Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 22
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
Tipo de documento
País de afiliação
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Cell ; 186(1): 178-193.e15, 2023 01 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36608653

RESUMO

The hypothalamus regulates innate social behaviors, including mating and aggression. These behaviors can be evoked by optogenetic stimulation of specific neuronal subpopulations within MPOA and VMHvl, respectively. Here, we perform dynamical systems modeling of population neuronal activity in these nuclei during social behaviors. In VMHvl, unsupervised analysis identified a dominant dimension of neural activity with a large time constant (>50 s), generating an approximate line attractor in neural state space. Progression of the neural trajectory along this attractor was correlated with an escalation of agonistic behavior, suggesting that it may encode a scalable state of aggressiveness. Consistent with this, individual differences in the magnitude of the integration dimension time constant were strongly correlated with differences in aggressiveness. In contrast, approximate line attractors were not observed in MPOA during mating; instead, neurons with fast dynamics were tuned to specific actions. Thus, different hypothalamic nuclei employ distinct neural population codes to represent similar social behaviors.


Assuntos
Comportamento Sexual Animal , Núcleo Hipotalâmico Ventromedial , Animais , Comportamento Sexual Animal/fisiologia , Núcleo Hipotalâmico Ventromedial/fisiologia , Hipotálamo/fisiologia , Agressão/fisiologia , Comportamento Social
2.
Cell ; 174(1): 44-58.e17, 2018 06 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29779950

RESUMO

Many naturalistic behaviors are built from modular components that are expressed sequentially. Although striatal circuits have been implicated in action selection and implementation, the neural mechanisms that compose behavior in unrestrained animals are not well understood. Here, we record bulk and cellular neural activity in the direct and indirect pathways of dorsolateral striatum (DLS) as mice spontaneously express action sequences. These experiments reveal that DLS neurons systematically encode information about the identity and ordering of sub-second 3D behavioral motifs; this encoding is facilitated by fast-timescale decorrelations between the direct and indirect pathways. Furthermore, lesioning the DLS prevents appropriate sequence assembly during exploratory or odor-evoked behaviors. By characterizing naturalistic behavior at neural timescales, these experiments identify a code for elemental 3D pose dynamics built from complementary pathway dynamics, support a role for DLS in constructing meaningful behavioral sequences, and suggest models for how actions are sculpted over time.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Corpo Estriado/metabolismo , Animais , Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Cálcio/metabolismo , Corpo Estriado/efeitos dos fármacos , Eletrodos Implantados , Feminino , Masculino , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Camundongos Transgênicos , N-Metilaspartato/farmacologia , Neurônios/efeitos dos fármacos , Neurônios/fisiologia , Fotometria , Receptores de Dopamina D1/deficiência , Receptores de Dopamina D1/genética
3.
Nature ; 614(7946): 108-117, 2023 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36653449

RESUMO

Spontaneous animal behaviour is built from action modules that are concatenated by the brain into sequences1,2. However, the neural mechanisms that guide the composition of naturalistic, self-motivated behaviour remain unknown. Here we show that dopamine systematically fluctuates in the dorsolateral striatum (DLS) as mice spontaneously express sub-second behavioural modules, despite the absence of task structure, sensory cues or exogenous reward. Photometric recordings and calibrated closed-loop optogenetic manipulations during open field behaviour demonstrate that DLS dopamine fluctuations increase sequence variation over seconds, reinforce the use of associated behavioural modules over minutes, and modulate the vigour with which modules are expressed, without directly influencing movement initiation or moment-to-moment kinematics. Although the reinforcing effects of optogenetic DLS dopamine manipulations vary across behavioural modules and individual mice, these differences are well predicted by observed variation in the relationships between endogenous dopamine and module use. Consistent with the possibility that DLS dopamine fluctuations act as a teaching signal, mice build sequences during exploration as if to maximize dopamine. Together, these findings suggest a model in which the same circuits and computations that govern action choices in structured tasks have a key role in sculpting the content of unconstrained, high-dimensional, spontaneous behaviour.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Reforço Psicológico , Recompensa , Animais , Camundongos , Corpo Estriado/metabolismo , Dopamina/metabolismo , Sinais (Psicologia) , Optogenética , Fotometria
4.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 119(15): e2113961119, 2022 04 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35385355

RESUMO

In probabilistic and nonstationary environments, individuals must use internal and external cues to flexibly make decisions that lead to desirable outcomes. To gain insight into the process by which animals choose between actions, we trained mice in a task with time-varying reward probabilities. In our implementation of such a two-armed bandit task, thirsty mice use information about recent action and action­outcome histories to choose between two ports that deliver water probabilistically. Here we comprehensively modeled choice behavior in this task, including the trial-to-trial changes in port selection, i.e., action switching behavior. We find that mouse behavior is, at times, deterministic and, at others, apparently stochastic. The behavior deviates from that of a theoretically optimal agent performing Bayesian inference in a hidden Markov model (HMM). We formulate a set of models based on logistic regression, reinforcement learning, and sticky Bayesian inference that we demonstrate are mathematically equivalent and that accurately describe mouse behavior. The switching behavior of mice in the task is captured in each model by a stochastic action policy, a history-dependent representation of action value, and a tendency to repeat actions despite incoming evidence. The models parsimoniously capture behavior across different environmental conditionals by varying the stickiness parameter, and like the mice, they achieve nearly maximal reward rates. These results indicate that mouse behavior reaches near-maximal performance with reduced action switching and can be described by a set of equivalent models with a small number of relatively fixed parameters.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Tomada de Decisões , Camundongos , Animais , Camundongos/psicologia , Recompensa , Incerteza
5.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 19(9): e1011067, 2023 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37695776

RESUMO

To behave adaptively, animals must learn to predict future reward, or value. To do this, animals are thought to learn reward predictions using reinforcement learning. However, in contrast to classical models, animals must learn to estimate value using only incomplete state information. Previous work suggests that animals estimate value in partially observable tasks by first forming "beliefs"-optimal Bayesian estimates of the hidden states in the task. Although this is one way to solve the problem of partial observability, it is not the only way, nor is it the most computationally scalable solution in complex, real-world environments. Here we show that a recurrent neural network (RNN) can learn to estimate value directly from observations, generating reward prediction errors that resemble those observed experimentally, without any explicit objective of estimating beliefs. We integrate statistical, functional, and dynamical systems perspectives on beliefs to show that the RNN's learned representation encodes belief information, but only when the RNN's capacity is sufficiently large. These results illustrate how animals can estimate value in tasks without explicitly estimating beliefs, yielding a representation useful for systems with limited capacity.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem , Reforço Psicológico , Animais , Teorema de Bayes , Recompensa , Redes Neurais de Computação
6.
Biostatistics ; 23(2): 643-665, 2022 04 13.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33417699

RESUMO

Personalized cancer treatments based on the molecular profile of a patient's tumor are an emerging and exciting class of treatments in oncology. As genomic tumor profiling is becoming more common, targeted treatments for specific molecular alterations are gaining traction. To discover new potential therapeutics that may apply to broad classes of tumors matching some molecular pattern, experimentalists and pharmacologists rely on high-throughput, in vitro screens of many compounds against many different cell lines. We propose a hierarchical Bayesian model of how cancer cell lines respond to drugs in these experiments and develop a method for fitting the model to real-world high-throughput screening data. Through a case study, the model is shown to capture nontrivial associations between molecular features and drug response, such as requiring both wild type TP53 and overexpression of MDM2 to be sensitive to Nutlin-3(a). In quantitative benchmarks, the model outperforms a standard approach in biology, with $\approx20\%$ lower predictive error on held out data. When combined with a conditional randomization testing procedure, the model discovers markers of therapeutic response that recapitulate known biology and suggest new avenues for investigation. All code for the article is publicly available at https://github.com/tansey/deep-dose-response.


Assuntos
Antineoplásicos , Neoplasias , Antineoplásicos/farmacologia , Teorema de Bayes , Avaliação Pré-Clínica de Medicamentos/métodos , Detecção Precoce de Câncer , Ensaios de Triagem em Larga Escala , Humanos , Neoplasias/tratamento farmacológico , Neoplasias/genética
7.
bioRxiv ; 2024 May 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38826298

RESUMO

Line attractors are emergent population dynamics hypothesized to encode continuous variables such as head direction and internal states. In mammals, direct evidence of neural implementation of a line attractor has been hindered by the challenge of targeting perturbations to specific neurons within contributing ensembles. Estrogen receptor type 1 (Esr1)-expressing neurons in the ventrolateral subdivision of the ventromedial hypothalamus (VMHvl) show line attractor dynamics in male mice during fighting. We hypothesized that these dynamics may encode continuous variation in the intensity of an internal aggressive state. Here, we report that these neurons also show line attractor dynamics in head-fixed mice observing aggression. We exploit this finding to identify and perturb line attractor-contributing neurons using 2-photon calcium imaging and holographic optogenetic perturbations. On-manifold perturbations demonstrate that integration and persistent activity are intrinsic properties of these neurons which drive the system along the line attractor, while transient off-manifold perturbations reveal rapid relaxation back into the attractor. Furthermore, stimulation and imaging reveal selective functional connectivity among attractor-contributing neurons. Intriguingly, individual differences among mice in line attractor stability were correlated with the degree of functional connectivity among contributing neurons. Mechanistic modelling indicates that dense subnetwork connectivity and slow neurotransmission are required to explain our empirical findings. Our work bridges circuit and manifold paradigms, shedding light on the intrinsic and operational dynamics of a behaviorally relevant mammalian line attractor.

8.
bioRxiv ; 2023 May 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37292695

RESUMO

Cyclic changes in hormonal state are well-known to regulate mating behavior during the female reproductive cycle, but whether and how these changes affect the dynamics of neural activity in the female brain is largely unknown. The ventromedial hypothalamus, ventro-lateral subdivision (VMHvl) contains a subpopulation of VMHvl Esr1+,Npy2r- neurons that controls female sexual receptivity. Longitudinal single cell calcium imaging of these neurons across the estrus cycle revealed that overlapping but distinct subpopulations were active during proestrus (mating-accepting) vs. non-proestrus (rejecting) phases. Dynamical systems analysis of imaging data from proestrus females uncovered a dimension with slow ramping activity, which generated approximate line attractor-like dynamics in neural state space. During mating, the neural population vector progressed along this attractor as male mounting and intromission proceeded. Attractor-like dynamics disappeared in non-proestrus states and reappeared following re-entry into proestrus. They were also absent in ovariectomized females but were restored by hormone priming. These observations reveal that hypothalamic line attractor-like dynamics are associated with female sexual receptivity and can be reversibly regulated by sex hormones, demonstrating that attractor dynamics can be flexibly modulated by physiological state. They also suggest a potential mechanism for the neural encoding of female sexual arousal.

9.
bioRxiv ; 2023 Apr 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37066383

RESUMO

To behave adaptively, animals must learn to predict future reward, or value. To do this, animals are thought to learn reward predictions using reinforcement learning. However, in contrast to classical models, animals must learn to estimate value using only incomplete state information. Previous work suggests that animals estimate value in partially observable tasks by first forming "beliefs"-optimal Bayesian estimates of the hidden states in the task. Although this is one way to solve the problem of partial observability, it is not the only way, nor is it the most computationally scalable solution in complex, real-world environments. Here we show that a recurrent neural network (RNN) can learn to estimate value directly from observations, generating reward prediction errors that resemble those observed experimentally, without any explicit objective of estimating beliefs. We integrate statistical, functional, and dynamical systems perspectives on beliefs to show that the RNN's learned representation encodes belief information, but only when the RNN's capacity is sufficiently large. These results illustrate how animals can estimate value in tasks without explicitly estimating beliefs, yielding a representation useful for systems with limited capacity. Author Summary: Natural environments are full of uncertainty. For example, just because my fridge had food in it yesterday does not mean it will have food today. Despite such uncertainty, animals can estimate which states and actions are the most valuable. Previous work suggests that animals estimate value using a brain area called the basal ganglia, using a process resembling a reinforcement learning algorithm called TD learning. However, traditional reinforcement learning algorithms cannot accurately estimate value in environments with state uncertainty (e.g., when my fridge's contents are unknown). One way around this problem is if agents form "beliefs," a probabilistic estimate of how likely each state is, given any observations so far. However, estimating beliefs is a demanding process that may not be possible for animals in more complex environments. Here we show that an artificial recurrent neural network (RNN) trained with TD learning can estimate value from observations, without explicitly estimating beliefs. The trained RNN's error signals resembled the neural activity of dopamine neurons measured during the same task. Importantly, the RNN's activity resembled beliefs, but only when the RNN had enough capacity. This work illustrates how animals could estimate value in uncertain environments without needing to first form beliefs, which may be useful in environments where computing the true beliefs is too costly.

10.
bioRxiv ; 2023 Sep 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37732217

RESUMO

The ability to make advantageous decisions is critical for animals to ensure their survival. Patch foraging is a natural decision-making process in which animals decide when to leave a patch of depleting resources to search for a new one. To study the algorithmic and neural basis of patch foraging behavior in a controlled laboratory setting, we developed a virtual foraging task for head-fixed mice. Mouse behavior could be explained by ramp-to-threshold models integrating time and rewards antagonistically. Accurate behavioral modeling required inclusion of a slowly varying "patience" variable, which modulated sensitivity to time. To investigate the neural basis of this decision-making process, we performed dense electrophysiological recordings with Neuropixels probes broadly throughout frontal cortex and underlying subcortical areas. We found that decision variables from the reward integrator model were represented in neural activity, most robustly in frontal cortical areas. Regression modeling followed by unsupervised clustering identified a subset of neurons with ramping activity. These neurons' firing rates ramped up gradually in single trials over long time scales (up to tens of seconds), were inhibited by rewards, and were better described as being generated by a continuous ramp rather than a discrete stepping process. Together, these results identify reward integration via a continuous ramping process in frontal cortex as a likely candidate for the mechanism by which the mammalian brain solves patch foraging problems.

11.
bioRxiv ; 2023 Dec 23.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36993589

RESUMO

Keypoint tracking algorithms have revolutionized the analysis of animal behavior, enabling investigators to flexibly quantify behavioral dynamics from conventional video recordings obtained in a wide variety of settings. However, it remains unclear how to parse continuous keypoint data into the modules out of which behavior is organized. This challenge is particularly acute because keypoint data is susceptible to high frequency jitter that clustering algorithms can mistake for transitions between behavioral modules. Here we present keypoint-MoSeq, a machine learning-based platform for identifying behavioral modules ("syllables") from keypoint data without human supervision. Keypoint-MoSeq uses a generative model to distinguish keypoint noise from behavior, enabling it to effectively identify syllables whose boundaries correspond to natural sub-second discontinuities inherent to mouse behavior. Keypoint-MoSeq outperforms commonly used alternative clustering methods at identifying these transitions, at capturing correlations between neural activity and behavior, and at classifying either solitary or social behaviors in accordance with human annotations. Keypoint-MoSeq therefore renders behavioral syllables and grammar accessible to the many researchers who use standard video to capture animal behavior.

12.
Neuron ; 110(4): 568-570, 2022 02 16.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35176241

RESUMO

In this issue of Neuron, Krause and Drugowitsch (2022) present a novel approach to classifying sharp-wave ripples and find that far more encode spatial trajectories than previously thought. Their method compares a host of state-space models using what Bayesian statisticians call the model evidence.


Assuntos
Neurônios , Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Teorema de Bayes , Neurônios/fisiologia
13.
Nat Rev Phys ; 4(5): 292-305, 2022 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37409001

RESUMO

The brain evolved to produce behaviors that help an animal inhabit the natural world. During natural behaviors, the brain is engaged in many levels of activity from the detection of sensory inputs to decision-making to motor planning and execution. To date, most brain studies have focused on small numbers of neurons that interact in limited circuits. This allows analyzing individual computations or steps of neural processing. During behavior, however, brain activity must integrate multiple circuits in different brain regions. The activities of different brain regions are not isolated, but may be contingent on one another. Coordinated and concurrent activity within and across brain areas is organized by (1) sensory information from the environment, (2) the animal's internal behavioral state, and (3) recurrent networks of synaptic and non-synaptic connectivity. Whole-brain recording with cellular resolution provides a new opportunity to dissect the neural basis of behavior, but whole-brain activity is also mutually contingent on behavior itself. This is especially true for natural behaviors like navigation, mating, or hunting, which require dynamic interaction between the animal, its environment, and other animals. In such behaviors, the sensory experience of an unrestrained animal is actively shaped by its movements and decisions. Many of the signaling and feedback pathways that an animal uses to guide behavior only occur in freely moving animals. Recent technological advances have enabled whole-brain recording in small behaving animals including nematodes, flies, and zebrafish. These whole-brain experiments capture neural activity with cellular resolution spanning sensory, decision-making, and motor circuits, and thereby demand new theoretical approaches that integrate brain dynamics with behavioral dynamics. Here, we review the experimental and theoretical methods that are being employed to understand animal behavior and whole-brain activity, and the opportunities for physics to contribute to this emerging field of systems neuroscience.

14.
Curr Opin Neurobiol ; 70: 193-205, 2021 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34861596

RESUMO

Individual neurons often produce highly variable responses over nominally identical trials, reflecting a mixture of intrinsic 'noise' and systematic changes in the animal's cognitive and behavioral state. Disentangling these sources of variability is of great scientific interest in its own right, but it is also increasingly inescapable as neuroscientists aspire to study more complex and naturalistic animal behaviors. In these settings, behavioral actions never repeat themselves exactly and may rarely do so even approximately. Thus, new statistical methods that extract reliable features of neural activity using few, if any, repeated trials are needed. Accurate statistical modeling in this severely trial-limited regime is challenging, but still possible if simplifying structure in neural data can be exploited. We review recent works that have identified different forms of simplifying structure - including shared gain modulations across neural subpopulations, temporal smoothness in neural firing rates, and correlations in responses across behavioral conditions - and exploited them to reveal novel insights into the trial-by-trial operation of neural circuits.


Assuntos
Neurônios , Neurociências , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Neurônios/fisiologia
15.
Adv Neural Inf Process Syst ; 34: 4738-4750, 2021 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38170102

RESUMO

Understanding the operation of biological and artificial networks remains a difficult and important challenge. To identify general principles, researchers are increasingly interested in surveying large collections of networks that are trained on, or biologically adapted to, similar tasks. A standardized set of analysis tools is now needed to identify how network-level covariates-such as architecture, anatomical brain region, and model organism-impact neural representations (hidden layer activations). Here, we provide a rigorous foundation for these analyses by defining a broad family of metric spaces that quantify representational dissimilarity. Using this framework, we modify existing representational similarity measures based on canonical correlation analysis and centered kernel alignment to satisfy the triangle inequality, formulate a novel metric that respects the inductive biases in convolutional layers, and identify approximate Euclidean embeddings that enable network representations to be incorporated into essentially any off-the-shelf machine learning method. We demonstrate these methods on large-scale datasets from biology (Allen Institute Brain Observatory) and deep learning (NAS-Bench-101). In doing so, we identify relationships between neural representations that are interpretable in terms of anatomical features and model performance.

16.
Elife ; 102021 07 14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34259623

RESUMO

We present an automated method to track and identify neurons in C. elegans, called 'fast Deep Neural Correspondence' or fDNC, based on the transformer network architecture. The model is trained once on empirically derived semi-synthetic data and then predicts neural correspondence across held-out real animals. The same pre-trained model both tracks neurons across time and identifies corresponding neurons across individuals. Performance is evaluated against hand-annotated datasets, including NeuroPAL (Yemini et al., 2021). Using only position information, the method achieves 79.1% accuracy at tracking neurons within an individual and 64.1% accuracy at identifying neurons across individuals. Accuracy at identifying neurons across individuals is even higher (78.2%) when the model is applied to a dataset published by another group (Chaudhary et al., 2021). Accuracy reaches 74.7% on our dataset when using color information from NeuroPAL. Unlike previous methods, fDNC does not require straightening or transforming the animal into a canonical coordinate system. The method is fast and predicts correspondence in 10 ms making it suitable for future real-time applications.


Understanding the intricacies of the brain often requires spotting and tracking specific neurons over time and across different individuals. For instance, scientists may need to precisely monitor the activity of one neuron even as the brain moves and deforms; or they may want to find universal patterns by comparing signals from the same neuron across different individuals. Both tasks require matching which neuron is which in different images and amongst a constellation of cells. This is theoretically possible in certain 'model' animals where every single neuron is known and carefully mapped out. Still, it remains challenging: neurons move relative to one another as the animal changes posture, and the position of a cell is also slightly different between individuals. Sophisticated computer algorithms are increasingly used to tackle this problem, but they are far too slow to track neural signals as real-time experiments unfold. To address this issue, Yu et al. designed a new algorithm based on the Transformer, an artificial neural network originally used to spot relationships between words in sentences. To learn relationships between neurons, the algorithm was fed hundreds of thousands of 'semi-synthetic' examples of constellations of neurons. Instead of painfully collated actual experimental data, these datasets were created by a simulator based on a few simple measurements. Testing the new algorithm on the tiny worm Caenorhabditis elegans revealed that it was faster and more accurate, finding corresponding neurons in about 10ms. The work by Yu et al. demonstrates the power of using simulations rather than experimental data to train artificial networks. The resulting algorithm can be used immediately to help study how the brain of C. elegans makes decisions or controls movements. Ultimately, this research could allow brain-machine interfaces to be developed.


Assuntos
Caenorhabditis elegans/fisiologia , Aprendizado Profundo , Neurônios/fisiologia , Algoritmos , Animais , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Mãos , Humanos , Aprendizado de Máquina , Redes Neurais de Computação
17.
Neuron ; 109(18): 2967-2980.e11, 2021 09 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34363753

RESUMO

Neurons in the medial entorhinal cortex alter their firing properties in response to environmental changes. This flexibility in neural coding is hypothesized to support navigation and memory by dividing sensory experience into unique episodes. However, it is unknown how the entorhinal circuit as a whole transitions between different representations when sensory information is not delineated into discrete contexts. Here we describe rapid and reversible transitions between multiple spatial maps of an unchanging task and environment. These remapping events were synchronized across hundreds of neurons, differentially affected navigational cell types, and correlated with changes in running speed. Despite widespread changes in spatial coding, remapping comprised a translation along a single dimension in population-level activity space, enabling simple decoding strategies. These findings provoke reconsideration of how the medial entorhinal cortex dynamically represents space and suggest a remarkable capacity of cortical circuits to rapidly and substantially reorganize their neural representations.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico/métodos , Córtex Entorrinal/fisiologia , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Comportamento Espacial/fisiologia , Animais , Feminino , Masculino , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL
18.
Adv Neural Inf Process Syst ; 33: 14350-14361, 2020 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35002191

RESUMO

Sparse sequences of neural spikes are posited to underlie aspects of working memory [1], motor production [2], and learning [3, 4]. Discovering these sequences in an unsupervised manner is a longstanding problem in statistical neuroscience [5-7]. Promising recent work [4, 8] utilized a convolutive nonnegative matrix factorization model [9] to tackle this challenge. However, this model requires spike times to be discretized, utilizes a sub-optimal least-squares criterion, and does not provide uncertainty estimates for model predictions or estimated parameters. We address each of these shortcomings by developing a point process model that characterizes fine-scale sequences at the level of individual spikes and represents sequence occurrences as a small number of marked events in continuous time. This ultra-sparse representation of sequence events opens new possibilities for spike train modeling. For example, we introduce learnable time warping parameters to model sequences of varying duration, which have been experimentally observed in neural circuits [10]. We demonstrate these advantages on experimental recordings from songbird higher vocal center and rodent hippocampus.

19.
Curr Biol ; 30(1): 70-82.e4, 2020 01 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31866367

RESUMO

Nervous systems have evolved to combine environmental information with internal state to select and generate adaptive behavioral sequences. To better understand these computations and their implementation in neural circuits, natural behavior must be carefully measured and quantified. Here, we collect high spatial resolution video of single zebrafish larvae swimming in a naturalistic environment and develop models of their action selection across exploration and hunting. Zebrafish larvae swim in punctuated bouts separated by longer periods of rest called interbout intervals. We take advantage of this structure by categorizing bouts into discrete types and representing their behavior as labeled sequences of bout types emitted over time. We then construct probabilistic models-specifically, marked renewal processes-to evaluate how bout types and interbout intervals are selected by the fish as a function of its internal hunger state, behavioral history, and the locations and properties of nearby prey. Finally, we evaluate the models by their predictive likelihood and their ability to generate realistic trajectories of virtual fish swimming through simulated environments. Our simulations capture multiple timescales of structure in larval zebrafish behavior and expose many ways in which hunger state influences their action selection to promote food seeking during hunger and safety during satiety.


Assuntos
Natação/fisiologia , Peixe-Zebra/fisiologia , Animais , Fome , Modelos Biológicos , Modelos Estatísticos , Comportamento Predatório/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
20.
Curr Opin Neurobiol ; 46: 14-24, 2017 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28732273

RESUMO

Computational neuroscience is, to first order, dominated by two approaches: the 'bottom-up' approach, which searches for statistical patterns in large-scale neural recordings, and the 'top-down' approach, which begins with a theory of computation and considers plausible neural implementations. While this division is not clear-cut, we argue that these approaches should be much more intimately linked. From a Bayesian perspective, computational theories provide constrained prior distributions on neural data-albeit highly sophisticated ones. By connecting theory to observation via a probabilistic model, we provide the link necessary to test, evaluate, and revise our theories in a data-driven and statistically rigorous fashion. This review highlights examples of this theory-driven pipeline for neural data analysis in recent literature and illustrates it with a worked example based on the temporal difference learning model of dopamine.


Assuntos
Biologia Computacional , Modelos Neurológicos , Modelos Estatísticos , Neurociências/métodos , Animais , Humanos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA