Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 40
Filter
1.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 153(2): 761, 2023 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36859129

ABSTRACT

For over 50 years, Richard R. (Dick) Fay made major contributions to our understanding of vertebrate hearing. Much of Dick's work focused on hearing in fishes and, particularly, goldfish, as well as a few other species, in a substantial body of work on sound localization mechanisms. However, Dick's focus was always on using his studies to try and understand bigger issues of vertebrate hearing and its evolution. This article is slightly adapted from an article that Dick wrote in 2010 on the closure of the Parmly Hearing Institute at Loyola University Chicago. Except for small modifications and minor updates, the words and ideas herein are those of Dick.


Subject(s)
Hearing , Sound Localization , Writing
2.
Adv Exp Med Biol ; 877: 31-51, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26515309

ABSTRACT

This paper discusses the 43+ year collaboration of Arthur Popper and Richard Fay. Over these years, we have co-authored over 30 papers and 55 books. The collaboration benefits from a strong friendship that includes our spouses and children. By any measure, our collaboration must be seen as being successful. The basis for this success is, we think, twofold. First, we have very complementary and overlapping research interests. This has enabled us to tackle issues, whether in research or in planning meetings or books, from different perspectives. Second, a hallmark of our successful collaboration has been our deep and close friendship and the extension of that friendship to our spouses and children. In this paper, we discuss some of the events that have shaped our collaboration, and some of the people who have impacted our lives.


Subject(s)
Cooperative Behavior , Fishes/physiology , Friends , Hearing/physiology , Animals , Fishes/classification , Hawaii , Humans , Publications , Research
3.
Adv Exp Med Biol ; 875: 1049-56, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26611067

ABSTRACT

Auditory evoked potentials (AEPs) have become popular for estimating hearing thresholds and audiograms. What is the utility of these measurements? How do AEP audiograms compare with behavioral audiograms? In general, AEP measurements for fishes and marine mammals often underestimate behavioral thresholds, but comparisons are especially complicated when the AEP and behavioral measures are obtained under different acoustic conditions. There is no single representative relationship between AEP and behavioral audiograms and these audiograms should not be considered equivalent. We suggest that the most valuable comparisons are those made by the same researcher using similar acoustic conditions for both measurements.


Subject(s)
Aquatic Organisms/physiology , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Auditory/physiology , Sound Spectrography , Animals , Auditory Threshold/physiology , Hearing/physiology
4.
Adv Exp Med Biol ; 875: 363-9, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26610980

ABSTRACT

In this paper, we discuss the issues encountered when trying to perform hearing experiments in water-filled tanks that are several meters in lateral extent, typically large in terms of the size of the animals under study but not necessarily so with respect to the wavelengths of interest. This paper presents measurements of pressure and particle motion fields in these "large" tanks. The observed characteristics and complexities are discussed in reference to their potential impact on the planning and interpretation of hearing experiments.


Subject(s)
Acoustics , Fisheries , Animals , Fishes , Pressure
5.
Adv Exp Med Biol ; 875: 933-41, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26611052

ABSTRACT

Researchers often perform hearing studies on fish in small tanks. The acoustic field in such a tank is considerably different from the acoustic field that occurs in the animal's natural environment. The significance of these differences is magnified by the nature of the fish's auditory system where either acoustic pressure (a scalar), acoustic particle velocity (a vector), or both may serve as the stimulus. It is essential for the underwater acoustician to understand the acoustics of small tanks to be able to carry out valid auditory research in the laboratory and to properly compare and interpret the results of others.


Subject(s)
Acoustics , Fisheries , Animals , Auditory Threshold/physiology , Hearing/physiology , Models, Theoretical , Pressure , Salmo salar/physiology
6.
J Exp Biol ; 217(Pt 12): 2078-88, 2014 Jun 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24675557

ABSTRACT

We investigated the roles of the swim bladder and the lateral line system in sound localization behavior by the plainfin midshipman fish (Porichthys notatus). Reproductive female midshipman underwent either surgical deflation of the swim bladder or cryoablation of the lateral line and were then tested in a monopolar sound source localization task. Fish with nominally 'deflated' swim bladders performed similar to sham-deflated controls; however, post-experiment evaluation of swim bladder deflation revealed that a majority of 'deflated' fish (88%, seven of the eight fish) that exhibited positive phonotaxis had partially inflated swim bladders. In total, 95% (21/22) of fish that localized the source had at least partially inflated swim bladders, indicating that pressure reception is likely required for sound source localization. In lateral line experiments, no difference was observed in the proportion of females exhibiting positive phonotaxis with ablated (37%) versus sham-ablated (47%) lateral line systems. These data suggest that the lateral line system is likely not required for sound source localization, although this system may be important for fine-tuning the approach to the sound source. We found that midshipman can solve the 180 deg ambiguity of source direction in the shallow water of our test tank, which is similar to their nesting environment. We also found that the potential directional cues (phase relationship between pressure and particle motion) in shallow water differs from a theoretical free-field. Therefore, the general question of how fish use acoustic pressure cues to solve the 180 deg ambiguity of source direction from the particle motion vector remains unresolved.


Subject(s)
Air Sacs/physiology , Batrachoidiformes/physiology , Lateral Line System/physiology , Sound Localization , Animals , California , Cues , Female , Motion , Pressure
8.
JAMA Intern Med ; 184(5): 484-492, 2024 May 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38466302

ABSTRACT

Importance: Chronic kidney disease (CKD) affects 37 million adults in the United States, and for patients with CKD, hypertension is a key risk factor for adverse outcomes, such as kidney failure, cardiovascular events, and death. Objective: To evaluate a computerized clinical decision support (CDS) system for the management of uncontrolled hypertension in patients with CKD. Design, Setting, and Participants: This multiclinic, randomized clinical trial randomized primary care practitioners (PCPs) at a primary care network, including 15 hospital-based, ambulatory, and community health center-based clinics, through a stratified, matched-pair randomization approach February 2021 to February 2022. All adult patients with a visit to a PCP in the last 2 years were eligible and those with evidence of CKD and hypertension were included. Intervention: The intervention consisted of a CDS system based on behavioral economic principles and human-centered design methods that delivered tailored, evidence-based recommendations, including initiation or titration of renin-angiotensin-aldosterone system inhibitors. The patients in the control group received usual care from PCPs with the CDS system operating in silent mode. Main Outcomes and Measures: The primary outcome was the change in mean systolic blood pressure (SBP) between baseline and 180 days compared between groups. The primary analysis was a repeated measures linear mixed model, using SBP at baseline, 90 days, and 180 days in an intention-to-treat repeated measures model to account for missing data. Secondary outcomes included blood pressure (BP) control and outcomes such as percentage of patients who received an action that aligned with the CDS recommendations. Results: The study included 174 PCPs and 2026 patients (mean [SD] age, 75.3 [0.3] years; 1223 [60.4%] female; mean [SD] SBP at baseline, 154.0 [14.3] mm Hg), with 87 PCPs and 1029 patients randomized to the intervention and 87 PCPs and 997 patients randomized to usual care. Overall, 1714 patients (84.6%) were treated for hypertension at baseline. There were 1623 patients (80.1%) with an SBP measurement at 180 days. From the linear mixed model, there was a statistically significant difference in mean SBP change in the intervention group compared with the usual care group (change, -14.6 [95% CI, -13.1 to -16.0] mm Hg vs -11.7 [-10.2 to -13.1] mm Hg; P = .005). There was no difference in the percentage of patients who achieved BP control in the intervention group compared with the control group (50.4% [95% CI, 46.5% to 54.3%] vs 47.1% [95% CI, 43.3% to 51.0%]). More patients received an action aligned with the CDS recommendations in the intervention group than in the usual care group (49.9% [95% CI, 45.1% to 54.8%] vs 34.6% [95% CI, 29.8% to 39.4%]; P < .001). Conclusions and Relevance: These findings suggest that implementing this computerized CDS system could lead to improved management of uncontrolled hypertension and potentially improved clinical outcomes at the population level for patients with CKD. Trial Registration: ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT03679247.


Subject(s)
Antihypertensive Agents , Decision Support Systems, Clinical , Hypertension , Renal Insufficiency, Chronic , Humans , Female , Male , Hypertension/drug therapy , Hypertension/complications , Renal Insufficiency, Chronic/complications , Renal Insufficiency, Chronic/therapy , Antihypertensive Agents/therapeutic use , Aged , Middle Aged , Primary Health Care/methods
9.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23411503

ABSTRACT

Previous work on auditory processing in Opsanus tau has focused on the descending octaval nucleus; however, the magnocellular octaval nucleus receives similar inputs from the otolithic endorgans. The purpose of this study was to assess whether cells in any of the three subdivisions of the magnocellular nucleus respond to auditory frequencies and encode sound source direction. Extracellular recording sites were chosen based on anatomical landmarks, and neurobiotin injections confirmed the location of auditory sites in subdivisions of the magnocellular nucleus. In general, the auditory cells in M2 and M3 responded best to frequencies at or below 100 Hz. Most auditory cells responded well to directional stimuli presented along axes in the horizontal plane. Cells in M3 (not M2) also responded to lateral line stimulation, consistent with otolithic endorgan and lateral line inputs to M3. The convergence of auditory and lateral line inputs in M3, the lack of Mauthner cells in this species, and previous evidence that the magnocellular nucleus does not contribute to ascending auditory pathways suggest to us that the large cells of M3 may play a role in rapid behavioral responses to particle motion stimuli in oyster toadfish.


Subject(s)
Auditory Pathways/physiology , Auditory Perception , Batrachoidiformes/physiology , Brain/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Animals , Biotin/administration & dosage , Biotin/analogs & derivatives , Female , Injections , Male , Neuroanatomical Tract-Tracing Techniques , Neuronal Tract-Tracers/administration & dosage , Sound Localization
10.
BMJ Open ; 13(12): e077520, 2023 12 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38135330

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: There is an urgent need for scalable strategies for treating overweight and obesity in clinical settings. PROPS 2.0 (Partnerships for Reducing Overweight and Obesity with Patient-Centered Strategies 2.0) aims to adapt and implement the combined intervention from the PROPS Study at scale, in a diverse cross-section of patients and providers. METHODS AND ANALYSIS: We are implementing PROPS 2.0 across a variety of clinics at Brigham and Women's Hospital, targeting enrolment of 5000 patients. Providers can refer patients or patients can self-refer. Eligible patients must be ≥20 years old and have a body mass index (BMI) of ≥30 kg/m2 or a BMI of 25-29.9 kg/m2 plus another cardiovascular risk factor or obesity-related condition. After enrolment, patients register for the RestoreHealth online programme/app (HealthFleet Inc.) and participate for 12 months. Patients can engage with the programme and receive personalized feedback from a coach. Patient navigators help to enrol patients, enter updates in the electronic health record, and refer patients to additional resources. The RE-AIM (Reach, Effectiveness, Adoption, Implementation, and Maintenance) framework is guiding the evaluation. ETHICS AND DISSEMINATION: The Mass General Brigham Human Research Committee approved this protocol. An implementation guide will be created and disseminated, to help other sites adopt the intervention in the future. TRIAL REGISTRATION NUMBER: NCT0555925.


Subject(s)
Overweight , Weight Reduction Programs , Adult , Female , Humans , Young Adult , Body Mass Index , Obesity/prevention & control , Overweight/prevention & control , Patient-Centered Care , Weight Reduction Programs/methods
11.
J Neurophysiol ; 107(2): 658-65, 2012 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22031776

ABSTRACT

The lake sturgeon, Acipenser fulvescens, belongs to one of the few extant nonteleost ray-finned fishes and diverged from the main vertebrate lineage about 250 million years ago. The aim of this study was to use this species to explore the peripheral neural coding strategies for sound direction and compare these results to modern bony fishes (teleosts). Extracellular recordings were made from afferent neurons innervating the saccule and lagena of the inner ear while the fish was stimulated using a shaker system. Afferents were highly directional and strongly phase locked to the stimulus. Directional response profiles resembled cosine functions, and directional preferences occurred at a wide range of stimulus intensities (spanning at least 60 dB re 1 nm displacement). Seventy-six percent of afferents were directionally selective for stimuli in the vertical plane near 90° (up down) and did not respond to horizontal stimulation. Sixty-two percent of afferents responsive to horizontal stimulation had their best axis in azimuths near 0° (front back). These findings suggest that in the lake sturgeon, in contrast to teleosts, the saccule and lagena may convey more limited information about the direction of a sound source, raising the possibility that this species uses a different mechanism for localizing sound. For azimuth, a mechanism could involve the utricle or perhaps the computation of arrival time differences. For elevation, behavioral strategies such as directing the head to maximize input to the area of best sensitivity may be used. Alternatively, the lake sturgeon may have a more limited ability for sound source localization compared with teleosts.


Subject(s)
Fishes/physiology , Hair Cells, Auditory/physiology , Peripheral Nerves/physiology , Saccule and Utricle/cytology , Signal Detection, Psychological/physiology , Sound Localization/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Action Potentials/physiology , Animals , Auditory Pathways/physiology , Orientation , Reaction Time , Space Perception , Time Factors
12.
Proc Biol Sci ; 279(1739): 2816-24, 2012 Jul 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22438494

ABSTRACT

Turtles, like other amphibious animals, face a trade-off between terrestrial and aquatic hearing. We used laser vibrometry and auditory brainstem responses to measure their sensitivity to vibration stimuli and to airborne versus underwater sound. Turtles are most sensitive to sound underwater, and their sensitivity depends on the large middle ear, which has a compliant tympanic disc attached to the columella. Behind the disc, the middle ear is a large air-filled cavity with a volume of approximately 0.5 ml and a resonance frequency of approximately 500 Hz underwater. Laser vibrometry measurements underwater showed peak vibrations at 500-600 Hz with a maximum of 300 µm s(-1) Pa(-1), approximately 100 times more than the surrounding water. In air, the auditory brainstem response audiogram showed a best sensitivity to sound of 300-500 Hz. Audiograms before and after removing the skin covering reveal that the cartilaginous tympanic disc shows unchanged sensitivity, indicating that the tympanic disc, and not the overlying skin, is the key sound receiver. If air and water thresholds are compared in terms of sound intensity, thresholds in water are approximately 20-30 dB lower than in air. Therefore, this tympanic ear is specialized for underwater hearing, most probably because sound-induced pulsations of the air in the middle ear cavity drive the tympanic disc.


Subject(s)
Ear, Middle/anatomy & histology , Ear, Middle/physiology , Hearing/physiology , Turtles/anatomy & histology , Turtles/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Animals , Auditory Threshold , Evoked Potentials, Auditory, Brain Stem/physiology , Models, Biological , Water
13.
J Exp Biol ; 215(Pt 1): 152-60, 2012 Jan 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22162863

ABSTRACT

Sound-source localization behavior was studied in the plainfin midshipman fish (Porichthys notatus) by making use of the naturally occurring phonotaxis response of gravid females to playback of the male's advertisement call. The observations took place outdoors in a circular concrete tank. A dipole sound projector was placed at the center of the tank and an 80-90 Hz tone (the approximate fundamental frequency to the male's advertisement call) was broadcast to gravid females that were released from alternative sites approximately 100 cm from the source. The phonotaxic responses of females to the source were recorded, analyzed and compared with the sound field. One release site was approximately along the vibratory axis of the dipole source, and the other was approximately orthogonal to the vibratory axis. The sound field in the tank was fully characterized through measurements of the sound pressure field using hydrophones and acoustic particle motion using an accelerometer. These measurements confirmed that the sound field was a nearly ideal dipole. When released along the dipole vibratory axis, the responding female fish took essentially straight paths to the source. However, when released approximately 90 deg to the source's vibratory axis, the responding females took highly curved paths to the source that were approximately in line with the local particle motion axes. These results indicate that the acoustic cues used by fish during sound-source localization include the axes of particle motion of the local sound field.


Subject(s)
Batrachoidiformes/physiology , Sound Localization , Acoustics , Animals , Cues , Female , Hearing , Male
14.
Transcult Psychiatry ; 59(4): 413-424, 2022 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35300551

ABSTRACT

Communication about well-being and distress involves multiple stakeholders, including experts by experience (EBE), researchers, clinical practitioners, interpreters, and translators. Communication can involve a variety of discourses and languages and each of the stakeholders may employ diverging epistemologies to understand and explain experiences. These epistemologies may link to different sources of authority and be articulated using particular linguistic resources. Epistemic injustice can occur when stakeholders, intentionally or unintentionally, fail to recognise the validity of other stakeholders' ways of conceptualising and verbalising their experience of well-being and distress. Language lies at the heart of the risk of epistemic injustice involved in the process of expressing well-being and distress as seen in: 1) the interface between divergent discourses on well-being and distress (e.g., biomedical vs. spiritual); and 2) communications involving multiple linguistic resources, which can be subdivided into multi-language communications involving a) translation of assessment measures, and b) interpreted interactions. Some of the challenges of multi-language communication can be addressed by translators or interpreters who strive for conceptual equivalence. We argue, however, that all stakeholders have an important role as "epistemic brokers" in the languaging of possible epistemological differences. Effective epistemic brokering requires that all stakeholders are reflexively and critically aware of the risks of epistemic injustice inherent in multi-language communication. The article concludes with a set of prompts to help raise stakeholder awareness and reflexivity when engaging in communication about well-being and distress.


Subject(s)
Knowledge , Mental Healing , Morals , Psychological Distress , Communication , Humans , Language
15.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 129(5): 3367-72, 2011 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21568437

ABSTRACT

The masking effects of white and amplitude comodulated noise were studied with respect to simple signal detection and sound source determination in goldfish. A stimulus generalization method was used to determine the signal-to-noise ratio required to completely determine the signal's characteristics. It was found that the S∕N required for this determination is about 4 dB greater than that required for signal detection, or was about 4 dB greater than the critical masking ratio. This means that the potential harm to fish of a given masking noise is at least 4 dB greater than previously thought, based on critical masking ratios. However, for amplitude comodulated noise between 10 and 50 Hz modulation rate, the potential harmful effects are up to 5.3 dB less than would be predicted from the critical masking ratio for unmodulated noise.


Subject(s)
Goldfish/physiology , Noise/adverse effects , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Air Sacs/physiology , Animals , Auditory Threshold , Conditioning, Classical , Differential Threshold , Ear Ossicles/physiology , Electroshock , Goldfish/anatomy & histology , Pressure , Random Allocation , Respiration , Signal Detection, Psychological/physiology , Species Specificity
16.
J Exp Biol ; 213(Pt 9): 1567-78, 2010 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20400642

ABSTRACT

Acipenser fulvescens, the lake sturgeon, belongs to one of the few extant non-teleost ray-finned (bony) fishes. The sturgeons (family Acipenseridae) have a phylogenetic history that dates back about 250 million years. The study reported here is the first investigation of peripheral coding strategies for spectral analysis in the auditory system in a non-teleost bony fish. We used a shaker system to simulate the particle motion component of sound during electrophysiological recordings of isolated single units from the eighth nerve innervating the saccule and lagena. Background activity and response characteristics of saccular and lagenar afferents (such as thresholds, response-level functions and temporal firing) resembled the ones found in teleosts. The distribution of best frequencies also resembled data in teleosts (except for Carassius auratus, goldfish) tested with the same stimulation method. The saccule and lagena in A. fulvescens contain otoconia, in contrast to the solid otoliths found in teleosts, however, this difference in otolith structure did not appear to affect threshold, frequency tuning, intensity- or temporal responses of auditory afferents. In general, the physiological characteristics common to A. fulvescens, teleosts and land vertebrates reflect important functions of the auditory system that may have been conserved throughout the evolution of vertebrates.


Subject(s)
Fishes/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Animals , Hearing , Saccule and Utricle/physiology
17.
J Exp Biol ; 213(Pt 20): 3536-47, 2010 Oct 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20889834

ABSTRACT

In goldfish and other otophysans, the Weberian ossicles mechanically link the saccule of the inner ear to the anterior swimbladder chamber (ASB). These structures are correlated with enhanced sound-pressure sensitivity and greater sensitivity at high frequencies (600-2000 Hz). However, surprisingly little is known about the potential impact of the ASB on other otolithic organs and about how auditory responses are modulated by discrete sources that change their location or orientation with respect to the ASB. In this study, saccular and lagenar nerve fiber responses and conditioned behaviors of goldfish were measured to a small, low-frequency (50 Hz) vibrating sphere (dipole) source as a function of its location along the body and its orientation with respect to the ASB. Conditioned behaviors and saccular nerve fiber activity exhibited response characteristics nearly identical to those measured from a hydrophone in the same relative position as the ASB. By contrast, response patterns from lagena fibers could not be predicted by pressure inputs to the ASB. Deflation of the ASB abolished the characteristic spatial response pattern of saccular but not lagena fibers. These results show that: (1) the lagena is not driven by ASB-mediated pressure inputs to the ear; (2) the ASB-saccule pathway dominates behavioral responsiveness, operating effectively at frequencies as low as 50 Hz; and (3) behavioral and neural (saccular) responses are strongly modulated by the position and orientation of the dipole with respect to the ASB.


Subject(s)
Acoustic Stimulation , Auditory Pathways/physiology , Goldfish/physiology , Air Sacs/physiology , Animals , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Conditioning, Psychological/physiology , Models, Biological , Nervous System Physiological Phenomena , Pressure , Saccule and Utricle/physiology
18.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 127(5): 3104-13, 2010 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21117759

ABSTRACT

The aim of this study was to use plainfin midshipman fish (Porichthys notatus) as a general model to explore how fishes localize an underwater sound source in the relatively simple geometry of a monopole sound field. The robust phonotaxic responses displayed by gravid females toward a monopole sound projector (J-9) broadcasting a low-frequency (90 Hz) tone similar to the fundamental frequency of the male's advertisement call were examined. The projector's sound field was mapped at 5 cm resolution azimuth using an eight-hydrophone array. Acoustic pressure was measured with the array and acoustic particle motion was calculated from pressure gradients between hydrophones. The response pathways of the fish were analyzed from video recordings and compared to the sound field. Gravid females at initial release were directed toward the sound source, and the majority (73%) swam to the playback projector with straight to slightly curved tracks in the direction of the source and in line with local particle motion vectors. In contrast, the initial direction of the control (sound-off) group did not differ from random. This paper reports on a comparison of fish localization behavior with directional cues available in the form of local particle motion vectors.


Subject(s)
Batrachoidiformes/physiology , Cues , Sound Localization , Vocalization, Animal , Acoustic Stimulation , Acoustics/instrumentation , Animals , Female , Male , Motion , Pressure , Sound , Sound Spectrography , Transducers, Pressure , Vibration , Video Recording
19.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 125(6): 4053-9, 2009 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19507986

ABSTRACT

The perception of two simultaneous tones was investigated in goldfish using classical respiratory conditioning and a stimulus generalization paradigm. Pairs of tones were used to make up a mixture of 150 Hz and a higher harmonic or a mistuned harmonic. Fish were conditioned to the two-tone mixture and then tested for generalization to several pure tones. The simultaneous tones tended to be segregated in perception, with the generalization gradient for single tones having two peaks corresponding to the frequencies of the tone pairs. There were no consistent differences in the generalization gradients following conditioning to harmonic or inharmonic tone pairs. In addition, experiments were carried out in which the two tones of the pair were heard on alternate trials, always as single tones, followed by generalization tests to single tones. There was more generalization in this experiment, reflecting the fact that conditioning and generalization test stimuli were both single tones. However, the shapes of the generalization gradients were similar to those in which fish were conditioned to two simultaneous tones, indicating that the simultaneity of the tones did not make them harder to segregate. As the frequency separation between the two components narrowed, segregation tended to fail.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception , Goldfish/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Animals , Conditioning, Classical , Discrimination, Psychological
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL