Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 70
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
País de afiliação
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Psychol Sci ; 25(11): 1987-93, 2014 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25210012

RESUMO

Adolescence has been characterized as a period of both opportunity and vulnerability. Numerous clinical conditions, including substance-use disorders, often emerge during adolescence. These maladaptive behaviors have been linked to problems with cognitive control, yet few studies have investigated how rewards differentially modulate attentional processes in adolescents versus adults. Here, we trained adults and adolescents on a visual task to establish stimulus-reward associations. Later, we assessed learning in an extinction task in which previously rewarded stimuli periodically appeared as distractors. Both age groups initially demonstrated value-driven attentional capture; however, the effect persisted longer in adolescents than in adults. The results could not be explained by developmental differences in visual working memory. Given the importance of attentional control to daily behaviors and clinical conditions such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, these results reveal that cognitive control failures in adolescence may be linked to a value-based attentional-capture effect.


Assuntos
Comportamento do Adolescente/psicologia , Atenção , Recompensa , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Sinais (Psicologia) , Tomada de Decisões , Feminino , Humanos , Iowa , Aprendizagem , Masculino , Memória de Curto Prazo , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Adulto Jovem
2.
Psychol Sci ; 24(8): 1585-90, 2013 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23761926

RESUMO

Recent studies have demonstrated that brief periods of training facilitate the ability to overcome distraction during future performance of a given task, and researchers have proposed that these effects rely on relational memory systems that enable individuals to link specific attentional states to their learned context. In the current work, we examined whether medial temporal lobe (MTL) structures critical for relational and contextual learning contribute to these effects. A group of amnesic patients with bilateral MTL damage and a group of matched comparison subjects both completed an attentional-capture task in which a brief training session typically leads to decreased distraction in a subsequent testing session. Whereas the comparison subjects showed normal training-related decreases in distractibility, the amnesic patients did not. Thus, our results indicate that MTL-mediated learning plays a critical role in the ability to use past experience to overcome distraction. This suggests a tight linkage between MTL-dependent relational-learning mechanisms and cognitive control.


Assuntos
Amnésia/fisiopatologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Memória de Longo Prazo/fisiologia , Lobo Temporal/lesões , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Humanos , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Tempo de Reação , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia , Lobo Temporal/fisiopatologia
3.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 85(3): 785-795, 2023 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36045310

RESUMO

Attention is guided by several factors, including task-relevant target features, which attract attention, but also statistical regularities associated distractors, which repel attention away from themselves. However, whether feature-based distractor regularities (e.g., color) are extracted automatically from a feature dimension orthogonal to the target-guiding dimension (e.g., shape) remains to be tested. In two experiments, we tested if learned distractor rejection by color operated when color was not part of the attentional control settings, specifically, while attention was guided by a shape-based target template. Participants performed a visual search task for a task-relevant shape in displays containing two unsegregated colors. These displays allowed us to manipulate target guidance (based on shape) independently from distractor-based regularities (based on color). In both experiments we found clear evidence for learned distractor rejection: faster mean response times to locate the target when a consistent distractor color was present than when it was absent. Critically, these task-irrelevant learned distractor rejection effects were robust despite strong target guidance by an orthogonal search dimension. These findings corroborate recent demonstrations of learned distractor rejection during strong target guidance, indicating that learned distractor rejection and target guidance can operate on separate feature dimensions.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores , Aprendizagem , Humanos , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
4.
Exp Aging Res ; 38(4): 411-21, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22830667

RESUMO

UNLABELLED: BACKGROUND/STUDY CONTEXT: Typical measures for assessing the useful field (UFOV) of view involve many components of attention. The objective of the current experiment was to examine differences in visual search efficiency for older individuals with and without UFOV impairment. METHODS: The authors used a computerized screening instrument to assess the useful field of view and to characterize participants as having an impaired or normal UFOV. Participants also performed two visual search tasks, a feature search (e.g., search for a green target among red distractors) or a conjunction search (e.g., a green target with a gap on its left or right side among red distractors with gaps on the left or right and green distractors with gaps on the top or bottom). RESULTS: Visual search performance did not differ between UFOV impaired and unimpaired individuals when searching for a basic feature. However, search efficiency was lower for impaired individuals than unimpaired individuals when searching for a conjunction of features. CONCLUSION: The results suggest that UFOV decline in normal aging is associated with conjunction search. This finding suggests that the underlying cause of UFOV decline may arise from an overall decline in attentional efficiency. Because the useful field of view is a reliable predictor of driving safety, the results suggest that decline in the everyday visual behavior of older adults might arise from attentional declines.


Assuntos
Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Condução de Veículo/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
5.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 84(6): 1964-1981, 2022 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35386017

RESUMO

Target templates stored in visual memory guide visual attention toward behaviorally relevant target objects. Visual attention also is guided away from nontarget distractors by longer-term learning, a phenomenon termed "learned distractor rejection." Template guidance and learned distractor rejection can occur simultaneously to further increase search efficiency. However, the underlying processes guiding learned distractor rejection are unknown. In two visual search experiments employing eye-tracking, we tested between two plausible processes: proactive versus reactive attentional control. Participants searched through two-color, spatially unsegregated displays. Participants could guide attention by both target templates and consistent nontarget distractors. We observed fewer distractor fixations (including the first eye movement) and shorter distractor dwell times. The data supported a single mechanism of learned distractor rejection, whereby observers adopted a learned, proactive attentional control setting to avoid distraction whenever possible. Further, when distraction occurred, observers rapidly recovered. We term this proactive mechanism "learned oculomotor avoidance." The current study informs theories of visual attention by demonstrating the underlying processes leading to learned distractor suppression during strong target guidance.


Assuntos
Movimentos Oculares , Aprendizagem , Tecnologia de Rastreamento Ocular , Humanos , Memória , Tempo de Reação , Percepção Visual
6.
Front Neurosci ; 16: 982005, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36685236

RESUMO

Previous research has shown that attention can be biased to targets appearing near the hand that require action responses, arguing that attention to the hand facilitates upcoming action. It is unclear whether attention orients to non-targets near the hand not requiring responses. Using electroencephalography/event-related potentials (EEG/ERP), this study investigated whether hand position affected visual orienting to non-targets under conditions that manipulated the distribution of attention. We modified an attention paradigm in which stimuli were presented briefly and rapidly on either side of fixation; participants responded to infrequent targets (15%) but not standard non-targets and either a hand or a block was placed next to one stimulus location. In Experiment 1, attention was distributed across left and right stimulus locations to determine whether P1 or N1 ERP amplitudes to non-target standards were differentially influenced by hand location. In Experiment 2, attention was narrowed to only one stimulus location to determine whether attentional focus affected orienting to non-target locations near the hand. When attention was distributed across both stimulus locations, the hand increased overall N1 amplitudes relative to the block but not selectively to stimuli appearing near the hand. However, when attention was focused on one location, amplitudes were affected by the location of attentional focus and the stimulus, but not by hand or block location. Thus, hand position appears to contribute only a non-location-specific input to standards during visual orienting, but only in cases when attention is distributed across stimulus locations.

7.
J Vis ; 11(13)2011 Nov 17.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22095972

RESUMO

Recent research suggests that perceptual processing begins earlier for figures than for background regions (B. D. Lester, L. N. Hecht, & S. P. Vecera, 2009). This "prior entry" effect begins to account for reported figural benefits. However, another difference in perceptual processing may also contribute to these observed reports: Figures may also be afforded additional perceptual processing. The current experiments examined this claim and provide evidence that targets presented on figures are perceived as offsetting later than targets appearing on grounds, suggesting extended processing of figures relative to background regions.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Humanos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia
8.
Psychol Sci ; 21(9): 1254-8, 2010 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20713634

RESUMO

Specialized, bimodal neural systems integrate visual and tactile information in the space near the hand. Here, we show that visuo-tactile representations allow attention to influence early perceptual processing, namely, figure-ground assignment. Regions that were reached toward were more likely than other regions to be assigned as foreground figures, and hand position competed with image-based information to bias figure-ground assignment. Our findings suggest that hand position allows attention to influence visual perceptual processing and that visual processes typically viewed as unimodal can be influenced by bimodal visuo-tactile representations.


Assuntos
Atenção , Mãos , Percepção Visual , Humanos , Movimento , Orientação , Postura , Tempo de Reação , Percepção do Tato
9.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 46(9): 926-941, 2020 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32391708

RESUMO

Visual attention is guided toward behaviorally relevant objects by target "templates" stored in visual memory. Visual attention also is guided away from nontarget distractors by learned distractor rejection. In a series of 5 visual search experiments, we asked if learned distractor rejection operated while attention was simultaneously guided by a target template. Participants performed a visual search in 2-color, spatially unsegregated displays where we manipulated attentional guidance by both target templates and consistent nontarget distractors. We observed faster mean response times to the target when a consistent nontarget distractor was present than when it was absent-the hallmark of learned distractor rejection-despite the use of strong target guidance. Learned distractor rejection indeed operates alongside guidance from a target template, indicating that theories of visual attention should incorporate guidance by both target templates and learned nontargets. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
10.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 16(3): 529-36, 2009 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19451380

RESUMO

Research has shown that there are at least two kinds of visual selective attention: location based and object based. In the present study, we sought to determine the locus of spatially invariant object-based selection using a dual-task paradigm. In four experiments, observers performed an attention task (object feature report or visual search) with a concurrent memory task (object memory or spatial memory). Object memory was interfered with more by a concurrent object-based attention task than by a concurrent location-based attention task. However, this interference pattern was reversed for spatial memory, with greater interference by a location-based attention task than by an object-based attention task. These findings suggest that object-based attention and location-based attention are functionally dissociable and that some forms of object-based selection operate within visual short-term memory.


Assuntos
Atenção , Percepção de Cores , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Rememoração Mental , Orientação , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Retenção Psicológica , Humanos , Psicofísica
11.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 16(2): 404-10, 2009 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19293114

RESUMO

The abrupt appearance of a new object captures attention, even when the object is task irrelevant. These findings suggest that abrupt onsets capture attention in a stimulus-driven manner and are not susceptible to top-down influences on attentional control. However, previous studies examining the ability of abrupt onsets to capture attention have used search displays that lacked significant complexity. Because attention is a limited capacity mechanism, it is possible that increasing the complexity, or perceptual load, of the search arrays may modulate capture by abrupt onsets. We used a flanker task to examine the effect of perceptual load on attentional capture by abruptly appearing objects. Subjects searched for a target letter through low-load (set size = 1) and high-load (set size = 6) displays. On each trial, irrelevant flankers also appeared, one as an onset and the other as an offset. Onset flankers affected search in low-load but not high-load displays. This modulation of attentional capture was not caused by generalized slowing when subjects searched through high-load displays; search for a single perceptually degraded target slowed response times but did not affect attentional capture. These findings demonstrate that attentional capture by an abrupt onset is attenuated when people search through high-load scenes.


Assuntos
Atenção , Memória de Curto Prazo , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Sensibilidades de Contraste , Discriminação Psicológica , Humanos , Orientação , Tempo de Reação
12.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 16(4): 654-9, 2009 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19648448

RESUMO

Attended stimuli reach perceptual-level processes before unattended stimuli do, a finding that is referred to as visual prior entry. We asked whether a similar effect arises for salient objects (foreground figures) in a visual scene. If prior entry holds for figure-ground perception, targets will be perceived to appear earlier on figures than on grounds. Participants performed a temporal order judgment by reporting the order in which targets appeared. Participants perceived that targets appearing on foreground figures occurred earlier than did those appearing on backgrounds. These findings did not result from a response bias for targets appearing on figures. Most important, when figures and grounds were spatially separated and did not share an edge, no prior-entry effects were observed. Our results suggest that figural regions are available to perceptual-level processes sooner than are grounds.


Assuntos
Atenção , Percepção de Cores , Discriminação Psicológica , Área de Dependência-Independência , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Humanos , Julgamento , Orientação , Psicofísica , Tempo de Reação
13.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 81(2): 359-376, 2019 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30421366

RESUMO

Ignoring distracting information is critical for effective visual search. When individuals are cued to ignore a stimulus, they first attend the to-be-ignored stimulus before learning to reject it. Individuals can learn to overcome the initial distraction produced by the explicit cues, although this cued distractor rejection appears for only one distractor feature. Multiple distractor colors cannot be rejected effectively, even with extensive experience. We asked if this apparent limit on distractor rejection was caused by a restriction on the number of different features (i.e., colors) that could be learned and rejected as distractors. To explore this potential capacity limitation, we asked if attention can learn to reject the smallest possible number of multiple distractors, namely, two. In four experiments examining cued distractor rejection, individuals searched through heterogeneously colored arrays containing reliable, non-target color information. In Experiments 1 and 2, we explicitly cued individuals with which of two colors (both colors in Experiment 1 or one color in Experiment 2) could be safely ignored. Cued distractors were not reliably rejected, replicating previous findings. Additionally, in Experiment 2, we presented a to-be-ignored color without explicit cues and we found that these "uncued" distractors were reliably rejected. In Experiments 3 and 4, we presented the to-be-ignored color information without explicit cues; individuals learned to reliably ignore multiple distractor colors without explicit cueing. These results suggest that learned distractor rejection is better suited to experience-driven learning than explicitly cued distractor learning: Explicit cueing reliably interferes with learned distractor rejection.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Aprendizagem , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Cor , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Tempo de Reação , Adulto Jovem
14.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 45(3): 419-433, 2019 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30802131

RESUMO

Ignoring salient distracting information is paramount to efficiently guiding attention during visual search. Learning to reject or suppress these strong sources of distraction leads to more effective visual search for targets. Participants can learn to overcome salient distractors if given reliable search regularities. If salient distractors appear in 1 location more frequently than any other, the visual system can use this environmental regularity to reduce attentional capture at the more frequent location (Wang & Theeuwes, 2018). We asked if reduced attentional capture is limited to location-based regularities, or, if the visual attentional system is configured to use feature-based regularities in reducing attentional capture as well. In 4 experiments examining attentional capture by task-irrelevant color singletons, participants searched for a shape singleton target among homogenously colored distractors. Critically, on a proportion of trials, a salient, color singleton distractor was presented. Color singleton distractors that appeared at a frequent location captured attention less than color singleton distractors that appeared at infrequent locations, replicating previous findings. In subsequent experiments we manipulated the frequency of the colors of the color singleton distractors and observed robust increases in capture based on color feature regularities. Despite strong location information, we observed reliable attentional capture attenuation by frequently presented distractor colors. Our results suggest that attentional capture is attenuated by both location and feature information. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
15.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 34(4): 842-53, 2008 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18665730

RESUMO

The authors investigated 2 effects of object-based attention: the spread of attention within an attended object and the prioritization of search across possible target locations within an attended object. Participants performed a flanker task in which the location of the task-relevant target was fixed and known to participants. A spreading attention account predicts that object-based attention will arise from the spread of attention through an attended object. A prioritization account predicts that there will be a small, if any, object-based effect because the location of the target is known in advance and objects are not required to prioritize the deployment of attentional search. The results suggested that object-based attention operates via the spread of attention within an object.


Assuntos
Atenção , Percepção de Forma , Orientação , Percepção Espacial , Percepção de Cores , Sinais (Psicologia) , Percepção de Distância , Humanos , Rememoração Mental , Mascaramento Perceptivo , Estimulação Luminosa , Prática Psicológica , Desempenho Psicomotor , Tempo de Reação , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Detecção de Sinal Psicológico
16.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 38(8): 1405-13, 2008 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18074212

RESUMO

We examined the ability to use static line drawings of eye gaze cues to orient visual-spatial attention in children with high functioning autism (HFA) compared to typically developing children (TD). The task was organized such that on valid trials, gaze cues were directed toward the same spatial location as the appearance of an upcoming target, while on invalid trials gaze cues were directed to an opposite location. Unlike TD children, children with HFA showed no advantage in reaction time (RT) on valid trials compared to invalid trials (i.e., no significant validity effect). The two stimulus onset asynchronies (200 ms, 700 ms) did not differentially affect these findings. The results suggest that children with HFA show impairments in utilizing static line drawings of gaze cues to orient visual-spatial attention.


Assuntos
Transtorno Autístico/diagnóstico , Sinais (Psicologia) , Movimentos Oculares , Orientação , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Adolescente , Transtorno Autístico/psicologia , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Inteligência , Masculino , Tempo de Reação , Valores de Referência
17.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 25(3): 1021-1027, 2018 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28537005

RESUMO

Successful goal-directed visual behavior depends on efficient disengagement of attention. Attention must be withdrawn from its current focus before being redeployed to a new object or internal process. Previous research has demonstrated that occupying cognitive processes with a secondary cellular phone conversation impairs attentional functioning and driving behavior. For example, attentional processing is significantly impacted by concurrent cell phone use, resulting in decreased explicit memory for on-road information. Here, we examined the impact of a critical component of cell-phone use-active listening-on the effectiveness of attentional disengagement. In the gap task-a saccadic manipulation of attentional disengagement-we measured saccade latencies while participants performed a secondary active listening task. Saccadic latencies significantly increased under an active listening load only when attention needed to be disengaged, indicating that active listening delays a disengagement operation. Simple dual-task interference did not account for the observed results. Rather, active cognitive engagement is required for measurable disengagement slowing to be observed. These results have implications for investigations of attention, gaze behavior, and distracted driving. Secondary tasks such as active listening or cell-phone conversations can have wide-ranging impacts on cognitive functioning, potentially impairing relatively elementary operations of attentional function, including disengagement.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Humanos , Adulto Jovem
18.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 80(2): 485-499, 2018 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29230673

RESUMO

Distraction impairs performance of many important, everyday tasks. Attentional control limits distraction by preferentially selecting important items for limited-capacity cognitive operations. Research in attentional control has typically investigated the degree to which selection of items is stimulus-driven versus goal-driven. Recent work finds that when observers initially learn a task, the selection is based on stimulus-driven factors, but through experience, goal-driven factors have an increasing influence. The modulation of selection by goals has been studied within the paradigm of learned distractor rejection, in which experience over a sequence of trials enables individuals eventually to ignore a perceptually salient distractor. The experiments presented examine whether observers can generalize learned distractor rejection to novel distractors. Observers searched for a target and ignored a salient color-singleton distractor that appeared in half of the trials. In Experiment 1, observers who learned distractor rejection in a variable environment rejected a novel distractor more effectively than observers who learned distractor rejection in a less variable, homogeneous environment, demonstrating that variable, heterogeneous stimulus environments encourage generalizable learned distractor rejection. Experiments 2 and 3 investigated the time course of learned distractor rejection across the experiment and found that after experiencing four color-singleton distractors in different blocks, observers could effectively reject subsequent novel color-singleton distractors. These results suggest that the optimization of attentional control to the task environment can be interpreted as a form of learning, demonstrating experience's critical role in attentional control.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Generalização Psicológica/fisiologia , Percepção de Cores , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
19.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 14(6): 1205-11, 2007 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18229498

RESUMO

What object properties warrant selection by object-based attention? Previous research has suggested that surface uniformity is required for object-based attentional selection (Watson & Kramer, 1999), yet nonuniform objects are encountered frequently. In the present experiments, we investigated the interplay between surface uniformity and part boundaries and their effect on object-based attention. Specifically, we asked if attention can select nonuniform objects whose surface changes occur at part boundaries. Although uniformly colored objects did exhibit object-based effects, we only observed an object-based effect for multicolored objects when surface changes occurred at part boundaries. These findings suggest that attention can only select nonuniform objects when the surface change occurs at a part boundary.


Assuntos
Atenção , Comportamento de Escolha , Humanos , Tempo de Reação , Percepção Visual
20.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 43(1): 169-180, 2017 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27808546

RESUMO

When searching for a target object in a cluttered scene, the currently attended object is typically matched against a target template, a memory representation of the object being actively searched for. To determine if the currently attended item is the target requires a high degree of similarity to the template; any imprecision would make it difficult to distinguish between targets and visually similar nontargets. Thus, for attention to be efficient in finding targets requires the target template to be highly precise. Initial research on the precision of the target template suggested that the template was a highly precise depiction of the target object. In contrast, more recent findings suggested an imprecise template, demonstrating that participants were inaccurate in detecting a target when it appeared among visually similar distractors. In the current experiments, we demonstrate that visually similar distractors can hinder attentional selection because of limitations in selection and masking, not because of template imprecision. We conclude that the target template can be highly precise yet performance limited by factors not related to the target template itself. (PsycINFO Database Record


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Objetivos , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Mascaramento Perceptivo/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA