Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 8 de 8
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
Tipo de documento
País de afiliação
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 118(19)2021 05 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33952702

RESUMO

A degraded, black-and-white image of an object, which appears meaningless on first presentation, is easily identified after a single exposure to the original, intact image. This striking example of perceptual learning reflects a rapid (one-trial) change in performance, but the kind of learning that is involved is not known. We asked whether this learning depends on conscious (hippocampus-dependent) memory for the images that have been presented or on an unconscious (hippocampus-independent) change in the perception of images, independently of the ability to remember them. We tested five memory-impaired patients with hippocampal lesions or larger medial temporal lobe (MTL) lesions. In comparison to volunteers, the patients were fully intact at perceptual learning, and their improvement persisted without decrement from 1 d to more than 5 mo. Yet, the patients were impaired at remembering the test format and, even after 1 d, were impaired at remembering the images themselves. To compare perceptual learning and remembering directly, at 7 d after seeing degraded images and their solutions, patients and volunteers took either a naming test or a recognition memory test with these images. The patients improved as much as the volunteers at identifying the degraded images but were severely impaired at remembering them. Notably, the patient with the most severe memory impairment and the largest MTL lesions performed worse than the other patients on the memory tests but was the best at perceptual learning. The findings show that one-trial, long-lasting perceptual learning relies on hippocampus-independent (nondeclarative) memory, independent of any requirement to consciously remember.


Assuntos
Estado de Consciência/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Amnésia/fisiopatologia , Feminino , Hipocampo/fisiopatologia , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/fisiopatologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estimulação Luminosa , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Lobo Temporal/fisiopatologia
2.
Int J Aging Hum Dev ; : 914150241253257, 2024 May 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38751054

RESUMO

We examined how symptoms across the mood spectrum relate to Alzheimer's disease (AD) biomarkers in older women at high risk for AD. Participants included 25 women aged 65+ with mild cognitive deficits and elevated AD genetic risk. The Profile of Mood States Questionnaire measured mood symptoms and a total mood disturbance (TMD) score. Tau burden in the meta-temporal region of interest was measured using MK-6240 Tau positron emission tomography (PET) imaging. A subset (n = 12) also had p-Tau181, and Aß40/42 levels measured in plasma. Higher TMD scores related to higher tau PET standardized uptake value ratio (SUVR). Greater negative mood symptoms correlated with higher tau PET SUVR, while greater vigor correlated with lower SUVR. Similar results were seen with plasma p-Tau181 levels, but not with Aß40/42 levels. In conclusion, positive and negative mood symptoms related to tau pathology in older women at high risk for AD, highlighting a role of mental well-being in AD risk.

3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 116(10): 4619-4624, 2019 03 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30792351

RESUMO

We studied the narrative recollections of memory-impaired patients with medial temporal lobe (MTL) damage who took a 25-min guided walk during which 11 planned events occurred. The recollections of the patients, recorded directly after the walk, were compared with the recollections of controls tested directly after the walk (C1), after one month (C2), or after 2.6 years (C3). With respect to memory for the walk, the narrative recollections of the patients were impoverished compared with C1 but resembled the recollections of volunteers tested after long delays (C2 and C3). In addition, how language was used by the patients in their recollections resembled how language was used by groups C2 and C3 (higher-frequency words, less concrete words, fewer nouns, more adverbs, more pronouns, and more indefinite articles). These findings appear to reflect how individuals, either memory-impaired patients or controls, typically speak about the past when memory is weak and lacks detail and need not have special implications about language use and MTL function beyond the domain of memory. A notable exception to the similarity between patient narratives and the narratives of C2 and C3 was that the control groups reported the events of the walk in correct chronological order, whereas the order in which patients reported events bore no relationship to the order in which events occurred. We suggest that the MTL is especially important for accessing global information about events and the relationships among their elements.


Assuntos
Transtornos da Memória/fisiopatologia , Rememoração Mental , Lobo Temporal/lesões , Lobo Temporal/fisiopatologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
4.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 30(9): 1323-1329, 2018 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29791297

RESUMO

It is difficult to predict whether newly learned information will be retrievable in the future. A biomarker of long-lasting learning, capable of predicting an individual's future ability to retrieve a particular memory, could positively influence teaching and educational methods. ERPs were investigated as a potential biomarker of long-lasting learning. Prior ERP studies have supported a dual-process model of recognition memory that categorizes recollection and familiarity as distinct memorial processes with distinct ERP correlates. The late positive component is thought to underlie conscious recollection and the frontal N400 signal is thought to reflect familiarity [Yonelinas, A. P. Components of episodic memory: The contribution of recollection and familiarity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, Biological Sciences, 356, 1363-1374, 2001]. Here we show that the magnitude of the late positive component, soon after initial learning, is predictive of subsequent recollection of anatomical terms among medical students 6 months later.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Adulto , Anatomia , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estudantes de Medicina , Terminologia como Assunto , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
5.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 41(2): 204-218, 2019 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30179518

RESUMO

Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a progressive neurodegenerative disorder that presents predominantly with impairments in learning and memory. Patients with AD are also susceptible to false memories, a clinically relevant memory distortion where a patient remembers an incorrect memory that they believe to be true. The use of cognitive strategies to improve memory performance among patients with AD by reducing false memories has taken on added importance given the lack of disease-modifying agents for AD. However, existing evidence suggests that cognitive strategies to reduce false memories in patients with AD are of limited effectiveness, although these strategies may be useful at earlier stages of the disease. The purpose of this review is to examine experimental findings of false memories and associated memory processes in patients with mild cognitive impairment due to AD and mild AD dementia. Cognitive strategies to reduce false memories in these patient populations are also reviewed. Approaches to clinically relevant future research are suggested and discussed.


Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Disfunção Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Remediação Cognitiva , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Repressão Psicológica , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Doença de Alzheimer/fisiopatologia , Doença de Alzheimer/psicologia , Doença de Alzheimer/terapia , Cognição/fisiologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/fisiopatologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/psicologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/terapia , Emoções/fisiologia , Feminino , Lobo Frontal/fisiopatologia , Humanos , Masculino , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Resultado do Tratamento
6.
Geroscience ; 39(2): 199-220, 2017 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28238188

RESUMO

While cognitive decline is observed in the normal aging monkey, neurons are not lost with age. Instead, frontal white matter is lost as myelin degenerates and both correlate with age-related cognitive decline. As age-related myelin damage increases, there should be an increase in clearance of damaged myelin by microglial phagocytosis. In this study, brains of behaviorally tested rhesus monkeys were assessed using unbiased stereology to quantify the density of activated microglia (LN3 antibody positive) and phagocytic microglia (galectin-3 (Gal-3) antibody positive) in three white matter regions: the corpus callosum, cingulum bundle (CGB), and frontal white matter (FWM). LN3 cell density was significantly increased in the CGB, whereas Gal-3 cell density was significantly increased in all regions. Increases in Gal-3 cell density in the FWM were associated with cognitive impairment. In the FWM of old animals, Gal-3-positive microglia were classified by morphological subtype as ramified, hypertrophic, or amoeboid. The densities of hypertrophic and amoeboid microglia significantly correlated with cognitive impairment. Finally, microglia were double-labeled with LN3 and Gal-3 showing that 91% of Gal-3 cells were also LN3 positive, thus expressing an "activated" phenotype. Furthermore, 15% of all double-labeled cells formed phagocytic cups. Overall, these results suggest that microglia become activated in white matter with age where the majority express a phagocytic phenotype. We hypothesize that age-related phagocytic activation of microglia is a response to accumulating myelin pathology. The association of Gal-3 in the FWM with cognitive impairment may reflect regional differences in damage or dysfunction of normal clearance mechanisms.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/patologia , Encéfalo/patologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/patologia , Microglia/fisiologia , Fagocitose/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/metabolismo , Animais , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Disfunção Cognitiva/etiologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/metabolismo , Feminino , Galectina 3/metabolismo , Macaca mulatta , Masculino
7.
J Histochem Cytochem ; 65(3): 153-171, 2017 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28080173

RESUMO

Storage of tissue sections for long periods allows multiple samples, acquired over months or years, to be processed together, in the same reagents, for quantitative histochemical studies. Protocols for freezer storage of free-floating frozen sections using sucrose with different additives have been reported and assert that storage has no effect on histochemistry, but no quantitative support has been provided. The present study analyzed the efficacy of long-term storage of brain tissue sections at -80C in buffered 15% glycerol. To determine whether histochemical reactivity is affected, we analyzed 11 datasets from 80 monkey brains that had sections stored for up to 10 years. For processing, sections from multiple cases were removed from storage, thawed, and batch-processed at the same time for different histochemical measures, including IHC for neuronal nuclear antigen, parvalbumin, orexin-A, doublecortin, bromodeoxyuridine, the pro-form of brain-derived neurotrophic factor, and damaged myelin basic protein as well as a histochemical assay for hyaluronic acid. Results were quantified using stereology, optical densitometry, fluorescence intensity, or percent area stained. Multiple regression analyses controlling for age and sex demonstrated the general stability of these antigens for up to a decade when stored in 15% glycerol at -80C.


Assuntos
Química Encefálica , Secções Congeladas/métodos , Animais , Antígenos Nucleares/análise , Fator Neurotrófico Derivado do Encéfalo/análise , Bromodesoxiuridina/análise , Contagem de Células , Criopreservação/métodos , Proteínas do Domínio Duplacortina , Feminino , Ácido Hialurônico/análise , Imuno-Histoquímica/métodos , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Proteínas Associadas aos Microtúbulos/análise , Proteína Básica da Mielina/análise , Proteínas do Tecido Nervoso/análise , Neuropeptídeos/análise , Orexinas/análise , Parvalbuminas/análise
8.
Front Syst Neurosci ; 9: 102, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26236203

RESUMO

The generation of new neurons in the adult mammalian brain is well-established for the hippocampal dentate gyrus (DG). However, the role of neurogenesis in hippocampal function and cognition, how it changes in aging, and the mechanisms underlying this are yet to be elucidated in the monkey brain. To address this, we investigated adult neurogenesis in the DG of 42 rhesus monkeys (39 cognitively tested) ranging in age from young adult to the elderly. We report here that there is an age-related decline in proliferation and a delayed development of adult neuronal phenotype. Additionally, we show that many of the new neurons survive throughout the lifetime of the animal and may contribute to a modest increase in total neuron number in the granule cell layer of the DG over the adult life span. Lastly, we find that measures of decreased adult neurogenesis are only modestly predictive of age-related cognitive impairment.

SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA