Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 144
Filtrar
1.
Cogn Psychol ; 149: 101641, 2024 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38377823

RESUMO

Position-specific intrusions of items from prior lists are rare but important phenomena that distinguish broad classes of theory in serial memory. They are uniquely predicted by position coding theories, which assume items on all lists are associated with the same set of codes representing their positions. Activating a position code activates items associated with it in current and prior lists in proportion to their distance from the activated position. Thus, prior list intrusions are most likely to come from the coded position. Alternative "item dependent" theories based on associations between items and contexts built from items have difficulty accounting for the position specificity of prior list intrusions. We tested the position coding account with a position-cued recognition task designed to produce prior list interference. Cuing a position should activate a position code, which should activate items in nearby positions in the current and prior lists. We presented lures from the prior list to test for position-specific activation in response time and error rate; lures from nearby positions should interfere more. We found no evidence for such interference in 10 experiments, falsifying the position coding prediction. We ran two serial recall experiments with the same materials and found position-specific prior list intrusions. These results challenge all theories of serial memory: Position coding theories can explain the prior list intrusions in serial recall and but not the absence of prior list interference in cued recognition. Item dependent theories can explain the absence of prior list interference in cued recognition but cannot explain the occurrence of prior list intrusions in serial recall.


Assuntos
Rememoração Mental , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Humanos , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Tempo de Reação , Memória de Curto Prazo
2.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 35(11): 1806-1822, 2023 11 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37677065

RESUMO

Limbic and motor integration is enabled by a mesial temporal to motor cortex network. Parkinson disease (PD) is characterized by a loss of dorsal striatal dopamine but relative preservation of mesolimbic dopamine early in disease, along with changes to motor action control. Here, we studied 47 patients with PD using the Simon conflict task and [18F]fallypride PET imaging. Additionally, a cohort of 16 patients participated in a single-blinded dextroamphetamine (dAMPH) study. Task performance was evaluated using the diffusion model for conflict tasks, which allows for an assessment of interpretable action control processes. First, a voxel-wise examination disclosed a negative relationship, such that longer non-decision time is associated with reduced D2-like binding potential (BPND) in the bilateral putamen, left globus pallidus, and right insula. Second, an ROI analysis revealed a positive relationship, such that shorter non-decision time is associated with reduced D2-like BPND in the amygdala and ventromedial OFC. The difference in non-decision time between off-dAMPH and on-dAMPH trials was positively associated with D2-like BPND in the globus pallidus. These findings support the idea that dysfunction of the traditional striatal-motor loop underlies action control deficits but also suggest that a compensatory parallel limbic-motor loop regulates motor output.


Assuntos
Dopamina , Doença de Parkinson , Humanos , Corpo Estriado/metabolismo , Dopamina/metabolismo , Doença de Parkinson/diagnóstico por imagem , Tomografia por Emissão de Pósitrons/métodos , Receptores de Dopamina D2/metabolismo
3.
Cogn Psychol ; 145: 101583, 2023 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37429216

RESUMO

Guided by the conjecture that memory retrieval is attention turned inward, we examined serial attention in serial memory, combining the psychological refractory period (PRP) procedure from attention research with cued recall of two items from brief six-item lists. We report six experiments showing robust PRP effects in cued recall from memory (1-4) and cued report from perceptual displays (5-6), which suggest that memory retrieval requires the same attentional bottleneck as "retrieval" from perception. There were strong direction effects in each memory experiment. Response time (RT) was shorter and accuracy was higher when the cues occurred in the forward direction (left-to-right, top-to-bottom, first-to-last), replicating differences between forward and backward serial recall. Cue positions had strong effects on RT and accuracy in the memory experiments (1-4). The pattern suggested that subjects find cued items in memory by stepping through the list from the beginning or the end, with a preference for starting at the beginning. The perceptual experiments (5-6) showed weak effects of position that were more consistent with direct access. In all experiments, the distance between the cues in the list (lag) had weak effects, suggesting that subjects searched for each cue from the beginning or end of the list more often than they moved through the list from the first cue to the second. Direction, distance, and lag effects on RT and inter-response interval changed with SOA in a manner that suggested they affect bottleneck or pre-bottleneck processes that create and execute a plan for successive retrievals. We conclude that sequential retrieval from memory and sequential attention to perception engage the same computations and we show how computational models of memory can be interpreted as models of attention focused on memory.


Assuntos
Sinais (Psicologia) , Período Refratário Psicológico , Humanos , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Memória , Tempo de Reação
4.
Behav Brain Sci ; 46: e140, 2023 07 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37462181

RESUMO

The binary distinction De Neys questions has been put forward many times since the beginnings of psychology, in slightly different forms and under different names. It has proved enormously useful and has received detailed empirical support and careful modeling. At heart the distinction is that between knowledge in long-term memory and control processes in short-term memory.


Assuntos
Memória de Curto Prazo , Humanos , Tempo
5.
J Neurosci ; 41(48): 10023-10030, 2021 12 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34750225

RESUMO

The stop-signal task is a well-established assessment of response inhibition, and in humans, proficiency is linked to dorsal striatum D2 receptor availability. Parkinson's disease (PD) is characterized by changes to efficiency of response inhibition. Here, we studied 17 PD patients (6 female and 11 male) using the stop-signal paradigm in a single-blinded d-amphetamine (dAMPH) study. Participants completed [18F]fallypride positron emission topography (PET) imaging in both placebo and dAMPH conditions. A voxel-wise analysis of the relationship between binding potential (BPND) and stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) revealed that faster SSRT is associated with greater D2-like BPND in the amygdala and hippocampus (right cluster qFDR-corr = 0.026, left cluster qFDR-corr = 0.002). A region of interest (ROI) examination confirmed this association in both the amygdala (coefficient = -48.26, p = 0.005) and hippocampus (coefficient = -104.94, p = 0.007). As healthy dopaminergic systems in the dorsal striatum appear to regulate response inhibition, we interpret our findings in PD to indicate either nigrostriatal damage unmasking a mesolimbic contribution to response inhibition, or a compensatory adaptation from the limbic and mesial temporal dopamine systems. These novel results expand the conceptualization of action-control networks, whereby limbic and motor loops may be functionally connected.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT While Parkinson's disease (PD) is characteristically recognized for its motor symptoms, some patients develop impulsive and compulsive behaviors (ICBs), manifested as repetitive and excessive participation in reward-driven activities, including sex, gambling, shopping, eating, and hobbyism. Such cognitive alterations compel a consideration of response inhibition in PD. To investigate inhibitory control and assess the brain regions that may participate, we assessed PD patients using a single-blinded d-amphetamine (dAMPH) study, with [18F]fallypride positron emission topography (PET) imaging, and stop-signal task performance. We find a negative relationship between D2-like binding in the mesial temporal region and top-signal reaction time (SSRT), with greater BPND associated with a faster SSRT. These discoveries indicate a novel role for mesolimbic dopamine in response inhibition, and advocate for limbic regulation of action control in this clinical population.


Assuntos
Tonsila do Cerebelo/metabolismo , Hipocampo/metabolismo , Doença de Parkinson/metabolismo , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Receptores de Dopamina D2/metabolismo , Idoso , Tonsila do Cerebelo/fisiopatologia , Dextroanfetamina/farmacologia , Inibidores da Captação de Dopamina/farmacologia , Feminino , Hipocampo/fisiopatologia , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Doença de Parkinson/fisiopatologia , Tomografia por Emissão de Pósitrons , Tempo de Reação/efeitos dos fármacos , Método Simples-Cego
6.
J Neurophysiol ; 121(4): 1300-1314, 2019 04 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30726163

RESUMO

Stochastic accumulator models account for response times and errors in perceptual decision making by assuming a noisy accumulation of perceptual evidence to a threshold. Previously, we explained saccade visual search decision making by macaque monkeys with a stochastic multiaccumulator model in which accumulation was driven by a gated feed-forward integration to threshold of spike trains from visually responsive neurons in frontal eye field that signal stimulus salience. This neurally constrained model quantitatively accounted for response times and errors in visual search for a target among varying numbers of distractors and replicated the dynamics of presaccadic movement neurons hypothesized to instantiate evidence accumulation. This modeling framework suggested strategic control over gate or over threshold as two potential mechanisms to accomplish speed-accuracy tradeoff (SAT). Here, we show that our gated accumulator model framework can account for visual search performance under SAT instructions observed in a milestone neurophysiological study of frontal eye field. This framework captured key elements of saccade search performance, through observed modulations of neural input, as well as flexible combinations of gate and threshold parameters necessary to explain differences in SAT strategy across monkeys. However, the trajectories of the model accumulators deviated from the dynamics of most presaccadic movement neurons. These findings demonstrate that traditional theoretical accounts of SAT are incomplete descriptions of the underlying neural adjustments that accomplish SAT, offer a novel mechanistic account of decision-making mechanisms during speed-accuracy tradeoff, and highlight questions regarding the identity of model and neural accumulators. NEW & NOTEWORTHY A gated accumulator model is used to elucidate neurocomputational mechanisms of speed-accuracy tradeoff. Whereas canonical stochastic accumulators adjust strategy only through variation of an accumulation threshold, we demonstrate that strategic adjustments are accomplished by flexible combinations of both modulation of the evidence representation and adaptation of accumulator gate and threshold. The results indicate how model-based cognitive neuroscience can translate between abstract cognitive models of performance and neural mechanisms of speed-accuracy tradeoff.


Assuntos
Modelos Neurológicos , Movimentos Sacádicos , Percepção Visual , Animais , Tomada de Decisões , Macaca , Córtex Sensório-Motor/fisiologia , Filtro Sensorial , Processos Estocásticos , Campos Visuais
7.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 111(7): 2848-53, 2014 Feb 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24550315

RESUMO

Decision-making is explained by psychologists through stochastic accumulator models and by neurophysiologists through the activity of neurons believed to instantiate these models. We investigated an overlooked scaling problem: How does a response time (RT) that can be explained by a single model accumulator arise from numerous, redundant accumulator neurons, each of which individually appears to explain the variability of RT? We explored this scaling problem by developing a unique ensemble model of RT, called e pluribus unum, which embodies the well-known dictum "out of many, one." We used the e pluribus unum model to analyze the RTs produced by ensembles of redundant, idiosyncratic stochastic accumulators under various termination mechanisms and accumulation rate correlations in computer simulations of ensembles of varying size. We found that predicted RT distributions are largely invariant to ensemble size if the accumulators share at least modestly correlated accumulation rates and RT is not governed by the most extreme accumulators. Under these regimes the termination times of individual accumulators was predictive of ensemble RT. We also found that the threshold measured on individual accumulators, corresponding to the firing rate of neurons measured at RT, can be invariant with RT but is equivalent to the specified model threshold only when the rate correlation is very high.


Assuntos
Modelos Neurológicos , Modelos Psicológicos , Neurônios/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Biologia Computacional , Simulação por Computador , Humanos , Método de Monte Carlo , Neurofisiologia , Processos Estocásticos
8.
J Hand Surg Am ; 42(1): 16-23.e2, 2017 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27863829

RESUMO

PURPOSE: To describe the effect of carpal tunnel release (CTR) on typing performance. METHODS: We prospectively studied 27 patients undergoing open CTR. Patient demographics and clinical characteristics including nerve conduction studies, electromyography results, and duration of symptoms were collected. Before surgery and at 8 time points after surgery, ranging from 1 to 12 weeks, typing performance for an approximately 500-character paragraph was assessed via an on-line platform. The Michigan Hand Questionnaire (MHQ) and the Boston Carpal Tunnel Questionnaire functional component (BCTQ-F) and symptom severity component (BCTQ-S) component were completed before surgery and at 1, 3, 6, and 12 weeks after surgery. We used repeated-measures analyses of variance and follow-up dependent-samples t tests to analyze change in typing performance across sessions, and linear regressions to assess relationships between typing performance and demographic and outcome measures. We compared typing speed with the MHQ, BCTQ-F, and BCTQ-S using the Pearson correlation test. RESULTS: Average typing speed decreased significantly from 49.7 ± 2.7 words per minute (wpm) before surgery to 45.2 ± 3.1 wpm at 8 to 10 days after surgery. Mean typing speed for the group exceeded the preoperative value between weeks 2 and 3, with continued improvement to 53.5 ± 3.5 wpm at 12 weeks after surgery. No clinical or demographic variables were associated with the rate of recovery or the magnitude of improvement after CTR. The MHQ, BCTQ-F, and BCTQ-S each demonstrated significant improvement from preoperative values over the 12-week period. The MHQ and BCTQ-F scores correlated well with typing speed. CONCLUSIONS: On average, typing speed returned to preoperative levels between 2 and 3 weeks after CTR and typing speed showed improvement beyond preoperative levels after surgery. The MHQ and BCTQ-F correlate well with typing speed after CTR. TYPE OF STUDY/LEVEL OF EVIDENCE: Prognostic IV.


Assuntos
Síndrome do Túnel Carpal/cirurgia , Periféricos de Computador , Avaliação da Deficiência , Eletromiografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Condução Nervosa , Estudos Prospectivos , Inquéritos e Questionários , Resultado do Tratamento
9.
J Neural Transm (Vienna) ; 122(12): 1693-701, 2015 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26354102

RESUMO

Freezing of gait is a disabling symptom of Parkinson's disease (PD) that involves failure to initiate and continue motor activity appropriately. PD disrupts fronto-basal ganglia circuitries that also implement the inhibition of responses, leading to the hypothesis that freezing of gait may involve fundamental changes in both initiation and inhibition of motor actions. We asked whether PD patients who show freezing of gait show selective deficits in their ability to inhibit upper and lower extremity reactions. We compared older healthy controls, older PD controls without freezing of gait, and older PD participants with freezing of gait, in stop-signal tasks that measured the initiation (go trials) and inhibition (stop trials) of both hand and foot responses. When only go trials were presented, all three groups showed similar initiation speeds across lower and upper extremity responses. When stop-signal trials were introduced, both PD groups slowed their reactions nearly twice as much as healthy controls. While this adjustment helped PD controls stop their actions as quickly as healthy controls, PD patients with freezing showed significantly delayed inhibitory control of both upper and lower extremities. When anticipating the need to stop their actions urgently, PD patients show greater adjustments (i.e., slowing) to reaction speed than healthy controls. Despite these proactive adjustments, PD patients who freeze show marked impairments in inhibiting both upper and lower extremity responses, suggesting that freezing may involve a fundamental disruption to the brain's inhibitory control system.


Assuntos
Inibição Psicológica , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Doença de Parkinson/fisiopatologia , Idoso , Feminino , Pé/fisiopatologia , Transtornos Neurológicos da Marcha/fisiopatologia , Mãos/fisiopatologia , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Doença de Parkinson/psicologia , Tempo de Reação
10.
Brain Cogn ; 95: 90-8, 2015 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25769133

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Cognitive control impairments are linked to functional outcome in schizophrenia. The goal of the current study was to investigate precise abnormalities in two aspects of cognitive control: reactively changing a prepared response, and monitoring performance and adjusting behavior accordingly. We adapted an oculomotor task from neurophysiological studies of the cellular basis of cognitive control in nonhuman primates. METHODS: 16 medicated outpatients with schizophrenia (SZ) and 18 demographically-matched healthy controls performed the modified double-step task. In this task, participants were required to make a saccade to a visual target. Infrequently, the target jumped to a new location and participants were instructed to rapidly inhibit and change their response. A race model provided an estimate of the time needed to cancel a planned movement. Response monitoring was assessed by measuring reaction time (RT) adjustments based on trial history. RESULTS: SZ patients had normal visually-guided saccadic RTs but required more time to switch the response to the new target location. Additionally, the estimated latency of inhibition was longer in patients and related to employment. Finally, although both groups slowed down on trials that required inhibiting and changing a response, patients showed exaggerated performance-based adjustments in RTs, which was correlated with positive symptom severity. CONCLUSIONS: SZ patients have impairments in rapidly inhibiting eye movements and show idiosyncratic response monitoring. These results are consistent with functional abnormalities in a network involving cortical oculomotor regions, the superior colliculus, and basal ganglia, as described in neurophysiological studies of non-human primates using an identical paradigm, and provide a translational bridge for understanding cognitive symptoms of SZ.


Assuntos
Função Executiva/fisiologia , Inibição Psicológica , Movimentos Sacádicos , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatologia , Psicologia do Esquizofrênico , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação
11.
Mem Cognit ; 43(6): 837-51, 2015 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25663003

RESUMO

Two modes of response selection--a mediated route involving categorization and a nonmediated route involving instance-based memory retrieval--have been proposed to explain response congruency effects in task-switching situations. In the present study, we sought a better understanding of the development and characteristics of the nonmediated route. In two experiments involving training and transfer phases, we investigated practice effects at the level of individual target presentations, transfer effects associated with changing category-response mappings, target-specific effects from comparisons of old and new targets during transfer, and the percentages of early responses associated with task-nonspecific response selection (the target preceded the task cue on every trial). The training results suggested that the nonmediated route is quickly learned in the context of target-cue order and becomes increasingly involved in response selection with practice. The transfer results suggested that the target-response instances underlying the nonmediated route involve abstract response labels coding response congruency that can be rapidly remapped to alternative responses, but not rewritten when category-response mappings change after practice. Implications for understanding the nonmediated route and its relationship with the mediated route are discussed.


Assuntos
Formação de Conceito/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Transferência de Experiência/fisiologia , Adulto , Humanos , Adulto Jovem
12.
Psychol Rev ; 2024 Feb 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38386394

RESUMO

This article introduces an integrated and biologically inspired theory of decision making, motor preparation, and motor execution. The theory is formalized as an extension of the diffusion model, in which diffusive accumulated evidence from the decision-making process is continuously conveyed to motor areas of the brain that prepare the response, where it is smoothed by a mechanism that approximates a Kalman-Bucy filter. The resulting motor preparation variable is gated prior to reaching agonist muscles until it exceeds a particular level of activation. We tested this gated cascade diffusion model by continuously probing the electrical activity of the response agonists through electromyography in four choice tasks that span a variety of domains in cognitive sciences, namely motion perception, numerical cognition, recognition memory, and lexical knowledge. The model provided a good quantitative account of behavioral and electromyographic data and systematically outperformed previous models. This work represents an advance in the integration of processes involved in simple decisions and sheds new light on the interplay between decision and motor systems. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

13.
J Neurosci ; 32(10): 3433-46, 2012 Mar 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22399766

RESUMO

We describe a stochastic accumulator model demonstrating that visual search performance can be understood as a gated feedforward cascade from a salience map to multiple competing accumulators. The model quantitatively accounts for behavior and predicts neural dynamics of macaque monkeys performing visual search for a target stimulus among different numbers of distractors. The salience accumulated in the model is equated with the spike trains recorded from visually responsive neurons in the frontal eye field. Accumulated variability in the firing rates of these neurons explains choice probabilities and the distributions of correct and error response times with search arrays of different set sizes if the accumulators are mutually inhibitory. The dynamics of the stochastic accumulators quantitatively predict the activity of presaccadic movement neurons that initiate eye movements if gating inhibition prevents accumulation before the representation of stimulus salience emerges. Adjustments in the level of gating inhibition can control trade-offs in speed and accuracy that optimize visual search performance.


Assuntos
Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Filtro Sensorial/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Animais , Macaca radiata , Masculino , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Processos Estocásticos
14.
Psychol Sci ; 24(3): 352-62, 2013 Mar 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23399493

RESUMO

The stop-signal paradigm is a popular method for examining response inhibition and impulse control in psychology, cognitive neuroscience, and clinical domains because it allows the estimation of the covert latency of the stop process: the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT). In three sets of simulations, we examined to what extent SSRTs that were estimated with the popular mean and integration methods were influenced by the skew of the reaction time distribution and the gradual slowing of the response latencies. We found that the mean method consistently overestimated SSRT. The integration method tended to underestimate SSRT when response latencies gradually increased. This underestimation bias was absent when SSRTs were estimated with the integration method for smaller blocks of trials. Thus, skewing and response slowing can lead to spurious inhibitory differences. We recommend that the mean method of estimating SSRT be abandoned in favor of the integration method.


Assuntos
Inibição Psicológica , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Simulação por Computador/estatística & dados numéricos , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos
15.
Psychol Rev ; 130(6): 1672-1687, 2023 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36892899

RESUMO

We address four issues in response to Osth and Hurlstone's (2022) commentary on the context retrieval and updating (CRU) theory of serial order (Logan, 2021). First, we clarify the relations between CRU, chains, and associations. We show that CRU is not equivalent to a chaining theory and uses similarity rather than association to retrieve contexts. Second, we fix an error Logan (2021) made in accounting for the tendency to recall ACB instead of ACD in recalling ABCDEF (fill-in vs. in-fill errors, respectively). When implemented correctly, the idea that subjects mix the current context with an initial list cue after the first order error correctly predicts that fill-in errors are more frequent than in-fill errors. Third, we address position-specific prior-list intrusions, suggesting modifications to CRU and introducing a position-coding model based on CRU representations to account for them. We suggest that position-specific prior-list intrusions are evidence for position coding on some proportion of the trials but are not evidence against item coding on other trials. Finally, we address position-specific between-group intrusions in structured lists, agreeing with Osth and Hurlstone that reasonable modifications to CRU cannot account for them. We suggest that such intrusions support position coding on some proportion of the trials but do not rule out CRU-like item-based codes. We conclude by suggesting that item-independent and item-dependent coding are alternative strategies for serial recall and we stress the importance of accounting for immediate performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Rememoração Mental , Aprendizagem Seriada , Humanos , Aprendizagem Seriada/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Memória de Curto Prazo
16.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 30(3): 1028-1040, 2023 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36471229

RESUMO

Guided by the idea that memory retrieval is selective attention turned inward, we report four experiments examining the time-course of focusing attention on memory. We used a novel episodic flanker task that turns the famous perceptual flanker task inward, presenting memory lists followed by probes that asked whether a cued letter had appeared in the same position in the memory list. Like the perceptual flanker task, we manipulated distance to measure the sharpness of the focus of attention on memory, and compatibility to measure the resistance to distraction. To measure the time-course of focusing, we presented a cue indicating the probed position in the interval between the list and the probe and varied the interval between the cue and the probe (0, 250, 500, 750 ms). The main questions were whether the focus would become sharper and resistance to distraction would become stronger as cue-probe delay increased. Experiments 1a and 1b showed strong distance effects and strong cue-probe interval effects but no reliable interaction between them. Experiments 2a and 2b showed robust compatibility effects and cue-probe interval effects but no interaction between them. Thus, there is no evidence that the sharpness of the focus increases and little evidence that the resistance to distraction improves over time. The robust reduction in response time and slight increase in accuracy with cue-probe interval may reflect the time-course of orienting to the cued position in the memory list prior to focusing on the item it contains.


Assuntos
Atenção , Memória de Curto Prazo , Humanos , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia
17.
J Neurosci ; 31(35): 12604-12, 2011 Aug 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21880921

RESUMO

Humans and macaque monkeys adjust their response time adaptively in stop-signal (countermanding) tasks, responding slower after stop-signal trials than after control trials with no stop signal. We investigated the neural mechanism underlying this adaptive response time adjustment in macaque monkeys performing a saccade countermanding task. Earlier research showed that movements are initiated when the random accumulation of presaccadic movement-related activity reaches a fixed threshold. We found that a systematic delay in response time after stop-signal trials was accomplished not through a change of threshold, baseline, or accumulation rate, but instead through a change in the time when activity first began to accumulate. The neurons underlying movement initiation have been identified with stochastic accumulator models of response time performance. Therefore, this new result provides surprising new insights into the neural instantiation of stochastic accumulator models and the mechanisms through which executive control can be exerted.


Assuntos
Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Animais , Potenciais Evocados Visuais/fisiologia , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Inibição Psicológica , Modelos Lineares , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos , Movimento , Neurônios/classificação , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos
18.
J Cogn ; 5(1): 37, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36072096

RESUMO

Often, we depart from an intended course of events to react to sudden situational demands (an intervening event) before resuming the originally planned action. Executing an action to an intervening event can be delayed if the features of this action plan partly overlap with an action plan retained in working memory (WM) compared to when they completely overlap or do not overlap. This delay is referred to as a partial repetition cost (PRC). PRCs are typically attributed to code confusion between action plans in WM. We tested this by training the component action plans extensively to reduce their reliance on WM. If PRCs are caused by code confusion within WM, then PRCs should be reduced and possibly eliminated with extensive practice. To test this, participants performed a partial repetition (PR) task after 0, 4 and 8.5 sessions of stimulus-response (S-R) training. In the PR task, participants saw two visual events. They retained an action to the first event while executing a speeded action to a second (intervening) event; afterwards, they executed the retained action. The two action plans either partly overlapped or did not overlap. Results showed that extensive (S-R and PR task) practice reduced but did not eliminate PRCs. A reduction in PRCs (code confusion) with practice is compatible with memory models that assume action events become more specific and less reliant on WM with practice. These findings merit expansions of PR tasks to other domains and broader conceptions of action plans that incorporate the formal structure of memory models.

19.
Psychol Rev ; 129(5): 1144-1182, 2022 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35389715

RESUMO

Decisions about where to move the eyes depend on neurons in frontal eye field (FEF). Movement neurons in FEF accumulate salience evidence derived from FEF visual neurons to select the location of a saccade target among distractors. How visual neurons achieve this salience representation is unknown. We present a neuro-computational model of target selection called salience by competitive and recurrent interactions (SCRI), based on the competitive interaction model of attentional selection and decision-making (Smith & Sewell, 2013). SCRI selects targets by synthesizing localization and identification information to yield a dynamically evolving representation of salience across the visual field. SCRI accounts for neural spiking of individual FEF visual neurons, explaining idiosyncratic differences in neural dynamics with specific parameters. Many visual neurons resolve the competition between search items through feedforward inhibition between signals representing different search items, some also require lateral inhibition, and many act as recurrent gates to modulate the incoming flow of information about stimulus identity. SCRI was tested further by using simulated spiking representations of visual salience as input to the gated accumulator model of FEF movement neurons (Purcell et al., 2010, 2012). Predicted saccade response times fit those observed for search arrays of different set sizes and different target-distractor similarities, and accumulator trajectories replicated movement neuron discharge rates. These findings offer new insights into visual decision-making through converging neuro-computational constraints and provide a novel computational account of the diversity of FEF visual neurons. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Movimentos Sacádicos , Campos Visuais , Humanos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Lobo Frontal/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
20.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 23(11): 3388-99, 2011 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21452946

RESUMO

The right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG) has been hypothesized to mediate response inhibition. Typically response inhibition is signaled by an external stop cue, which provides a top-down signal to initiate the process. However, recent behavioral findings suggest that response inhibition can also be triggered automatically by bottom-up processes. In the present study, we evaluated whether rIFG activity would also be observed during automatic inhibition, in which no stop cue was presented and no motor inhibition was actually required. We measured rIFG activation in response to stimuli that were previously associated with stop signals but which required a response on the current trial (reversal trials). The results revealed an increase in rIFG (pars triangularis) activity, suggesting that it can be activated by associations between stimuli and stopping. Moreover, its role in inhibition tasks is not contingent on the presence of an external stop cue. We conclude that rIFG involvement in stopping is consistent with a role in reprogramming of action plans, which may comprise inhibition, and its activity can be triggered through automatic, bottom-up processing.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Sinais (Psicologia) , Lobo Frontal/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Inibição Psicológica , Tomada de Decisões/fisiologia , Tomada de Decisões/efeitos da radiação , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Tempo de Reação , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA