Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 23
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
Tipo de documento
País de afiliação
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Antimicrob Agents Chemother ; 60(4): 2209-21, 2016 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26810654

RESUMO

Chlorhexidine is a bisbiguanide antiseptic used for infection control. Vancomycin-resistantE. faecium(VREfm) is among the leading causes of hospital-acquired infections. VREfm may be exposed to chlorhexidine at supra- and subinhibitory concentrations as a result of chlorhexidine bathing and chlorhexidine-impregnated central venous catheter use. We used RNA sequencing to investigate how VREfm responds to chlorhexidine gluconate exposure. Among the 35 genes upregulated ≥10-fold after 15 min of exposure to the MIC of chlorhexidine gluconate were those encoding VanA-type vancomycin resistance (vanHAX) and those associated with reduced daptomycin susceptibility (liaXYZ). We confirmed thatvanAupregulation was not strain or species specific by querying other VanA-type VRE. VanB-type genes were not induced. ThevanHpromoter was found to be responsive to subinhibitory chlorhexidine gluconate in VREfm, as was production of the VanX protein. UsingvanHreporter experiments withBacillus subtilisand deletion analysis in VREfm, we found that this phenomenon is VanR dependent. Deletion ofvanRdid not result in increased chlorhexidine susceptibility, demonstrating thatvanHAXinduction is not protective against chlorhexidine. As expected, VanA-type VRE is more susceptible to ceftriaxone in the presence of sub-MIC chlorhexidine. Unexpectedly, VREfm is also more susceptible to vancomycin in the presence of subinhibitory chlorhexidine, suggesting that chlorhexidine-induced gene expression changes lead to additional alterations in cell wall synthesis. We conclude that chlorhexidine induces expression of VanA-type vancomycin resistance genes and genes associated with daptomycin nonsusceptibility. Overall, our results indicate that the impacts of subinhibitory chlorhexidine exposure on hospital-associated pathogens should be further investigated in laboratory studies.


Assuntos
Anti-Infecciosos Locais/farmacologia , Proteínas de Bactérias/agonistas , Clorexidina/análogos & derivados , Enterococcus faecium/efeitos dos fármacos , Regulação Bacteriana da Expressão Gênica , Resistência a Vancomicina/efeitos dos fármacos , Antibacterianos/farmacologia , Bacillus subtilis/genética , Bacillus subtilis/metabolismo , Proteínas de Bactérias/genética , Proteínas de Bactérias/metabolismo , Carbono-Oxigênio Ligases/genética , Carbono-Oxigênio Ligases/metabolismo , Parede Celular/efeitos dos fármacos , Parede Celular/metabolismo , Clorexidina/farmacologia , Daptomicina/farmacologia , Enterococcus faecium/genética , Enterococcus faecium/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Enterococcus faecium/metabolismo , Perfilação da Expressão Gênica , Testes de Sensibilidade Microbiana , Fatores de Transcrição/genética , Fatores de Transcrição/metabolismo , Vancomicina/farmacologia , Resistência a Vancomicina/genética
2.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 36(8): 2342-2360, 2022 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34311662

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To cross-validate RAVLT performance validity cut-offs and the RAVLT/RO discriminant function in a large neuropsychological sample. METHOD: RAVLT scores and the RAVLT/RO discriminant function were compared in credible (n = 100) and noncredible (n = 353) neuropsychology referrals. RESULTS: Noncredible patients scored lower than credible patients on RAVLT scores and the RAVLT/RO discriminant function. With cut-offs set to ≥90% specificity, highest sensitivities were observed for the discriminant function (cut-off ≤.064; 55.8%), recognition total (cut-off ≤9; 53.1%), the recognition combination score (≤10; 47.7%), and total learning across trials (cut-off ≤31; 45.3%). Individuals with histories of learning difficulties were over-represented in the 10% of credible patients exceeding cut-offs. When these individuals were removed, cut-offs could be tightened while still maintaining at least 90% specificity, and thereby increasing sensitivity (e.g., recognition total cut-off ≤10, 65% sensitivity; RAVLT/RO discriminant function cut-off ≤.176, 58% sensitivity). When three of the most sensitive, non-overlapping scores were considered in combination, 17% of credible patients failed ≥1 of the three cut-offs, while 3% failed two, and only 1% failed all three. In contrast, in the noncredible sample, more than two-thirds failed one or more of the three cut-offs, nearly half failed ≥2, and nearly a quarter failed all three. CONCLUSIONS: RAVLT PVT cut-offs and the RAVLT/RO discriminant function achieve approximately 50% sensitivity, and approach 65% sensitivity when cut-offs specific to samples without histories of learning problems are employed, confirming that RAVLT cut-offs and the RAVLT/RO discriminant function continue to be valuable techniques in the identification of performance invalidity.


Assuntos
Simulação de Doença , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Humanos , Simulação de Doença/psicologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Projetos de Pesquisa , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Sensibilidade e Especificidade
3.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 34(8): 1367-1380, 2019 Nov 27.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30395181

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Evaluate the effectiveness of Rey 15-item plus recognition data in a large neuropsychological sample. METHOD: Rey 15-item plus recognition scores were compared in credible (n = 138) and noncredible (n = 353) neuropsychology referrals. RESULTS: Noncredible patients scored significantly worse than credible patients on all Rey 15-item plus recognition scores. When cut-offs were selected to maintain at least 89.9% specificity, cut-offs could be made more stringent, with the highest sensitivity found for recognition correct (cut-off ≤11; 62.6% sensitivity) and the combination score (recall + recognition - false positives; cut-off ≤22; 60.6% sensitivity), followed by recall correct (cut-off ≤11; 49.3% sensitivity), and recognition false positive errors (≥3; 17.9% sensitivity). A cut-off of ≥4 applied to a summed qualitative error score for the recall trial resulted in 19.4% sensitivity. Approximately 10% of credible subjects failed either recall correct or recognition correct, whereas two-thirds of noncredible patients (67.7%) showed this pattern. Thirteen percent of credible patients failed either recall correct, recognition correct, or the recall qualitative error score, whereas nearly 70% of noncredible patients failed at least one of the three. Some individual qualitative recognition errors had low false positive rates (<2%) indicating that their presence was virtually pathognomonic for noncredible performance. Older age (>50) and IQ < 80 were associated with increased false positive rates in credible patients. CONCLUSIONS: Data on a larger sample than that available in the 2002 validation study show that Rey 15-item plus recognition cut-offs can be made more stringent, and thereby detect up to 70% of noncredible test takers, but the test should be used cautiously in older individuals and in individuals with lowered IQ.


Assuntos
Rememoração Mental , Testes Neuropsicológicos/estatística & dados numéricos , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Reações Falso-Positivas , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Simulação de Doença/diagnóstico , Simulação de Doença/psicologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Desempenho Psicomotor , Valores de Referência , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Sensibilidade e Especificidade , Adulto Jovem
4.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 32(6): 1054-1067, 2018 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29345192

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To cross-validate the Dot Counting Test in a large neuropsychological sample. METHOD: Dot Counting Test scores were compared in credible (n = 142) and non-credible (n = 335) neuropsychology referrals. RESULTS: Non-credible patients scored significantly higher than credible patients on all Dot Counting Test scores. While the original E-score cut-off of ≥17 achieved excellent specificity (96.5%), it was associated with mediocre sensitivity (52.8%). However, the cut-off could be substantially lowered to ≥13.80, while still maintaining adequate specificity (≥90%), and raising sensitivity to 70.0%. Examination of non-credible subgroups revealed that Dot Counting Test sensitivity in feigned mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) was 55.8%, whereas sensitivity was 90.6% in patients with non-credible cognitive dysfunction in the context of claimed psychosis, and 81.0% in patients with non-credible cognitive performance in depression or severe TBI. Thus, the Dot Counting Test may have a particular role in detection of non-credible cognitive symptoms in claimed psychiatric disorders. Alternative to use of the E-score, failure on ≥1 cut-offs applied to individual Dot Counting Test scores (≥6.0″ for mean grouped dot counting time, ≥10.0″ for mean ungrouped dot counting time, and ≥4 errors), occurred in 11.3% of the credible sample, while nearly two-thirds (63.6%) of the non-credible sample failed one of more of these cut-offs. CONCLUSIONS: An E-score cut-off of 13.80, or failure on ≥1 individual score cut-offs, resulted in few false positive identifications in credible patients, and achieved high sensitivity (64.0-70.0%), and therefore appear appropriate for use in identifying neurocognitive performance invalidity.


Assuntos
Transtornos Mentais/diagnóstico , Transtornos Mentais/psicologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos/normas , Encaminhamento e Consulta/normas , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Disfunção Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Disfunção Cognitiva/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Simulação de Doença/diagnóstico , Simulação de Doença/psicologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Adulto Jovem
5.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 32(1): 165-182, 2018 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28585455

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: The current study evaluated MSPQ sensitivity to noncredible PVT performance in the context of external incentive, and examined MSPQ false positive rates in noncompensation-seeking neuropsychology patients; and investigated effects of ethnicity/culture, gender, and somatoform diagnosis on MSPQ scores, and relationships with PVT and MMPI-2-RF data. METHOD: MSPQ scores were compared in credible (n = 110) and noncredible (n = 153) neuropsychology referrals. RESULTS: Noncredible patients scored higher than credible patients. When the credible group was divided into those with somatoform orientation (n = 39) versus those without (n = 71), the credible nonsomatoform group scored lower than the other two groups, who did not differ from each other. MSPQ elevations were found in ethnic minorities, and in individuals who learned English as a second language or concurrently with another language. MSPQ elevations were also associated with chronic systemic diseases, neurologic illness, and substance abuse. Women scored higher than men, but men and women were equally represented among those patients scoring beyond cut-offs. MSPQ scores were minimally related to PVT data but were more strongly correlated with MMPI-2-RF scales, particularly over-report validity scales, RC1, and Somatic/Cognitive scales, with more widespread relationships observed in noncredible patients. CONCLUSIONS: A cut-off of 18 resulted in few false positives in credible nonsomatoform patients, and appears appropriate for identifying physical symptom over-report (due to malingering or somatoform orientations), with associated sensitivity of 29%. However, clinicians are cautioned regarding using the MSPQ in patients with systemic, neurologic, and substance abuse conditions, and in ethnic minorities and non-monolingual English-speakers.


Assuntos
Simulação de Doença/diagnóstico , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Transtornos Somatoformes/diagnóstico , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto , Doença Crônica , Características Culturais , Avaliação da Deficiência , Etnicidade , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Simulação de Doença/etnologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Fatores Sexuais , Transtornos Somatoformes/etnologia
6.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 22(2): 175-86, 2007 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17280813

RESUMO

UNLABELLED: Cognitive impairment is common among patients with end-stage liver disease (ESLD). This study examined cognitive dysfunction in patients with ESLD using the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS). METHOD: 66 patients with ESLD awaiting liver transplant were recruited. Patients were evaluated with the RBANS, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised, and Beck Depression Inventory-II. RESULTS: Patients with ESLD uniformly performed below expectations on all RBANS index scores compared to the healthy normative sample (all p's<.0001) and they also displayed a "subcortical" pattern of cognitive performance (p<.0001). Performances on RBANS attention, language, immediate memory, and total index scores were correlated with education and ethnicity (r's range=|.32-.57|; p's<.01). There was no association between performance on any of the RBANS index scores or subtests and ESLD patient characteristics. In summary, the RBANS appears to adequately characterize known patterns of cognitive dysfunction in ESLD patients.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Encefalopatia Hepática/diagnóstico , Transplante de Fígado/psicologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos/estatística & dados numéricos , Listas de Espera , Adulto , Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Escolaridade , Feminino , Encefalopatia Hepática/psicologia , Humanos , Cirrose Hepática Alcoólica/diagnóstico , Cirrose Hepática Alcoólica/psicologia , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Prognóstico , Psicometria/estatística & dados numéricos , Desempenho Psicomotor , Valores de Referência , Aprendizagem Verbal
7.
Sci STKE ; 2005(284): re6, 2005 May 17.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15900032

RESUMO

Many signaling pathways regulate the activity of effector transcription factors by controlling their subcellular localization. Until recently, the cytoplasmic retention of inactive transcription factors was mainly attributed to binding partners that mask the nuclear localization signals (NLSs) of target proteins. Inactive transcription factors were thought to be exclusively cytoplasmic until their activation, after which the NLSs were unmasked to allow nuclear translocation. There is now a growing body of evidence, however, that challenges this simple model. This review discusses recent reports that suggest that inducible transcription factors can constantly shuttle between the cytoplasm and the nucleus, and that their apparent cytoplasmic retention can be achieved by binding partners that mask the NLSs, tether the transcription factor to cytoplasmic structures, or mark the transcription factor for proteasomal degradation. We also discuss the possibility that this more complex model of cytoplasmic retention might be applicable to a broader range of transcription factors and their associated signaling pathways.


Assuntos
Frações Subcelulares/metabolismo , Fatores de Transcrição/metabolismo , Transcrição Gênica/fisiologia , Animais , Núcleo Celular/metabolismo , Citoplasma/metabolismo , Ácidos Graxos Insaturados/farmacologia , Humanos , Proteínas I-kappa B/metabolismo , Peptídeos e Proteínas de Sinalização Intracelular/fisiologia , Carioferinas/antagonistas & inibidores , Carioferinas/metabolismo , NF-kappa B/metabolismo , Complexo de Endopeptidases do Proteassoma/metabolismo , Transporte Proteico , Receptores Citoplasmáticos e Nucleares/antagonistas & inibidores , Receptores Citoplasmáticos e Nucleares/metabolismo , Transdução de Sinais/fisiologia , Proteínas Smad/fisiologia , Proteína Exportina 1
8.
Stem Cell Rev ; 1(1): 21-7, 2005.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17132871

RESUMO

The long-held concept that transplanted bone marrow (BM)-derived cells contribute only to cells of the hematopoietic system was challenged by data from our laboratory showing that a single male BM-derived cell could not only reconstitute the hematopoietic system of an irradiated female recipient, but could also lead to the generation of mature BM-derived epithelial cells in the liver, lung, skin, and gastrointestinal tract. Careful costaining and single-cell analyses have been used to rule out false positive cells due to inadequate detection techniques in microscopy or cell overlay. Since this initial discovery, we have sought to understand the mechanisms underlying the formation of BM-derived epithelial cells, and to evaluate their therapeutic use for gene therapy and/or tissue regeneration. Several reports have shown that donor BM-derived cells, possibly macrophages, can fuse with existing host epithelial cells to form heterokaryons that express both donor and tissue-specific markers. While this is certainly true for murine tyrosinemia models, we have used a Cre-lox system to demonstrate that fusion is not a requirement for the generation of BM-derived epithelial cells and is likely not responsible for the BM-derived epithelial cells generated after standard BM transplantation. In a proof of principal experiment for potential gene therapy applications, we have shown that autologous BM-derived cells transfected with a transgene prior to BM transplantation are able to develop into mature type-II pneumocytes that express the transgene. We also discuss future research directions in the field and the therapeutic potential of BM-derived epithelia, including ongoing work to test whether combined cell and gene therapy can be used therapeutically in preclinical mouse models of human disease.


Assuntos
Células da Medula Óssea/citologia , Células da Medula Óssea/fisiologia , Células Epiteliais/transplante , Diferenciação Celular , Trato Gastrointestinal/citologia , Humanos , Pulmão/citologia , Masculino , Pele/citologia , Fenômenos Fisiológicos da Pele , Transplante Autólogo , Transplante Homólogo
9.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 28(6): 1030-47, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25157537

RESUMO

Neuropsychologists use performance validity tests (PVTs; Larrabee, 2012 ) to ensure that results of testing are reflective of the test taker's true neurocognitive ability, and their use is recommended in all compensation-seeking settings. However, whether the type of compensation context (e.g., personal injury litigation versus disability seeking) impacts the nature and extent of neurocognitive symptom feigning has not been adequately investigated. PVT performance was compared in an archival data set of noncredible individuals in either a personal injury litigation (n = 163) or a disability-seeking context (n = 201). Individuals were deemed noncredible based on meeting Slick, Sherman, and Iverson's ( 1999 ) criteria including failure on at least two PVTs and a lack of congruency between their low cognitive scores and normal function in activities of daily living (ADLs). In general, disability seekers tended to perform in a less sophisticated manner than did litigants (i.e., they failed more indicators and did so more extensively). Upon further investigation, these differences were in part accounted for by type of diagnoses feigned; those seeking compensation for mental health diagnoses were more likely to feign or exaggerate a wide variety of cognitive deficits, whereas those with claimed medical diagnoses (i.e., traumatic brain injury) were more targeted in their attempts to feign and/or exaggerate neurocognitive compromise.


Assuntos
Avaliação da Deficiência , Pessoas com Deficiência/psicologia , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Testes Neuropsicológicos/normas , Aceitação pelo Paciente de Cuidados de Saúde/psicologia , Ferimentos e Lesões/psicologia , Atividades Cotidianas/classificação , Adulto , Lesões Encefálicas/psicologia , Compensação e Reparação , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Simulação de Doença , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
10.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 28(6): 1048-70, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24985490

RESUMO

The purpose of this archival study was to identify performance validity tests (PVTs) and standard IQ and neurocognitive test scores, which singly or in combination, differentiate credible patients of low IQ (FSIQ ≤ 75; n = 55) from non-credible patients. We compared the credible participants against a sample of 74 non-credible patients who appeared to have been attempting to feign low intelligence specifically (FSIQ ≤ 75), as well as a larger non-credible sample (n = 383) unselected for IQ. The entire non-credible group scored significantly higher than the credible participants on measures of verbal crystallized intelligence/semantic memory and manipulation of overlearned information, while the credible group performed significantly better on many processing speed and memory tests. Additionally, credible women showed faster finger-tapping speeds than non-credible women. The credible group also scored significantly higher than the non-credible subgroup with low IQ scores on measures of attention, visual perceptual/spatial tasks, processing speed, verbal learning/list learning, and visual memory, and credible women continued to outperform non-credible women on finger tapping. When cut-offs were selected to maintain approximately 90% specificity in the credible group, sensitivity rates were highest for verbal and visual memory measures (i.e., TOMM trials 1 and 2; Warrington Words correct and time; Rey Word Recognition Test total; RAVLT Effort Equation, Trial 5, total across learning trials, short delay, recognition, and RAVLT/RO discriminant function; and Digit Symbol recognition), followed by select attentional PVT scores (i.e., b Test omissions and time to recite four digits forward). When failure rates were tabulated across seven most sensitive scores, a cut-off of ≥ 2 failures was associated with 85.4% specificity and 85.7% sensitivity, while a cut-off of ≥ 3 failures resulted in 95.1% specificity and 66.0% sensitivity. Results are discussed in light of extant literature and directions for future research.


Assuntos
Atenção , Inteligência , Simulação de Doença/diagnóstico , Testes Neuropsicológicos/normas , Desempenho Psicomotor , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Psicometria , Sensibilidade e Especificidade , Aprendizagem Verbal
12.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 27(3): 495-508, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23157695

RESUMO

The b Test (Boone, Lu, & Herzberg, 2002a) is a measure of cognitive performance validity originally validated on 91 non-credible participants and 7 credible clinical comparison groups (total n = 161). The purpose of the current study was to provide cross-validation data for the b Test on a known groups sample of non-credible participants (n = 212) and credible heterogeneous neuropsychological clinic patients (n = 103). The new data showed that while the original E-score cut-off of ≥ 155 achieved excellent specificity (99%), it was associated with relatively poor sensitivity (41%). However, the cut-off could be substantially lowered to ≥82, while still maintaining adequate specificity (≥90%) and raising sensitivity to 68%. Examination of non-credible subgroups revealed that b Test sensitivity in feigned mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) was 58%, whereas in non-credible patients claiming depression and psychosis, cut-off sensitivity was 76% and 67%, respectively. These data suggest that the b Test may have a particular role in detection of non-credible cognitive symptoms associated with feigned psychiatric symptoms, and that fabricated deficits in processing speed and vigilance/visual scanning, detected by the b Test, are more prominent in feigned psychiatric presentations than in feigned mTBI. Further, b Test failures in patients with somatoform disorders were common, indicating that the b Test may have a specific use in detection of non-consciously created cognitive dysfunction associated with somatoform conditions.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Simulação de Doença/diagnóstico , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Transtornos Cognitivos/etiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Simulação de Doença/psicologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Transtornos do Humor/complicações , Transtornos do Humor/psicologia , Doenças do Sistema Nervoso/complicações , Valores de Referência , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Estudos Retrospectivos , Sensibilidade e Especificidade , Adulto Jovem
13.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 27(3): 516-27, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23216255

RESUMO

The Rey Word Recognition Test, a brief and simple to administer free-standing neurocognitive performance validity test, was examined in a large known-groups sample (122 credible patients and 134 non-credible patients). Total correctly recognized was the most sensitive score, identifying 54% of non-credible participants using a cut-off of ≤6, while maintaining specificity of approximately 90%. However, specifically rates were somewhat lower in credible individuals with <12 years of education or borderline intelligence, or who were bilingual (spoke English as a second language, or learned English concurrently with another language), indicating that cut-offs may require minor adjustment in these groups. Sensitivity rates were much higher in non-credible female versus male mild traumatic brain injury patients (mTBI; 68% versus 48% for total correct), suggesting that the Rey Word Recognition Test is particularly effective in identifying performance invalidity in female mTBI compensation seekers.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Simulação de Doença/diagnóstico , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Vocabulário , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Simulação de Doença/psicologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Sensibilidade e Especificidade , Aprendizagem Verbal/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
14.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 28(1): 30-7, 2013 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23232864

RESUMO

A Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCFT) equation incorporating copy and recognition was found to be useful in detecting negative response bias in neuropsychological assessments (ROCFT Effort Equation; Lu, P. H., Boone, K. B., Cozolino, L., & Mitchell, C. (2003). Effectiveness of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test and the Meyers and Meyers recognition trial in the detection of suspect effort. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17, 426-440). In the current cross validation of this validity, the credible patient group (n = 146; 124 with equation data) outperformed the noncredible group (n = 157; 115 with equation data) on copy, 3-min recall, total recognition correct and the Effort Equation, but the latter was most effective in classifying subjects. A cut-off of ≤50 maintained specificity of 90% and achieved sensitivity of 80%. Results of the current cross validation provide corroboration that the ROCFT Effort Equation is an effective measure of neurocognitive response bias.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Simulação de Doença/diagnóstico , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Inteligência , Masculino , Simulação de Doença/psicologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Desempenho Psicomotor , Valores de Referência , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Adulto Jovem
15.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 27(6): 1060-76, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23742292

RESUMO

Practice guidelines recommend the use of multiple performance validity tests (PVTs) to detect noncredible performance during neuropsychological evaluations, and PVTs embedded in standard cognitive tests achieve this goal most efficiently. The present study examined the utility of the Comalli version of the Stroop Test as a measure of response bias in a large sample of "real world" noncredible patients (n = 129) as compared with credible neuropsychology clinic patients (n=233). The credible group performed significantly better than the noncredible group on all trials, but particularly on word-reading (Stroop A) and color-naming (Stroop B); cut-scores for Stroop A and Stroop B trials were associated with moderate sensitivity (49-53%) as compared to the low sensitivity found for the color interference trial (29%). Some types of diagnoses (including learning disability, severe traumatic brain injury, psychosis, and depression), very advanced age (⩾80), and lowered IQ were associated with increased rates of false positive identifications, suggesting the need for some adjustments to cut-offs in these subgroups. Despite some previous reports of an inverted Stroop effect (i.e., color-naming worse than color interference) in noncredible subjects, individual Stroop word reading and color naming trials were much more effective in identifying response bias.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores , Teste de Stroop , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Sensibilidade e Especificidade
16.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 24(7): 1243-56, 2010 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20924983

RESUMO

In the present study a large sample of credible patients (n = 172) scored significantly higher than a large sample of noncredible participants (n = 195) on several WAIS-III Picture Completion variables: Age Adjusted Scaled Score, raw score, a "Rarely Missed" index (the nine items least often missed by credible participants), a "Rarely Correct" index (nine items correct <26% of the time in noncredible participants and with at least a 25 percentage-point lower endorsement rate as compared to credible participants), and a "Most Discrepant" index (the six items that were the most discrepant in correct endorsement between groups-at least a 40 percentage point difference). Comparison of the various scores showed that the "Most Discrepant" index outperformed all the others in identifying response bias (nearly 65% sensitivity at 92.8% specificity as compared to at most 59% sensitivity for the other scores). While no differences in Picture Completion scores were observed between less-educated (<12 years) and better-educated (≥12 years) credible participants, noncredible participants with <12 years of education scored significantly poorer than noncredible participants with 12 or more years of education. On the "Most Discrepant" index, 76.7% of less-educated noncredible participants were detected as compared to 58.3% of better-educated noncredible participants. Results of the current study suggest that the Picture Completion subtest of the WAIS-III is an effective measure of response bias, and that it may have a unique role in identifying suboptimal effort in less-educated test takers.


Assuntos
Viés , Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Escalas de Wechsler , Pesos e Medidas , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Escolaridade , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Desempenho Psicomotor , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Sensibilidade e Especificidade , Estatística como Assunto , Adulto Jovem
17.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 25(1): 60-70, 2010 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19906738

RESUMO

Several studies have examined the usefulness of the Warrington Recognition Memory Test-Words as a measure to detect suspect effort, although samples have generally been small and/or comprised of simulators rather than "real world" credible and noncredible patients. The current study examined the Warrington Recognition Memory Test-Words total score and response time of "real world" noncredible patients (as determined by motive to feign, failure on > or =2 independent measures of response bias, low cognitive scores inconsistent with normal ADLs; n = 190) versus credible patients (as determined by no motive to feign, failure of < or =1 measure of response bias; n = 124) derived from an archival database of individuals from the Harbor-UCLA Medical Center, Department of Psychiatry, Outpatient Neuropsychology Service, and the private practice of the second author. Noncredible patients obtained significantly lower total scores and longer times to complete the task. A total correct cutoff of < or =42 was found to have excellent specificity (91.9%) and sensitivity (88.9%), whereas a time cutoff of > or =207'' was associated with 65.5% sensitivity at 90.7% specificity, and when the time cut-score was used in combination with the total score cutoff, an additional 5% of the noncredible participants were captured, raising overall sensitivity to 93.7% (at 87.1% specificity). Thus, the Warrington Recognition Memory Test-Words, although not originally created for the purposes of measuring suspect effort, appears to be an excellent measure for detecting response bias on neuropsychological testing.


Assuntos
Viés , Transtornos Cognitivos/fisiopatologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Vocabulário , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Simulação de Doença/diagnóstico , Simulação de Doença/psicologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Sensibilidade e Especificidade , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
18.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 24(2): 344-57, 2010 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19921593

RESUMO

The assessment of response validity during neuropsychological evaluation is an integral part of the testing process. Research has increasingly focused on the use of "embedded" effort measures (derived from standard neuropsychological tasks) because they do not require additional administration time and are less likely to be identified as effort indicators by test takers because of their primary focus as measures of cognitive function. The current study examined the clinical utility of various WMS-III Logical Memory scores in detecting response bias, as well as the Rarely Missed Index, an embedded effort indicator derived from the WMS-III Logical Memory Delayed Recognition subtest. The Rarely Missed Index cut-off only identified 24.1% of 63 non-credible participants (at >/=90% specificity in 125 credible patients), and cut-offs for other Logical Memory variables were in fact found to be more sensitive to non-credible performance. A new indicator, consisting of the weighted combination of the two most sensitive Logical Memory subtest scores (Logical Memory II raw score and Logical Memory Delayed Recognition raw score), was associated with 53% to 60% sensitivity, and thus may be an effective adjunct when utilized in conjunction with other validated effort indicators and collateral information in identifying non-credible performance.


Assuntos
Lógica , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Índice de Gravidade de Doença , Adulto Jovem
19.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 23(2): 297-313, 2009 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18821138

RESUMO

While it is recommended that judgments regarding the credibility of test performance be based on the results of more than one effort indicator, and recent efforts have been made to improve interpretation of multiple effort test failure, the field currently lacks adequate guidelines for using multiple measures of effort in concert with one another. A total of 103 patients were referred for outpatient neuropsychological evaluation, which included multiple measures of negative response bias embedded in standard test batteries. Using any pairwise failure combination to predict diagnostic classification was superior (sensitivity = 83.8%, specificity = 93.9%, overall hit rate = 90.3%) to using any one test by itself and to using any three-test failure combination. Further, the results were comparable to the results of logistical regression analyses using the embedded indicators as continuous predictors. Given its parsimony and clinical utility, the pairwise failure model is therefore a recommended criterion for identifying non-credible performance; however, there are of course other important contextual factors and influences to consider, which are also discussed.


Assuntos
Transtornos Mentais/psicologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Psicometria/métodos , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos Mentais/diagnóstico , Transtornos Mentais/fisiopatologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Pacientes Ambulatoriais , Desempenho Psicomotor , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
20.
Stem Cells ; 24(10): 2299-308, 2006 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16794262

RESUMO

Bone marrow-derived cells (BMDCs) can engraft as epithelial cells throughout the body, including in the lung, liver, and gastrointestinal (GI) tract following transplantation into lethally irradiated adult recipients. Except for rare disease models in which marrow-derived epithelial cells have a survival advantage over endogenous cells, the currently attained levels of epithelial engraftment of BMDCs are too low to be of therapeutic benefit. Here we tested whether the degree of bone marrow to epithelial engraftment would be higher if bone marrow transplantation (BMT) were performed on 1-day-old mice, when tissues are undergoing rapid growth and remodeling. BMT into newborn mice after multiple different regimens allowed for robust hematopoietic engraftment, as well as the development of rare donor-derived epithelial cells in the GI tract and lung but not in the liver. The highest epithelial engraftment (0.02%) was obtained in mice that received a preparative regimen of two doses of busulfan in utero. When BMDCs were transplanted into myelosuppressed newborn mice that lacked expression of the cystic fibrosis transmembrane conductance regulator (CFTR) protein, the chloride channel that is not functional in patients with cystic fibrosis, the engrafted mice showed partial restoration of CFTR channel activity, suggesting that marrow-derived epithelial cells in the GI tract were functional. However, BMT into newborn mice, regardless of the myeloablative regimen used, did not increase the number of bone marrow-derived epithelial cells over that which occurs after BMT into lethally irradiated adult mice.


Assuntos
Transplante de Medula Óssea/métodos , Regulador de Condutância Transmembrana em Fibrose Cística/genética , Células Epiteliais/citologia , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos , Regulador de Condutância Transmembrana em Fibrose Cística/metabolismo , Células Epiteliais/metabolismo , Feminino , Imunofluorescência/métodos , Transplante de Células-Tronco Hematopoéticas/métodos , Hibridização in Situ Fluorescente , Masculino , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Camundongos Endogâmicos , Camundongos Transgênicos , RNA Mensageiro/genética , RNA Mensageiro/metabolismo , Cromossomo Y/genética
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA