Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 9 de 9
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
Tipo de documento
País de afiliação
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 36(3): 492-507, 2024 03 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38165741

RESUMO

Previous work shows that automatic attention biases toward recently selected target features transfer across action and perception and even across different effectors such as the eyes and hands on a trial-by-trial basis. Although these findings suggest a common neural representation of selection history across effectors, the extent to which information about recently selected target features is encoded in overlapping versus distinct brain regions is unknown. Using fMRI and a priming of pop-out task where participants selected unpredictable, uniquely colored targets among homogeneous distractors via reach or saccade, we show that color priming is driven by shared, effector-independent underlying representations of recent selection history. Consistent with previous work, we found that the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) was commonly activated on trials where target colors were switched relative to those where the colors were repeated; however, the dorsal anterior insula exhibited effector-specific activation related to color priming. Via multivoxel cross-classification analyses, we further demonstrate that fine-grained patterns of activity in both IPS and the medial temporal lobe encode information about selection history in an effector-independent manner, such that ROI-specific models trained on activity patterns during reach selection could predict whether a color was repeated or switched on the current trial during saccade selection and vice versa. Remarkably, model generalization performance in IPS and medial temporal lobe also tracked individual differences in behavioral priming sensitivity across both types of action. These results represent a first step to clarify the neural substrates of experience-driven selection biases in contexts that require the coordination of multiple actions.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores , Movimentos Sacádicos , Humanos , Viés de Seleção , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Encéfalo , Mãos
2.
J Vis ; 21(13): 1, 2021 12 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34851391

RESUMO

To accomplish the deceptively simple task of perceiving the size of objects in the visual scene, the visual system combines information about the retinal size of the object with several other cues, including perceived distance, relative size, and prior knowledge. When local component elements are perceptually grouped to form objects, the task is further complicated because a grouped object does not have a continuous contour from which retinal size can be estimated. Here, we investigate how the visual system solves this problem and makes it possible for observers to judge the size of perceptually grouped objects. We systematically vary the shape and orientation of the component elements in a two-alternative forced-choice task and find that the perceived size of the array of component objects can be almost perfectly predicted from the distance between the centroids of the component elements and the center of the array. This is true whether the global contour forms a circle or a square. When elements were positioned such that the centroids along the global contour were at different distances from the center, perceived size was based on the average distance. These results indicate that perceived size does not depend on the size of individual elements, and that smooth contours formed by the outer edges of the component elements are not used to estimate size. The current study adds to a growing literature highlighting the importance of centroids in visual perception and may have implications for how size is estimated for ensembles of different objects.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma , Sinais (Psicologia) , Humanos , Retina , Percepção Visual
3.
J Vis ; 16(9): 7, 2016 07 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27467450

RESUMO

In daily life, humans interact with multiple objects in complex environments. A large body of literature demonstrates that target selection is biased toward recently attended features, such that reaches are faster and trajectory curvature is reduced when target features (i.e., color) are repeated (priming of pop-out). In the real world, however, objects are comprised of several features-some of which may be more suitable for action than others. When fetching a mug from the cupboard, for example, attention not only has to be allocated to the object, but also the handle. To date, no study has investigated the impact of hierarchical feature organization on target selection for action. Here, we employed a color-oddity search task in which targets were Pac-men (i.e., a circle with a triangle cut out) oriented to be either consistent or inconsistent with the percept of a global Kanizsa triangle. We found that reaches were initiated faster when a task-irrelevant illusory figure was present independent of color repetition. Additionally, consistent with priming of pop-out, both reach planning and execution were facilitated when local target colors were repeated, regardless of whether a global figure was present. We also demonstrated that figures defined by illusory, but not real contours, afforded an early target selection benefit. In sum, these findings suggest that when local targets are perceptually grouped to form an illusory surface, attention quickly spreads across the global figure and facilitates the early stage of reach planning, but not execution. In contrast, local color priming is evident throughout goal-directed reaching.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Objetivos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Cor , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
4.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 27(11): 2158-73, 2015 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26226075

RESUMO

When an object moves behind a bush, for example, its visible fragments are revealed at different times and locations across the visual field. Nonetheless, a whole moving object is perceived. Unlike traditional modal and amodal completion mechanisms known to support spatial form integration when all parts of a stimulus are simultaneously visible, relatively little is known about the neural substrates of the spatiotemporal form integration (STFI) processes involved in generating coherent object representations from a succession visible fragments. We used fMRI to identify brain regions involved in two mechanisms supporting the representation of stationary and rigidly rotating objects whose form features are shown in succession: STFI and position updating. STFI allows past and present form cues to be integrated over space and time into a coherent object even when the object is not visible in any given frame. STFI can occur whether or not the object is moving. Position updating allows us to perceive a moving object, whether rigidly rotating or translating, even when its form features are revealed at different times and locations in space. Our results suggest that STFI is mediated by visual regions beyond V1 and V2. Moreover, although widespread cortical activation has been observed for other motion percepts derived solely from form-based analyses [Tse, P. U. Neural correlates of transformational apparent motion. Neuroimage, 31, 766-773, 2006; Krekelberg, B., Vatakis, A., & Kourtzi, Z. Implied motion from form in the human visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 4373-4386, 2005], increased responses for the position updating that lead to rigidly rotating object representations were only observed in visual areas KO and possibly hMT+, indicating that this is a distinct and highly specialized type of processing.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Modelos Lineares , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Córtex Visual/irrigação sanguínea
5.
Conscious Cogn ; 22(4): 1384-92, 2013 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24100131

RESUMO

In grapheme-color synesthesia, graphemes (e.g., numbers or letters) evoke color experiences. It is generally reported that the opposite is not true: colors will not generate experiences of graphemes or their associated information. However, recent research has provided evidence that colors can implicitly elicit symbolic representations of associated graphemes. Here, we examine if these representations can be cognitively accessed. Using a mathematical verification task replacing graphemes with color patches, we find that synesthetes can verify such problems with colors as accurately as with graphemes. Doing so, however, takes time: ~250 ms per color. Moreover, we find minimal reaction time switch-costs for switching between computing with graphemes and colors. This demonstrates that given specific task demands, synesthetes can cognitively access numerical information elicited by physical colors, and they do so as accurately as with graphemes. We discuss these results in the context of possible cognitive strategies used to access the information.


Assuntos
Associação , Cognição/fisiologia , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Transtornos da Percepção/fisiopatologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Tempo de Reação , Sinestesia , Adulto Jovem
6.
Prog Brain Res ; 236: 163-192, 2017.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29157410

RESUMO

When an object partially or completely disappears behind an occluding surface, a representation of that object persists. For example, fragments of no longer visible objects can serve as an input into mid-level constructive visual processes, interacting and integrating with currently visible portions to form perceptual units and global motion signals. Remarkably, these persistent representations need not be static and can have their positions and orientations updated postdictively as new information becomes visible. In this chapter, we highlight historical considerations, behavioral evidence, and neural correlates of this type of representational updating of no longer visible information at three distinct levels of visual processing. At the lowest level, we discuss spatiotemporal boundary formation in which visual transients can be integrated over space and time to construct local illusory edges, global form, and global motion percepts. At an intermediate level, we review how the visual system updates form information seen at one moment in time and integrates it with subsequently available information to generate global shape and motion representations (e.g., spatiotemporal form integration and anorthoscopic perception). At a higher level, when an entire object completely disappears behind an occluder, the object's identity and predicted position can be maintained in the absence of visual information.


Assuntos
Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Humanos
7.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 77(8): 2740-54, 2015 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26269386

RESUMO

Objects in the world often are occluded and in motion. The visible fragments of such objects are revealed at different times and locations in space. To form coherent representations of the surfaces of these objects, the visual system must integrate local form information over space and time. We introduce a new illusion in which a rigidly rotating square is perceived on the basis of sequentially presented Pacman inducers. The illusion highlights two fundamental processes that allow us to perceive objects whose form features are revealed over time: Spatiotemporal Form Integration (STFI) and Position Updating. STFI refers to the spatial integration of persistent representations of local form features across time. Position updating of these persistent form representations allows them to be integrated into a rigid global motion percept. We describe three psychophysical experiments designed to identify spatial and temporal constraints that underlie these two processes and a fourth experiment that extends these findings to more ecologically valid stimuli. Our results indicate that although STFI can occur across relatively long delays between successive inducers (i.e., greater than 500 ms), position updating is limited to a more restricted temporal window (i.e., ~300 ms or less), and to a confined range of spatial (mis)alignment. These findings lend insight into the limits of mechanisms underlying the visual system's capacity to integrate transient, piecemeal form information, and support coherent object representations in the ever-changing environment.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Ilusões/fisiologia , Masculino , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Rotação , Adulto Jovem
8.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 18(5): 224-6, 2014 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24581557

RESUMO

A recent study showed that color synesthetes have increased color sensitivity but impaired motion perception. This is exciting because little research has examined how synesthesia affects basic perceptual processes outside the context of synesthetic experiences. The results suggest that synesthesia broadly impacts perception with greater neural implications than previously considered.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Transtornos da Percepção/fisiopatologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
9.
F1000Res ; 2: 58, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24555042

RESUMO

Our perception of an object's size arises from the integration of multiple sources of visual information including retinal size, perceived distance and its size relative to other objects in the visual field. This constructive process is revealed through a number of classic size illusions such as the Delboeuf Illusion, the Ebbinghaus Illusion and others illustrating size constancy. Here we present a novel variant of the Delbouef and Ebbinghaus size illusions that we have named the Binding Ring Illusion. The illusion is such that the perceived size of a circular array of elements is underestimated when superimposed by a circular contour - a binding ring - and overestimated when the binding ring slightly exceeds the overall size of the array. Here we characterize the stimulus conditions that lead to the illusion, and the perceptual principles that underlie it. Our findings indicate that the perceived size of an array is susceptible to the assimilation of an explicitly defined superimposed contour. Our results also indicate that the assimilation process takes place at a relatively high level in the visual processing stream, after different spatial frequencies have been integrated and global shape has been constructed. We hypothesize that the Binding Ring Illusion arises due to the fact that the size of an array of elements is not explicitly defined and therefore can be influenced (through a process of assimilation) by the presence of a superimposed object that does have an explicit size.

SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA