­paires mère et père ont écouté 600 seconds d'enregistrements de pleurs de bébés. Les participants ont fait état de leur frustration subjective en utilisant une échelle analogique visuelle continue informatisée (CVAS). Il s'est avéré n'y avoir aucune différence importante dans les réactions de frustration entre les mères et les pères au travers des échantillons à la fois avant l'accouchement et après l'accouchement. Ces résultats suggèrent que les coupes postpartum primipares sont plus à même de faire l'expérience de niveaux élevés de frustration en réaction aux pleurs du bébé une fois le bébé arrivé. Informer les parents qui sont parents pour la première fois des expériences universelles de frustration aux crises de pleurs prolongées qui caractérisent les premières semaines de la vie des bébés peut mener à une plus grande compréhension de leur bébé et peut­être à une baisse des case d réactions néfastes.


Assuntos
Choro , Pai , Frustração , Mães , Período Pós-Parto , Humanos , Choro/psicologia , Feminino , Masculino , Adulto , Pai/psicologia , Período Pós-Parto/psicologia , Estudos Transversais , Mães/psicologia , Lactente , Gravidez , Canadá , Adulto Jovem , Recém-Nascido
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(38): 23329-23335, 2020 09 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31611402

RESUMO

The development of biological markers of aging has primarily focused on adult samples. Epigenetic clocks are a promising tool for measuring biological age that show impressive accuracy across most tissues and age ranges. In adults, deviations from the DNA methylation (DNAm) age prediction are correlated with several age-related phenotypes, such as mortality and frailty. In children, however, fewer such associations have been made, possibly because DNAm changes are more dynamic in pediatric populations as compared to adults. To address this gap, we aimed to develop a highly accurate, noninvasive, biological measure of age specific to pediatric samples using buccal epithelial cell DNAm. We gathered 1,721 genome-wide DNAm profiles from 11 different cohorts of typically developing individuals aged 0 to 20 y old. Elastic net penalized regression was used to select 94 CpG sites from a training dataset (n = 1,032), with performance assessed in a separate test dataset (n = 689). DNAm at these 94 CpG sites was highly predictive of age in the test cohort (median absolute error = 0.35 y). The Pediatric-Buccal-Epigenetic (PedBE) clock was characterized in additional cohorts, showcasing the accuracy in longitudinal data, the performance in nonbuccal tissues and adult age ranges, and the association with obstetric outcomes. The PedBE tool for measuring biological age in children might help in understanding the environmental and contextual factors that shape the DNA methylome during child development, and how it, in turn, might relate to child health and disease.


Assuntos
Epigenômica/métodos , Células Epiteliais/metabolismo , Mucosa Bucal/citologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Estudos de Coortes , Ilhas de CpG , Epigênese Genética , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Mucosa Bucal/metabolismo , Adulto Jovem
3.
Behav Cogn Psychother ; 47(2): 129-147, 2019 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30153878

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Unwanted intrusive thoughts of intentionally harming one's infant (intrusive harm thoughts) are common distressing experiences among postpartum mothers and fathers. AIM: To understand infant crying as a stimulus for intrusive harm thoughts and associated emotional responses in prepartum and postpartum mothers and fathers in response to infant cry. METHOD: Following completion of self-report measures of negative mood and anger, prepartum (n = 48) and postpartum (n = 44) samples of mother and father pairs completed 10 minutes of listening to audio-recorded infant crying. Post-test questionnaires assessed harm thoughts, negative emotions, urges to comfort and flee, and thoughts of shaking as a soothing or coping strategy. RESULTS: One quarter of prepartum and 44% of postpartum parents reported intrusive infant-related harm thoughts following crying. Mothers and fathers did not differ in the likelihood of reporting harm thoughts, nor in the number of thoughts reported. Women reported more internalizing emotions compared with men. Hostile emotions were stronger among postpartum parents, and parents reporting harm thoughts. All parents reported strong urges to comfort the infant. Urges to flee were stronger among parents who reported harm thoughts. The likelihood of using infant shaking as a soothing or coping strategy was minimally endorsed, albeit more strongly by fathers and parents who also reported harm thoughts. CONCLUSIONS: In response to crying, harm thoughts are common and are associated with hostile emotions, urges to flee, and increased thoughts of using infant shaking. Reassuringly, the number of participants considering infant shaking as a strategy for soothing or for coping with a crying infant was low.


Assuntos
Choro , Emoções , Pai/psicologia , Mães/psicologia , Período Pós-Parto/psicologia , Pensamento , Adaptação Psicológica , Adulto , Afeto , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Autorrelato , Inquéritos e Questionários
4.
Prev Sci ; 19(6): 695-704, 2018 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28685210

RESUMO

This paper aims to estimate lifetime costs resulting from abusive head trauma (AHT) in the USA and the break-even effectiveness for prevention. A mathematical model incorporated data from Vital Statistics, the Healthcare Cost and Utilization Project Kids' Inpatient Database, and previous studies. Unit costs were derived from published sources. From society's perspective, discounted lifetime cost of an AHT averages $5.7 million (95% CI $3.2-9.2 million) for a death. It averages $2.6 million (95% CI $1.0-2.9 million) for a surviving AHT victim including $224,500 for medical care and related direct costs (2010 USD). The estimated 4824 incident AHT cases in 2010 had an estimated lifetime cost of $13.5 billion (95% CI $5.5-16.2 billion) including $257 million for medical care, $552 million for special education, $322 million for child protective services/criminal justice, $2.0 billion for lost work, and $10.3 billion for lost quality of life. Government sources paid an estimated $1.3 billion. Out-of-pocket benefits of existing prevention programming would exceed its costs if it prevents 2% of cases. When a child survives AHT, providers and caregivers can anticipate a lifetime of potentially costly and life-threatening care needs. Better effectiveness estimates are needed for both broad prevention messaging and intensive prevention targeting high-risk caregivers.


Assuntos
Maus-Tratos Infantis/economia , Traumatismos Craniocerebrais/economia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Efeitos Psicossociais da Doença , Humanos , Lactente , Modelos Estatísticos , Síndrome do Bebê Sacudido/economia
5.
Dev Psychopathol ; 29(5): 1517-1538, 2017 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29162165

RESUMO

Animal models of early postnatal mother-infant interactions have highlighted the importance of tactile contact for biobehavioral outcomes via the modification of DNA methylation (DNAm). The role of normative variation in contact in early human development has yet to be explored. In an effort to translate the animal work on tactile contact to humans, we applied a naturalistic daily diary strategy to assess the link between maternal contact with infants and epigenetic signatures in children 4-5 years later, with respect to multiple levels of child-level factors, including genetic variation and infant distress. We first investigated DNAm at four candidate genes: the glucocorticoid receptor gene, nuclear receptor subfamily 3, group C, member 1 (NR3C1), µ-opioid receptor M1 (OPRM1) and oxytocin receptor (OXTR; related to the neurobiology of social bonds), and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF; involved in postnatal plasticity). Although no candidate gene DNAm sites significantly associated with early postnatal contact, when we next examined DNAm across the genome, differentially methylated regions were identified between high and low contact groups. Using a different application of epigenomic information, we also quantified epigenetic age, and report that for infants who received low contact from caregivers, greater infant distress was associated with younger epigenetic age. These results suggested that early postnatal contact has lasting associations with child biology.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Metilação de DNA , Relações Mãe-Filho , Tato/fisiologia , Fator Neurotrófico Derivado do Encéfalo/genética , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Receptores de Glucocorticoides/genética , Receptores Opioides mu/genética , Receptores de Ocitocina/genética
6.
Arch Womens Ment Health ; 18(3): 447-55, 2015 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25377762

RESUMO

Ninety-eight mothers of healthy firstborn infants 0 to 6 months old were randomly assigned to listen to 10-min of infant crying or infant cooing while continuously rating subjective feelings of frustration. Participants completed pre-test measures of depressed mood, empathy, and trait anger and post-test measures of infant-related harm thoughts, negative and positive emotions, and urge to comfort and to flee. Twenty-three (23.5 %) participants endorsed unwanted thoughts of active harm (e.g., throwing, yelling at, shaking the infant). Women in the cry condition were more likely than women in the coo condition to report thoughts of harm. Women in the cry condition who endorsed thoughts of harm reported higher frustration levels over the 10 min of crying, higher levels of post-test negative emotions, and stronger urges to flee the infant but not stronger urges to comfort the infant. Trait anger and personal distress empathy predicted the occurrence of unwanted thoughts of infant harm, whereas negative mood did not. Unwanted, intrusive, infant-related thoughts of harm may be triggered by prolonged infant crying, are predicted by personal distress empathy and a tendency to experience anger, and are associated with higher frustration, negative emotions, and the urge to escape the infant.


Assuntos
Afeto , Choro/psicologia , Comportamento Materno/psicologia , Relações Mãe-Filho/psicologia , Mães/psicologia , Estresse Psicológico/psicologia , Adulto , Empatia/fisiologia , Feminino , Frustração , Humanos , Lactente , Comportamento do Lactente/fisiologia , Recém-Nascido , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino
7.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 109 Suppl 2: 17294-301, 2012 Oct 16.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23045677

RESUMO

Head trauma from abuse, including shaken baby syndrome, is a devastating and potentially lethal form of infant physical abuse first recognized in the early 1970s. What has been less recognized is the role of the early increase in crying in otherwise normal infants in the first few months of life as a trigger for the abuse. In part, this is because infant crying, especially prolonged unsoothable crying, has been interpreted clinically as something wrong with the infant, the infant's caregiver, or the interactions between them. Here, we review an alternative developmental interpretation, namely, that the early increase in crying is a typical behavioral development in normal infants and usually does not reflect anything wrong or abnormal. We also review evidence indicating that this normal crying pattern is the most common trigger for abusive head trauma (AHT). Together, these findings point to a conceptualization of AHT as the consequence of a failure in an otherwise common, iterative, and developmentally normal infant-caregiver interaction. They also imply that there is a window of opportunity for prevention of AHT, and potentially other forms of infant abuse, through a public health primary universal prevention strategy aimed at changing knowledge and behaviors of caregivers and society in general concerning normal development of infants and the significance of early increased infant crying. If effective, there may be important implications for prevention of infant abuse nationally and internationally.


Assuntos
Cuidadores/psicologia , Maus-Tratos Infantis/prevenção & controle , Traumatismos Craniocerebrais/prevenção & controle , Síndrome do Bebê Sacudido/prevenção & controle , Maus-Tratos Infantis/psicologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Traumatismos Craniocerebrais/psicologia , Choro/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Comportamento do Lactente/psicologia , Recém-Nascido , Relações Interpessoais , Masculino , Poder Familiar/psicologia , Síndrome do Bebê Sacudido/psicologia
8.
Pediatr Radiol ; 44 Suppl 4: S559-64, 2014 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25501727

RESUMO

The devastating and for the most part irremediable consequences for an infant, his or her family, and society in cases of abusive head trauma have spurred research into ways of preventing it. In the last four or five decades, increasing interest in infant crying and its clinical manifestation of colic has led to a reconceptualization of crying in early infancy, such that most of the characteristics of colic can be understood as manifestations of the crying typical of normal infants. This includes an early increase and then decrease in the amount of crying, the unexpected and unpredictable appearance of prolonged crying bouts, and the presence of inconsolable crying that occurs in the early months of life. When these concepts are merged with anecdotal clinical experiences, perpetrator confessions and epidemiological evidence of abusive head trauma, it is clear that these crying characteristics--and caregiver responses--are the predominant, and potentially modifiable, risk factors for abusive head trauma. This unfortunate but understandable relationship between early crying, shaking and abuse has opened windows of opportunity for primary, universal prevention efforts that are appropriate for--and support--all parents and may be able to prevent at least some of these tragic cases.


Assuntos
Maus-Tratos Infantis/prevenção & controle , Maus-Tratos Infantis/estatística & dados numéricos , Cólica/psicologia , Traumatismos Craniocerebrais/prevenção & controle , Traumatismos Craniocerebrais/psicologia , Choro/psicologia , Diagnóstico por Imagem/métodos , Causalidade , Maus-Tratos Infantis/psicologia , Cólica/epidemiologia , Cólica/terapia , Comorbidade , Traumatismos Craniocerebrais/epidemiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Incidência , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Medição de Risco
9.
BMC Pediatr ; 12: 135, 2012 Aug 29.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22928654

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Infant colic, characterised by excessive crying/fussing for no apparent cause, affects up to 20% of infants under three months of age and is a great burden to families, health professionals and the health system. One promising approach to improving its management is the use of oral probiotics. The Baby Biotics trial aims to determine whether the probiotic Lactobacillus reuteri DSM 17938 is effective in reducing crying in infants less than three months old (<13.0 weeks) with infant colic when compared to placebo. DESIGN: Double-blind, placebo-controlled randomised trial in Melbourne, Australia. PARTICIPANTS: 160 breast and formula fed infants less than three months old who present either to clinical or community services and meet Wessel's criteria of crying and/or fussing. INTERVENTION: Oral once-daily Lactobacillus reuteri (1x108 cfu) versus placebo for one month. PRIMARY OUTCOME: Infant crying/fussing time per 24 hours at one month. SECONDARY OUTCOMES: i) number of episodes of infant crying/fussing per 24 hours and ii) infant sleep duration per 24 hours (at 7, 14, 21, 28 days and 6 months); iii) maternal mental health scores, iv) family functioning scores, v) parent quality adjusted life years scores, and vi) intervention cost-effectiveness (at one and six months); and vii) infant faecal microbiota diversity, viii) infant faecal calprotectin levels and ix) Eschericia coli load (at one month only). ANALYSIS: Primary and secondary outcomes for the intervention versus control groups will be compared with t tests and non-parametric tests for continuous data and chi squared tests for dichotomous data. Regression models will be used to adjust for potential confounding factors. Intention-to-treat analysis will be applied. DISCUSSION: An effective, practical and acceptable intervention for infant colic would represent a major clinical advance. Because our trial includes breast and formula-fed babies, our results should generalise to most babies with colic. If cost-effective, the intervention's simplicity is such that it could be widely taken up as a new standard of care in the primary and secondary care sectors. TRIAL REGISTRATION: Current Controlled Trials ISRCTN95287767.


Assuntos
Cólica/dietoterapia , Probióticos/uso terapêutico , Método Duplo-Cego , Humanos , Lactente , Resultado do Tratamento
10.
J Pediatr ; 159(3): 425-430.e1-2, 2011 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21429518

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To investigate the differential association of three modes (fussing, crying, unsoothable crying) and three properties (duration/day, frequency/day, maximum bout length) of infant distress with daily caregiver frustration. STUDY DESIGN: Replicated cross-sectional studies were completed in Vancouver, British Columbia (n = 1065) and Seattle, Washington (n = 1857). Infant fussing, crying, and unsoothable crying and caregiver frustration were measured daily for 4 days at 5 weeks of age by the Baby's Day Diary. Generalized estimating equation models were used to predict caregiver frustration from nine measurements of distress. RESULTS: In Vancouver, measurements of distress significantly associated with caregiver frustration in decreasing order of magnitude were as follow: (1) maximum bout length of unsoothable crying; (2) duration/day of crying; and (3) frequency of unsoothable crying and duration/day of fussing. In Seattle, associated measurements of distress were: (1) maximum bout length of unsoothable crying; (2) maximum bout length and duration/day of crying, frequency of unsoothable crying, and duration/day of fussing; and (3) frequency/day of crying. CONCLUSIONS: Daily caregiver frustration is associated differentially with different modes and properties of infant distress. Specifically, maximum bout length of unsoothable crying was most strongly related in both sites. Additionally, frequency/day of unsoothable crying and duration/day of crying and of fussing were significantly associated at both sites.


Assuntos
Cuidadores/psicologia , Choro , Frustração , Comportamento do Lactente , Adulto , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Temperamento , Fatores de Tempo
11.
J Sleep Res ; 20(4): 598-605, 2011 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21707809

RESUMO

Reliable, valid and cost-effective methods for the assessment of infant sleep and sleep problems are of major importance. In this study, the first aim was to assess the agreement of an electronic diary as well as a paper diary with actigraphy for measuring infant sleep patterns in a community sample. The second aim was to assess the feasibility and acceptance of, and compliance with, the electronic diary and the paper diary. Ninety parents reported infant sleep behavior in a paper diary in their home environments for a total of 6 days, 95 in an electronic diary, within two consecutive weeks while actigraphic data were obtained simultaneously. We found moderate to good agreement between electronic diaries and actigraphy (r = 0.41-0.65, P < 0.01), and paper diaries and actigraphy (r = 0.47-0.70, P < 0.01). In addition, this study also found good agreement between both diaries and also between both diaries and actigraphy for sleep percentage over 24 h (electronic diaries and actigraphy: 54.1 ± 0.7%, 52.5 ± 0.7%, P < 0.05; paper diaries and actigraphy: 55.1 ± 0.5%, 52.2 ± 0.6%, P < 0.01) and for daytime (electronic diaries and actigraphy: 27.3 ± 0.9%, 23.5 ± 1.2%, P < 0.01; paper diaries and actigraphy: 27.3 ± 0.8%, 23.2 ± 1.0%, P < 0.01), with the exception that less daytime sleep was recorded on actigraphy than on either diary. In conclusion, the electronic diary and the paper diary are valid and well-accepted methods for the assessment of infant sleep. Parents preferred the electronic diary but, conversely, they were less compliant in completing it.


Assuntos
Prontuários Médicos , Pais , Sono , Actigrafia , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
12.
CMAJ ; 180(7): 727-33, 2009 Mar 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19255065

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Shaken baby syndrome often occurs after shaking in response to crying bouts. We questioned whether the use of the educational materials from the Period of PURPLE Crying program would change maternal knowledge and behaviour related to shaking. METHODS: We performed a randomized controlled trial in which 1279 mothers received materials from the Period of PURPLE Crying program or control materials during a home visit by a nurse by 2 weeks after the birth of their child. At 5 weeks, the mothers completed a diary to record their behaviour and their infants' behaviour. Two months after giving birth, the mothers completed a telephone survey to assess their knowledge and behaviour. RESULTS: The mean score (range 0-100 points) for knowledge about infant crying was greater among mothers who received the PURPLE materials (63.8 points) than among mothers who received the control materials (58.4 points) (difference 5.4 points, 95% confidence interval [CI] 4.1 to 6.5 points). The mean scores were similar for both groups for shaking knowledge and reported maternal responses to crying, inconsolable crying and self-talk responses. Compared with mothers who received control materials, mothers who received the PURPLE materials reported sharing information about walking away if frustrated more often (51.5% v. 38.5%, difference 13.0%, 95% CI 6.9% to 19.2%), the dangers of shaking (49.3% v. 36.4%, difference 12.9%, 95% CI 6.8% to 19.0%), and infant crying (67.6% v. 60.0%, difference 7.6%, 95% CI 1.7% to 13.5%). Walking away during inconsolable crying was significantly higher among mothers who received the PURPLE materials than among those who received control materials (0.067 v. 0.039 events per day, rate ratio 1.7, 95% CI 1.1 to 2.6). INTERPRETATION: The receipt of the Period of PURPLE Crying materials led to higher maternal scores for knowledge about infant crying and for some behaviours considered to be important for the prevention of shaking.


Assuntos
Choro , Comportamento Materno , Síndrome do Bebê Sacudido/prevenção & controle , Materiais de Ensino/provisão & distribuição , Adulto , Intervalos de Confiança , Feminino , Seguimentos , Educação em Saúde/organização & administração , Conhecimentos, Atitudes e Prática em Saúde , Humanos , Incidência , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Educação de Pacientes como Assunto , Probabilidade , Desenvolvimento de Programas , Avaliação de Programas e Projetos de Saúde , Valores de Referência , Fatores de Risco , Síndrome do Bebê Sacudido/epidemiologia
13.
Phys Occup Ther Pediatr ; 29(3): 222-35, 2009.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19842852

RESUMO

Concurrent with recommendations to place infants to sleep in supine, there has been a dramatic increase in the number of infants with positional plagiocephaly (PP). Recent evidence suggests that infants who have decreased exposure to prone position may have a higher incidence of PP and may be at risk for a delay in the acquisition of certain motor skills. The purpose of this study was to compare motor development between infants with PP and matched peers without PP. We also examined differences in infant positioning practices when asleep and awake between the two groups. Twenty-seven infants with PP, 3 to 8 months of age, were matched by age, gender, and race to infants without PP. Motor performance was evaluated using the Alberta Infant Motor Scale (AIMS) and the Peabody Developmental Motor Scales (PDMS). Parents completed a diary that recorded infant positioning over a 3-day period. Mean AIMS percentile score for infants with PP was 31.1 +/- 21.6 as compared with 42.7 +/- 20.2 in infants without PP (p = .06). Better performance on the AIMS was positively correlated with the amount of time in prone position when awake, for both groups of children (PP r = .52, no PP r = .44, p < .05). Therapists should be aware of a risk of a motor delay when evaluating infants with PP. It is also important for parents to be informed about the importance of supervised prone playtime to enhance the development of early motor skills.


Assuntos
Cuidado do Lactente/métodos , Atividade Motora , Transtornos das Habilidades Motoras/etiologia , Transtornos das Habilidades Motoras/fisiopatologia , Destreza Motora , Plagiocefalia não Sinostótica/complicações , Postura , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Feminino , Humanos , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Transtornos das Habilidades Motoras/prevenção & controle , Plagiocefalia não Sinostótica/prevenção & controle , Decúbito Ventral , Valores de Referência , Decúbito Dorsal
14.
Infant Behav Dev ; 56: 101246, 2019 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29605229

RESUMO

PURPOSE: To investigate the observation that perpetrators of abusive head trauma engage in repeated shakings because shaking "works" to quiet the infant. METHODS: Sixty first-time parent couples individually cared for a programmable model infant in two consecutive 7-min trials. After six minutes of consolable followed by inconsolable crying, parents selected one of three soothing techniques. For trial one, parents were randomized to a "Successful" or "Failed" Soothing Condition. Whether the soothing technique was repeated after trial two was determined by the study investigators. Parents rated their frustration after each trial. RESULTS: As hypothesized, parents were more likely to repeat a soothing technique that "worked" in trial one. Compared to fathers, mothers reported more frustration when soothing failed. CONCLUSIONS: That caregivers were more likely to repeat a successful soothing technique converges with perpetrator confessions that crying cessation after shaking may be a reason why shaking is used repeatedly in response to crying.


Assuntos
Maus-Tratos Infantis/psicologia , Choro/psicologia , Pais/psicologia , Síndrome do Bebê Sacudido/psicologia , Adulto , Cuidadores/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Masculino
15.
Epigenetics Chromatin ; 12(1): 1, 2019 01 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30602389

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The widespread use of accessible peripheral tissues for epigenetic analyses has prompted increasing interest in the study of tissue-specific DNA methylation (DNAm) variation in human populations. To date, characterizations of inter-individual DNAm variability and DNAm concordance across tissues have been largely performed in adult tissues and therefore are limited in their relevance to DNAm profiles from pediatric samples. Given that DNAm patterns in early life undergo rapid changes and have been linked to a wide range of health outcomes and environmental exposures, direct investigations of tissue-specific DNAm variation in pediatric samples may help inform the design and interpretation of DNAm analyses from early life cohorts. In this study, we present a systematic comparison of genome-wide DNAm patterns between matched pediatric buccal epithelial cells (BECs) and peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs), two of the most widely used peripheral tissues in human epigenetic studies. Specifically, we assessed DNAm variability, cross-tissue DNAm concordance and genetic determinants of DNAm across two independent early life cohorts encompassing different ages. RESULTS: BECs had greater inter-individual DNAm variability compared to PBMCs and highly the variable CpGs are more likely to be positively correlated between the matched tissues compared to less variable CpGs. These sites were enriched for CpGs under genetic influence, suggesting that a substantial proportion of DNAm covariation between tissues can be attributed to genetic variation. Finally, we demonstrated the relevance of our findings to human epigenetic studies by categorizing CpGs from published DNAm association studies of pediatric BECs and peripheral blood. CONCLUSIONS: Taken together, our results highlight a number of important considerations and practical implications in the design and interpretation of EWAS analyses performed in pediatric peripheral tissues.


Assuntos
Metilação de DNA , Epigenômica/normas , Variação Genética , Estudo de Associação Genômica Ampla/normas , Adolescente , Fatores Etários , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Monócitos/metabolismo , Mucosa Bucal/metabolismo
16.
Am J Prev Med ; 34(4 Suppl): S163-8, 2008 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18374269

RESUMO

A conference addressing how to establish the incidence of inflicted traumatic brain injury in young children provided the opportunity to examine issues of definitions, passive versus active surveillance, study designs, proxy measures, and statistical issues. Data were presented that had been collected in alternative ways. The participants concluded that an ideal system for measurement of the incidence does not yet exist. A new surveillance system will take a significant amount of time and money to establish. Such a system will require a combination of ascertainment approaches, attention to case finding, a large population, careful attention to coding and data quality. The ethical issues involved in measuring stigmatized and illegal behavior are not inconsequential. In an ideal system data from different sources-medical, legal, and social service, among others-will need to be linked. Perhaps most importantly, any surveillance approach will need to be maintained so that trend data can be used to assess the effectiveness of prevention efforts.


Assuntos
Lesões Encefálicas/epidemiologia , Maus-Tratos Infantis/diagnóstico , Vigilância da População/métodos , Lesões Encefálicas/diagnóstico , Lesões Encefálicas/etiologia , Pré-Escolar , Consenso , Ética em Pesquisa , Humanos , Incidência
17.
Child Abuse Negl ; 84: 106-114, 2018 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30077049

RESUMO

Low incidence rates and economic recession have hampered interpretation of educational prevention efforts to reduce abusive head trauma (AHT). Our objective was to determine whether the British Columbia experience implementing a province-wide prevention program reduced AHT hospitalization rates. A 3-dose primary, universal education program (the Period of PURPLE Crying) was implemented through maternal and public health units and assessed by retrospective-prospective surveillance. With parents of all newborn infants born between January 2009 and December 2016 (n = 354,477), nurses discussed crying and shaking while delivering a booklet and DVD during maternity admission (dose 1). Public health nurses reinforced Talking Points by telephone and/or home visits post-discharge (dose 2) and community education was instituted annually (dose 3). During admission, program delivery occurred for 90% of mothers. Fathers were present 74.4% of the time. By 2-4 months, 70.9% of mothers and 50.5% of fathers watched the DVD and/or read the booklet. AHT admissions decreased for <12-month-olds from 10.6 (95% CI: 8.3-13.5) to 7.1 (95% CI: 4.8-10.5) or, for <24-month-olds, from 6.7 (95% CI: 5.4-8.3) to 4.4 (95% CI: 3.1-6.2) cases per 100,000 person-years. Relative risk of admission was 0.67 (95% CI: 0.42-1.07, P = 0.090) and 0.65 (95% CI: 0.43-0.99, P = 0.048) respectively. We conclude that the intervention was associated with a 35% reduction in infant AHT admissions that was significant for <24-month-olds. The results are encouraging that, despite a low initial incidence and economic recession, reductions in AHT may be achievable with a system-wide implementation of a comprehensive parental education prevention program.


Assuntos
Maus-Tratos Infantis/prevenção & controle , Traumatismos Craniocerebrais/prevenção & controle , Colúmbia Britânica/epidemiologia , Cuidadores/educação , Maus-Tratos Infantis/estatística & dados numéricos , Traumatismos Craniocerebrais/epidemiologia , Choro , Pai/educação , Feminino , Educação em Saúde/métodos , Hospitalização/estatística & dados numéricos , Visita Domiciliar/estatística & dados numéricos , Humanos , Incidência , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Mães/educação , Estudos Prospectivos , Estudos Retrospectivos , Síndrome do Bebê Sacudido/epidemiologia , Síndrome do Bebê Sacudido/prevenção & controle
18.
Physiol Behav ; 193(Pt A): 43-54, 2018 09 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29730041

RESUMO

Crying is the principal means by which newborn infants shape parental behavior to meet their needs. While this mechanism can be highly effective, infant crying can also be an aversive stimulus that leads to parental frustration and even abuse. Fathers have recently become more involved in direct caregiving activities in modern, developed nations, and fathers are more likely than mothers to physically abuse infants. In this study, we attempt to explain variation in the neural response to infant crying among human fathers, with the hope of identifying factors that are associated with a more or less sensitive response. We imaged brain function in 39 first-time fathers of newborn infants as they listened to both their own and a standardized unknown infant cry stimulus, as well as auditory control stimuli, and evaluated whether these neural responses were correlated with measured characteristics of fathers and infants that were hypothesized to modulate these responses. Fathers also provided subjective ratings of each cry stimulus on multiple dimensions. Fathers showed widespread activation to both own and unknown infant cries in neural systems involved in empathy and approach motivation. There was no significant difference in the neural response to the own vs. unknown infant cry, and many fathers were unable to distinguish between the two cries. Comparison of these results with previous studies in mothers revealed a high degree of similarity between first-time fathers and first-time mothers in the pattern of neural activation to newborn infant cries. Further comparisons suggested that younger infant age was associated with stronger paternal neural responses, perhaps due to hormonal or novelty effects. In our sample, older fathers found infant cries less aversive and had an attenuated response to infant crying in both the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and the anterior insula, suggesting that compared with younger fathers, older fathers may be better able to avoid the distress associated with empathic over-arousal in response to infant cries. A principal components analysis revealed that fathers with more negative emotional reactions to the unknown infant cry showed decreased activation in the thalamus and caudate nucleus, regions expected to promote positive parental behaviors, as well as increased activation in the hypothalamus and dorsal ACC, again suggesting that empathic over-arousal might result in negative emotional reactions to infant crying. In sum, our findings suggest that infant age, paternal age and paternal emotional reactions to infant crying all modulate the neural response of fathers to infant crying. By identifying neural correlates of variation in paternal subjective reactions to infant crying, these findings help lay the groundwork for evaluating the effectiveness of interventions designed to increase paternal sensitivity and compassion.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Choro , Relações Pais-Filho , Comportamento Paterno/fisiologia , Adulto , Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Emoções/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Vias Neurais/diagnóstico por imagem , Vias Neurais/fisiologia , Comportamento Paterno/psicologia , Reconhecimento Fisiológico de Modelo/fisiologia , Percepção Social , Testosterona/metabolismo , Adulto Jovem
19.
J Dev Behav Pediatr ; 28(4): 288-93, 2007 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17700080

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: : This study aims to determine whether the age-specific incidences (1) of publicly reported cases of shaken baby syndrome (SBS) and (2) of publicly reported cases of SBS with crying as the stimulus have similar properties to the previously reported normal crying curve. METHODS: : The study reports cases of SBS by age of the child at the time of the inflicted trauma from the data set of the National Center on Shaken Baby Syndrome using cases entered between January 1, 2003 and August 31, 2004. RESULTS: : There were 591 cases of infants up to 1.5 years of age who had been reported to have been shaken or shaken and physically abused. Of these, crying was reported as the stimulus in 166 cases. In both samples, the curves of age-specific incidence started at 2-3 weeks, reached a clear peak at about 9-12 weeks of age, and declined to lower more stable levels by about 29-32 weeks of age, similar to the normal crying curve. These curves have similar onsets and shapes and a slightly later peak compared to the normal crying curve. CONCLUSIONS: : The findings provide convergent indirect evidence that crying, especially in the first 4 months of age, is an important stimulus for SBS.


Assuntos
Choro , Acontecimentos que Mudam a Vida , Síndrome do Bebê Sacudido/epidemiologia , Fatores Etários , Maus-Tratos Infantis/prevenção & controle , Maus-Tratos Infantis/estatística & dados numéricos , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Incidência , Lactente , Estados Unidos/epidemiologia
20.
Child Abuse Negl ; 30(1): 7-16, 2006 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16406023

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To determine whether there is an age-specific incidence of hospitalized cases of Shaken Baby Syndrome (SBS) that has similar properties to the previously reported "normal crying curve," as a form of indirect evidence that crying is an important stimulus for SBS. DESIGN AND SETTING: The study analyzed cases of Shaken Baby Syndrome by age at hospitalization from hospital discharge data for California hospitals from October 1996 through December 2000. PATIENTS: All cases of children less than 18 months (78 weeks) of age for whom the diagnostic code for Shaken Baby Syndrome (995.55) in the International Classification of Disease, Ninth Edition, Clinical Modification was assigned. RESULTS: There were 273 hospitalizations for SBS. Like the "normal crying curve," the curve of age-specific incidence starts at 2-3 weeks, has a clear peak, and declines to baseline by about 36 weeks of age. In contrast to the normal crying curve that peaks at 5-6 weeks, the peak of SBS hospitalizations occurs at 10-13 weeks. CONCLUSIONS: The age-specific incidence curve of hospitalized SBS cases has a similar starting point and shape to the previously reported normal crying curve but the peak occurs about 4-6 weeks later. Of the likely predisposing causes, this pattern is only consistent with the properties of early crying. There are numerous explanations for the lag in the peaks between crying and SBS hospitalizations, including the possibility of repeat shakings prior to hospitalization. The importance of crying as a stimulus to SBS may provide an opportunity for preventive intervention.


Assuntos
Choro , Hospitalização/estatística & dados numéricos , Síndrome do Bebê Sacudido/epidemiologia , Fatores Etários , California , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Humanos , Incidência , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Recidiva , Fatores de Risco , Síndrome do Bebê Sacudido/etiologia , Síndrome do Bebê Sacudido/prevenção & controle
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA